You are on page 1of 8

European Journal of Clinical Nutrition (2003) 57, 341348 2003 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 09543007/03

3 $25.00 www.nature.com/ejcn

ORIGINAL COMMUNICATION Modifying childrens food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable
J Wardle1*, M-L Herrera1, L Cooke1 and EL Gibson1
1 Cancer Research UK Health Behaviour Unit, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, UK

Objective: The aim of this study was to evaluate two interventions (one reward-based and one exposure-based) for increasing childrens acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable compared with a no-treatment control. It was predicted that the exposure condition would increase liking for, and consumption of, the vegetable relative to either the reward or control group. Design: Using a randomized controlled design, participants were assigned to one of two intervention groups (exposure or reward) or to a no-treatment control condition, for a 2 week period. Liking for, and consumption of, red pepper was assessed before and after the treatment period. Setting: The study was conducted in three primary schools in London. Subjects: Parental consent was obtained for 49 out of a possible 72 children. Interventions: Interventions comprised eight daily sessions during which participants in the exposure group were offered a taste of sweet red pepper and told that they could eat as much as they liked. Participants in the reward group were shown a sheet of cartoon stickers and told that they could choose one of them on condition that they ate at least one piece of the pepper. Results: The exposure-based intervention signicantly increased both liking (P 0.006) and consumption (P 0.03) compared with the control group. The outcome of the reward intervention was intermediate and did not differ signicantly from the exposure or control conditions. Conclusions: Repeated exposure to the taste of unfamiliar foods is a promising strategy for promoting liking of previously rejected foods in children. Sponsorship: This study was nanced by Cancer Research UK. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition (2003) 57, 341 348. doi:10.1038=sj.ejcn.1601541 Keywords: children; food acceptance; exposure; reward; vegetables

Introduction
There is now a consensus that the nutrients obtained from a diet high in fruit and vegetables can contribute to the

*Correspondence: J Wardle, Cancer Research UK Health Behaviour Unit, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK. E-mail: j.wardle@ucl.ac.uk Guarantor: J Wardle. Contributors: JW and M-LH were responsible for study design and coordination. M-LH was responsible for data collection. Data analysis was performed by JW and LC, with assistance in interpretation from ELG. All authors contributed to the writing of the paper. Received 26 November 2001; revised 15 May 2002; accepted 28 May 2002

prevention of cancer and cardiovascular disease and, in addition, may displace other foods from the diet, thereby indirectly reducing fat consumption (Hu et al, 2000; Liu et al, 2000; World Cancer Research Fund, 1995). However, fruit and vegetable consumption falls substantially below recommended levels in many developed Western countries (Gregory et al, 1990; Li et al, 2000; Steinmetz & Potter, 1996; Thompson et al, 1999). Recent campaigns to increase fruit and vegetable consumption have often targeted children. This is partly because intervention earlier in the lifecycle might be expected to yield maximum health benets (Perry et al, 1998). It is also because eating habits established in childhood are likely to track into adulthood (Kelder, et al, 1994; Resnicow et al,

Modifying childrens food preferences Wardle et al

342
1998; Singer et al, 1995). Furthermore, there is widespread concern amongst parents about their childrens diets, and especially consumption of vegetables, and therefore considerable motivation to develop new strategies to improve them (Gibson et al, 1998). Interventions directed at modifying childrens intake of fruit and vegetables have had limited success. For example, the 5-a-day programme in the USA comprised a series of interventions tailored to different sections of the community (Basch et al, 1994; Perry et al, 1998; Sorensen et al, 1999). Results so far appear limited in the light of the enormous investment involved (eg Havas et al, 1998), and there is still an urgent need for new strategies to increase consumption of fruit and vegetables. The focus of 5-a-day and several other campaigns (eg Domel et al, 1993) has been on increasing knowledge and changing attitudes, at which they have been reasonably successful. Achieving signicant changes in behaviour has proved more challenging, however, and it is possible that signicant barriers to behaviour change lie elsewhere. An alternative perspective focuses on hedonic factors, eg affective reactions to taste, texture or appearance. Attempts to explain why vegetable consumption is so low, in adults as well as children, have often highlighted taste preferences as a signicant barrier (Drewnowski et al, 1999; Wardle, 1995). Childrens liking for vegetables has been shown to be the most signicant predictor of their intake (Gibson et al, 1998). If liking or rather disliking is a major deterrent, then interventions which are designed to modify food preferences could provide some leverage in changing childrens acceptance of vegetables. Rozins (1976) notion of food neophobia provides a useful starting point in understanding food acceptance. Humans, in common with most omnivores, manifest an avoidance of new foods which may be adaptive in terms of providing protection from potentially poisonous substances. With experience (ie eating small amounts of the food and suffering no ill effects), the initial neophobic response can be transformed into acceptance via learned safety (Kalat & Rozin, 1973). Rozin and Schiller (1980) also note that some tastes are initially unpalatable, eg bitter or spicy tastes, but even these can become liked with increasing experience. Exposure alone (often called mere exposure in the literature) could therefore provide a means to modify acceptance of initially rejected or disliked foods. Experimental studies with infants, children and adults have provided strong support for the efcacy of exposure, demonstrating that tasting a food or drink more often increases liking for it (Birch & Marlin, 1982; de Silva, 1988; Pliner, 1982; Sullivan & Birch, 1994). Merely looking at the food has no such effect (Birch et al, 1987), but watching others consume the food may provide a form of modelling or exposure by proxy, which could also reduce rejection (Birch, 1980; Hobden & Pliner, 1995). Some further support for the efcacy of modelling comes from a series of small-scale studies evaluating a multicomponent programme combining video-based peer modelling together with rewards for eating fruit and vegetables and
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

daily letters of encouragement (the Food Dudes study; Horne et al, 1995, 1998). The full programme produced positive effects on intake of fruits and vegetables, although the peer modelling video alone failed to achieve signicant changes. Promise of a reward (eat up your peas and you can have some ice cream) is a time-honoured parental tactic for promoting consumption where the child is resistant. However, it has been argued in the psychological literature that treating food consumption in this way, as the instrumental component of a contingency, may actually decrease liking for that food. Lepper and Greenes (1978) Overjustication Theory suggests that offering a reward for an action devalues it for the child. On this analysis, the child interprets his or her own behaviour with concepts such as If I am rewarded for eating peas, I must not like them very much. In support of this idea, a number of studies have reported decreased liking for foods when children are rewarded for eating them (Birch et al, 1982; Birch et al, 1984; Newman & Taylor, 1992). The detrimental effects of reward have come to be widely accepted in the child-feeding literature (Birch, 1999). However a recent review and meta-analysis of studies of reward suggested that the case may have been greatly over-stated (Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996), and that, under some circumstances, rewards can enhance willingness to perform a behaviour. For example, offering verbal rather then tangible rewards, small rather than large rewards, and rewards for quality rather than quantity of behaviour, seem to be more successful. Furthermore, the multi-component programme described above (Horne et al, 1995, 1998) included a signicant reward component comprising a range of Food Dudes prizes (stickers, baseball caps, lunchboxes etc), which were awarded to children who consumed specied quantities of target foods. There were some positive effects of reward alone, but not as great as when modelling and rewards were combined (Lowe et al, 1998). The discrepancies in ndings between studies may arise from differences in outcome measures, the context in which rewards are given, or indeed in the types of rewards offered, and these have never been investigated systematically. It is also important to consider the fact that any reward-based intervention must, insofar as the child consumes any of the food, involve exposure. Therefore, some degree of positive change might be expected as a consequence of this exposure, even if it is reduced relative to exposure alone as a consequence of the overjustication effect. These considerations suggest that it would be valuable to compare the two procedures directly in the same population and under the same conditions. The aim of the present study was to evaluate the efcacy of two interventions, one based on exposure alone, and one incorporating reward, in addressing the real-world problem of increasing childrens liking for vegetables. Using a randomized controlled design, 5 7-y-old children were assigned to one of the two intervention groups (exposure or reward), or to a no treatment control condition, for a 2-week period.

Modifying childrens food preferences Wardle et al

343
We predicted that the daily exposure condition would increase liking for, and consumption of the vegetable relative to either the reward or control group. On the basis of the existing laboratory studies, rewarding consumption was not expected to increase liking, but consumption was expected to rise from baseline enough to meet the reward criterion. with respect to their sex. The reliability of such a measure of preference in young children has been demonstrated by Birch et al (1980) and, more recently, by Guthrie et al (2000). Participants understanding of the scale was established through discussion of hypothetical situations prior to using it to rate the taste of the pepper.

Method
Ethical approval Ethical approval for the study was granted by the Bethlem and Maudsley Hospital Research Ethics Committee.

Participants Participants were recruited from three primary schools in London. From a potential sample of 72 children, 49 (68.1%) parental consent forms were received. The children were reasonably balanced in terms of gender (23 boys and 26 girls) and ranged in age from 5 y 8 months to 7 y 10 months with a mean of 6 y 7 months. All participants attended both pre-and post-treatment assessments.

Design Participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups: exposure (group E, n 15, 7 boys, 8 girls), reward (group R, n 16, 8 boys, 8 girls) and control (group C, n 18, 8 boys, 10 girls). In order to control for possible school effects, each group contained children from all three schools. Groups E and R were scheduled for eight sessions of either mere exposure or reward although of the 31 children assigned to the two intervention groups, nine children failed to attend all treatment sessions. In the reward group, one child missed one session, and four missed ve; in the exposure group three children missed one session, and one missed six. Sweet red pepper (capsicum) was selected as the target vegetable for this study since exploratory taste tests with this age group had suggested that it was both comparatively novel and relatively disliked. Of the sample in the present study, 67% had tried it before. Liking and intake of red pepper were assessed before and after the eight treatment sessions. In order to examine the process of change in the two intervention groups, liking and intake were also assessed in each treatment session.

Procedures The study was carried out in schools over a 2-week period (10 school days). At the same time every day (between 10 am and 12 noon), children were seen individually in a quiet room close to their classroom. All children completed preand post-treatment assessments, which took place on day 1 and day 10, respectively. Children were presented with fresh, sweet red pepper cut into small pieces, and invited to eat as much as they liked, having previously watched the experimenter eating a piece. After they had eaten one piece, children were asked to rate their liking for the taste of the pepper using the measure detailed above. Consumption was measured by counting the number of pieces eaten. Between the pre- and post-treatment assessments, participants randomized to the exposure and reward groups completed eight treatment sessions from day 2 to day 9 of the study period. The daily procedure for the exposure group was the same as for the pre- and post-treatment assessments. They were offered a taste of red pepper and told: Please have a taste of this red pepper. You can eat as much as you like. Liking was rated using the faces scale and number of pieces eaten recorded. Participants in the reward group were shown a sheet of stickers featuring a variety of well-known cartoon characters and told: Please have a taste of this red pepper. If you eat at least one piece, you may choose one of these stickers. You can eat as much as you like. They rated their liking in the same way as the exposure group and were then invited to choose their sticker, having established that the child did not want to eat any more pieces. Participants in the control condition were not seen between the pre- and post-treatment tests and received no further intervention.

Measures Consumption was measured by counting the number of small pieces of raw red pepper (average weight 1.8 g) eaten by each child. Liking was assessed after tasting using a vepoint faces scale designed to represent the following responses I like it a lot, I like it a little bit, It is okay, I dislike it a little bit, I dislike it a lot (scored 2 to 7 2). The scale comprises cartoon faces, varying in expression from a broad smile to deep frown and designed to appear neutral

Statistical analysis Statistical analysis was carried out using SPSS (Version 9.0). Analyses were performed on data from all participants (n 49) since evidence concerning the degree of exposure required to alter preferences is equivocal at present and, furthermore, results were qualitatively replicated when participants who failed to attend every treatment session were excluded. Correlations and analysis of variance were used to establish whether age or gender represented confounding variables. In order to test for differential effects of the three conditions on liking and consumption, repeated measures analysis of variance was used to compare changes in liking and consumption from before to after the intervention period across the three groups. Paired t-tests were used to
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

Modifying childrens food preferences Wardle et al

344
examine the extent to which participants within each group changed their liking and consumption. Further repeated measures analyses were used to examine the prole of changes in liking and consumption over the treatment sessions (day 2 to day 8) in groups E and R, and to compare the groups in terms of the patterning of changes in consumption and liking. were observed across the three groups (F(1, 46) 10.61, P < 0.005), with the change in the reward group being intermediate between the others. Paired t-tests indicated that participants in both intervention groups showed increased liking for red pepper over the study period (group E: t[14] 4.68; P < 0.001; group R: t[15] 2.51; P < 0.05). Participants in Group C did not signicantly change in their liking over the study period.

Results
No signicant effects of age or sex were observed on liking, consumption nor any changes over time, so data from all participants were combined for all the analyses. Effects on consumption Changes in consumption from before to after the intervention are shown in Figure 2. There was a signicant main effect for time (F(1, 46) 25.83, P < 0.001) and a signicant group-by-time interaction (F(2, 46) 3.51, P < 0.05) for the number of pieces of red pepper that were consumed. Paralleling the liking results, post-hoc analyses showed that the difference between groups E and C was signicant (mean difference 5.44, P < 0.05), but comparisons involving group R did not reach signicance. Again, signicant linear effects in the extent of change in consumption were found across the three groups (F(1, 46) 7.01, P < 0.05). Participants in all three groups signicantly increased their consumption of red pepper over the study period (group E, t(14) 3.85, P < 0.01; group R, t(15) 2.30, P < 0.05; group C, t(17) 3.5, P < 0.01).

Effects on liking Pre-treatment, mean liking for red pepper was lower in the exposure group than in either reward or control groups, although this difference was not signicant (F(2, 46) 1.56, P 0.22). Changes in liking from pre- to post-treatment are shown in Figure 1, indicating greater liking after treatment, which was conrmed in the signicant effect of time (pre vs post) in the repeated measures analysis of variance (F(1, 46) 24.75, P < 0.001). As predicted, the change in liking between pretest and post-test differed signicantly between groups (group-by-time interaction: F(2, 46) 5.37, P < 0.01). Posthoc analyses showed that the exposure group changed signicantly more than the control group (mean difference 1.79, P < 0.01). The reward group had an intermediate level of change, which was not signicantly different from either the exposure or the control groups. However, signicant linear effects in the extent of change in liking

Process analysis liking and intake during the intervention sessions Liking for red pepper at pre- and post-treatment sessions and at each treatment session (T1 T8) for children who attended all eight sessions in groups E and R are illustrated in Figure 3.

Figure 1

Change in liking from pre-test to post-test.

European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

Modifying childrens food preferences Wardle et al

345

Figure 2 Change in consumption from pre-test to post-test.

This shows that liking increased from the pre-treatment test (which was, for many, their rst experience of eating red pepper) to the rst treatment session, especially in the reward group. The overall group by time effect over sessions 1 8, however, was not signicant. Nevertheless, out of interest we examined the patterning of results to see if the prole of change over the treatment period looked at all different in the two groups. A repeated measures analysis of variance revealed that the liking ratings of group E showed a signicant linear increase over the eight treatment sessions (F(1, 10) 5.23, P < 0.05) while no such trend was observed in group R.

Intakes at pre- and post-treatment assessments and at each treatment session (T1 T8) for groups E and R are illustrated in Figure 4. The data showed a similar pattern to that for liking. In both groups, intake in the rst treatment session was up on the pre-treatment test. On average they also consistently ate more than the minimum requested in the exposure protocol (taste one piece) or the reward protocol (if you taste a piece of pepper you can have a reward) in every session. Mean intakes over all treatment sessions were 6.73 (s.d. 5.30) for group E and 4.57 (s.d. 3.77) for group R, with a total intake over all sessions of 716 pieces in group E and 473 pieces in group R. Neither of these differences was

Figure 3 Liking for red pepper by treatment session.

European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

Modifying childrens food preferences Wardle et al

346

Figure 4

Intake of red pepper by treatment session.

statistically signicant. However, group E showed a steady increase in consumption over the eight treatment sessions, reected in a signicant linear time trend in a repeated measures analysis of variance (F(1, 10) 9.95, P < 0.05), which was not the case in group R.

Discussion
The results of the present study support the prediction that daily exposure to the taste of a target vegetable can increase childrens liking and consumption compared with no exposure. Eight sessions of exposure shifted childrens liking from being on the margin between liking and disliking ( 7 0.06 on a scale from 7 2 to 2) to maximum liking (2.00). Intake increased from just over one piece of pepper on the rst test session to more than nine pieces on the last session. These results are supportive of the ndings of previous research in this area (Birch & Marlin, 1982; Sullivan & Birch, 1994) and suggest that exposure is a promising technique for improving childrens liking of vegetables, which merits further investigation in a real-world setting. The outcome of the reward intervention was intermediate, lying between, and not differing signicantly from, the exposure or the control conditions. This could be dismissed as a negative result, in the light of the lack of statistical signicance in the difference from the control condition. However, it should be noted that the ordering of the three outcomes was signicantly linear, indicating that the reward effect lay between the effects of control and exposure groups. This means that, contrary to other ndings in this area (Birch et al, 1982, 1984; Newman & Taylor, 1992), the promise of a reward did not decrease liking. Furthermore, the amount of red pepper consumed by children in the reward group during
European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

the treatment sessions was always higher than necessary to achieve the reward, suggesting that their behaviour was not controlled entirely by the promise of reward. In interpreting these results it is important to remember that being rewarded for consumption ipso facto involves exposure the two cannot be separated. This means that the reward condition is in practice a reward plus exposure condition. Given the linear ordering in the change in liking over the three groups (exposure > reward > control), it is reasonable to propose that, rather than having an entirely adverse effect on liking, reward acts to limit the positive effects of exposure. This pattern of ndings is consistent with the idea that offering rewards for consumption might have two effects. First it should promote consumption in the short term, thereby achieving exposure, which enhances liking. At the same time, being rewarded for eating a food might modify childrens view of the acceptability of the food, along the lines suggested by the overjustication hypothesis (Lepper & Greene, 1978). It is worth noting that, where success has been claimed for reward-based interventions (Horne et al, 1995, 1998), the main outcome measure has been consumption. In contrast, studies reporting detrimental effects of reward (eg Birch et al, 1982) have typically used verbal statements of preference. At least in the short term, there may be differential effects of reward on liking and consumption, and the inclusion of both measures in the present study allowed us to look in more detail at these effects. Surprisingly, the control group also showed a statistically signicant increase in consumption over the study period, despite receiving no intervention. No corresponding increase in liking was observed. It may be that a single exposure is sufcient to reduce the typical neophobic response of reluctance to taste, but that further exposures are required to effect changes in liking.

Modifying childrens food preferences Wardle et al

347
We also analysed the ndings in the two treatment groups on a session-by-session basis over the whole treatment period, in an attempt to shed light on the mechanisms involved in the changes in liking. The patterning of results over the eight treatment sessions suggested that the exposure procedure (Please have a taste of this red pepper. You can eat as much as you like) resulted in a slow, steady increase in both liking and consumption, as evidenced by the signicant linear trends over the treatment sessions. In contrast, the reward procedure (Please have a taste of this red pepper. If you eat at least one piece, you may choose one of these stickers. You can eat as much as you like) produced a swift increase from the pre-test to treatment session one, but after that no signicant increase in consumption over the treatment sessions. The consumption effect was paralleled by a less steady increase in liking in the reward group. These ndings suggest that reward is contra-indicated as a technique for producing a steady increase in consumption, although it may be highly effective in the short term. It is worth noting, however, that the reward used (stickers) may not have been equally valued across participants in group R and that this may have diminished their effect. A further problem that has been identied with the offering of rewards concerns their eventual withdrawal. Failing to receive an expected reward may lead to extinction of the previously reinforced behaviour in this case eating the food. The present study used only a single non-rewarded post-treatment test and therefore had limited opportunity to test this proposition. Looking at the present results, liking continued to increase in the exposure group but declined in the reward group; however, this nding was not matched in the intake data. It is possible that further un-rewarded sessions would have seen a greater decline in consumption in the reward group. Alternatively, insofar as the child continues to eat some of the red pepper, the benecial effects of exposure alone could take over. Horne et al (1995, 1998) found that consumption of vegetables was still above baseline levels in many participants at 6-month follow-up. These results therefore suggest that a longer-term follow-up of the effectiveness of rewarded changes in consumption is crucial. Whilst the present study has demonstrated the efcacy of an exposure-based intervention in increasing childrens acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable, the underlying mechanisms of change remain unclear. That mere exposure produced increases in liking and consumption was expected, and is supported by the ndings of previous research in this area (Birch & Marlin, 1982; de Silva, 1988; Pliner, 1982; Sullivan & Birch, 1994). The nding that rewards did not decrease liking raises new questions. However we have argued that, as reward necessarily involves exposure, the way in which these two factors interact requires further investigation. There are a number of limitations to the present study which could be usefully addressed in future research. In common with many published studies in this area, the sample size was comparatively small, and would limit generalization to a wider population. In addition, it was beyond the scope of this study to control the length of time that had elapsed since participants had last eaten, although none would have had the opportunity to eat since breakfast. Furthermore, the setting of the study (in schools), and the implementation of the intervention by an unfamiliar researcher, meant that the social situation in which the children found themselves was far from naturalistic. Since most childrens meals are served to them at home by a parent, there is a strong case for locating future studies in home settings and for teaching parents themselves how to apply the exposure approach. There seems little doubt that parental co-operation would be good given the level of concern they commonly express about their childrens intake of vegetables. Finally, the lack of long-term followup in the present study means that we are unable to determine whether the positive effects of either exposure or reward are lasting and indeed whether any effects generalize to similar foods over time. One of the most important longer-term contributions of this line of research may be to improve the advice given to parents on how to manage food rejections in their children. Our ndings suggest that repeatedly inviting the child to taste a small amount of a rejected or disliked food, without great emphasis on how much they eat, is a good strategy for promoting liking. The element of repetition may be particularly crucial since the process of adaptation may be slower than parents expect, leading some parents to give up before the exposure has had a chance to work. We know that most parents will, at some point, resort to using rewards since it seems to them to be common sense. The results of the present study indicate that the offer of reward limits the effectiveness of exposure. However, there may be an argument for the use of rewards, to encourage a child who atly refuses to taste the food at all, since a food must be tasted for exposure to be effective (Birch et al, 1987). It seems likely that both parents and health professionals would welcome scientically based guidance on how to increase childrens acceptance of vegetables.

References
Basch CE, Zybert P & Shea S (1994): 5-a-day: dietary behavior and the fruit and vegetable intake of Latino children. Am. J. Public Health 84, 814 818. Birch LL (1980): Effects of peer models food choices and eating behaviors on preschoolers food preferences. Child Devl. 51, 489 496. Birch LL (1999): Development of food preferences. A. Rev. Nutr. 19, 41 62. Birch LL & Marlin DW (1982): I dont like it; I never tried it: effects of exposure on two-year-old childrens food preferences. Appetite 3, 353 360. Birch LL, Birch D, Marlin DW & Kramer L (1982): Effects of instrumental consumption on childrens food preference. Appetite 3, 125 134.

European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

Modifying childrens food preferences Wardle et al

348

Birch LL, Zimmerman S & Hind H (1980): The inuence of socialaffective context on the development of childrens food preferences. Child Devl. 52, 856 861. Birch LL, Marlin DW & Rotter J (1984): Eating as the means activity in a contingency: effects on young childrens food preference. Child Devl. 55, 431 439. Birch LL, McPhee L, Shoba BC, Pirok E & Steinberg L (1987): What kind of exposure reduces childrens food neophobia? Looking vs tasting. Appetite 9, 171 178. de Silva P (1988): The modication of human food aversions: a preliminary study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiat. 19, 217 220. Domel SB, Baranowski T, Davis H, Thompson WO, Leonard SB, Riley P, Baranowski J, Dudovitz B & Smyth M (1993): Development and evaluation of a school intervention to increase fruit and vegetable consumption among 4th and 5th grade students. J. Nutr. Educa. 17, 51 54. Drewnowski A, Henderson SA, Levine A & Hann C (1999): Taste and food preferences as predictors of dietary practices in young women. Public Health Nutr. 2, 513 519. Eisenberger R & Cameron J (1996): Detrimental effects of reward: reality or myth? Am. Psychol. 51, 1153 1166. Gibson EL, Wardle J & Watts CJ (1998): Fruit and vegetable consumption, nutritional knowledge and beliefs in mothers and children. Appetite 31, 205 228. Gregory J, Foster K, Tyler H & Wiseman M (1990): The Dietary and Nutritional Survey of Adults. London: HMSO. Guthrie CA, Rapoport L & Wardle J (2000): Young childrens food preferences: a comparison of three modalities of food stimuli. Appetite 35, 73 77. Havas S, Anliker J, Damron D, Langenberg P, Ballesteros M & Feldman R (1998): Final results of the Maryland WIC 5-a-day program. Am. J. Public Health 88, 1161 1167. Hobden K & Pliner P (1995): Effects of a model on food neophobia in humans. Appetite 25, 101 114. Horne PJ, Fergus Lowe C, Fleming PFJ & Dowey AJ (1995): An effective procedure for changing food preferences in 5 7-yearold children. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 54, 441 452. Horne PJ, Fergus Lowe C, Bowdery M & Egerton C (1998): The way to healthy eating for children. Br. Food J. 100, 133 140. Hu FB, Rimm EB, Stampfer MJ, Ascherio A, Spiegelman D & Willett WC (2000): Prospective study of major dietary patterns and risk of coronary heart disease in men. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 72, 912 921. Kalat JW & Rozin P (1973): Learned safety as a mechanism in long delay taste aversion learning in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 83, 198 207. Kelder SH, Peny CL, Klepp KI & Lytle L (1994): Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity and food choice behaviors. Am. J. Public Health 84, 1121 1126. Lepper MR & Greene D (1978): The Hidden Costs of Reward. New Perspectives on the Psychology of Human Motivation. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Li R, Serdula M, Bland S, Mokdad A, Bowman B & Nelson D (2000): Trends in fruit and vegetable consumption among adults in 16 US states: Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System, 1990 1996. Am. J. Public Health 90, 777 781. Liu S, Manson JE, Lee TM, Cole SR, Hennekens CH, Willett WC & Buring JE (2000): Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of cardiovascular disease: the Womens Health Study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 72, 922 928. Lowe CE, Dowey AJ & Horne PJ (1998): Changing what children eat. In The Nations Diet: The Social Science of Food Choice, ed. A Murcott, pp 57 80. London: Longman. Newman J & Taylor A (1992): Effect of a means-end contingency on young childrens food preferences. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 64, 200 216. Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor G, Murray DM, Mays RW, Dudovitz BS, Smyth M & Story M (1998): Changing fruit and vegetable consumption among children: the 5-a-day Power Plus program in St Paul, Minnesota. Am. J. Public Health 88, 603 609. Pliner P (1982): The effects of mere exposure on liking for edible substances. Appetite 3, 283 290. Resnicow K, Smith M, Baranowski T, Baranowski J, Vaughan R & Davis M (1998): Two year tracking of childrens fruit and vegetable intake. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 98, 785 789. Rozin P (1976): The selection of food by rats, humans and other animals. In: Advances in the Study of Behavior, ed. R Rosenblatt, RA Hinde, C Beer & E Shaw, pp 21 76. New York: Academic Press. Rozin P & Schiller D (1980): The nature and acquisition of a preference for chili pepper by humans. Motiv. Emotion 4, 77 101. Singer MR, Moore LL, Garrahie EJ & Ellison RC (1995): The tracking of nutrient intake in young children: the Framingham Childrens Study. Am. J. Public Health 82, 1673 1677. Sorensen G, Stoddard A, Peterson K, Cohen N, Hunt MK, Stein E, Palombo R & Lederman R (1999): Increasing fruit and vegetable consumption through worksites and families in the Treatwell 5-aday study. Am. J. Public Health 89, 54 60. Steinmetz KA & Potter JD (1996): Vegetables, fruit and cancer prevention: a review. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 96, 1027 1039. Sullivan SA & Birch LL (1994): Infant dietary experience and acceptance of solid foods. Pediatrics 93, 271 277. Thompson B, Demark-Wahnefried W, Taylor G, MeClelland JW, Stables G, Havas, S, Feng Z, Topor M, Heimendinger J, Reynolds KD & Cohen N (1999): Baseline fruit and vegetable intake among adults in seven 5-a-day study centers located in diverse geographic areas. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 99, 1241 1248. Wardle J (1995): Parental inuences on childrens diets. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 54, 141 151. World Cancer Research Fund (1995): Food Nutrition and the Prevention of Cancer: a Global Perspective. Washington, DC: American Institute for Cancer Research.

European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

You might also like