You are on page 1of 8

VIEWS & REVIEWS

Delusional misidentifications and duplications


Right brain lesions, left brain delusions

Orrin Devinsky, MD

ABSTRACT

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Orrin Devinsky, Departments of Neurology, Psychiatry, and Neurosurgery, NYU Epilepsy Center, 403 E. 34th Street, 4th floor, New York, NY 10016 od4@nyu.edu

When the delusional misidentification syndromes reduplicative paramnesia and Capgras syndromes result from neurologic disease, lesions are usually bifrontal and/or right hemispheric. The related disorders of confabulation and anosognosis share overlapping mechanisms and anatomic pathology. A dual mechanism is postulated for the delusional misidentification syndromes: negative effects from right hemisphere and frontal lobe dysfunction as well as positive effects from release (i.e., overactivity) of preserved left hemisphere areas. Negative effects of right hemisphere injury impair self-monitoring, ego boundaries, and attaching emotional valence and familiarity to stimuli. The unchecked left hemisphere unleashes a creative narrator from the monitoring of self, memory, and reality by the frontal and right hemisphere areas, leading to excessive and false explanations. Further, the left hemispheres cognitive style of categorization, often into dual categories, leads it to invent a duplicate or impostor to resolve conflicting information. Delusions result from right hemisphere lesions. But it is the left hemisphere that is deluded.
Neurology 2009;72:8087

Delusions are pathologic beliefs that remain fixed despite clear evidence that they are incorrect. They are not conventional beliefs of a culture or religion. Often bizarre in content and held with absolute certainty, delusions can cause significant morbidity. Their pathogenesis in primary psychotic disorders and neurologic disorders remains unknown, with theories encompassing psychological, cognitive, neurochemical, and structural anomalies. This article reviews the cognitive and anatomic systems underlying two delusional misidentification syndromes, reduplicative paramnesia and Capgras syndromes, and related disorders of confabulation and anosognosia. The division between delusions in psychiatric (primary) and neurologic-systemic disorders (secondary) is artificial since psychiatric disorders with delusions (e.g., schizophrenia) have functional and structural brain pathology. Like psychiatric delusions, neurologic misidentification disorders may be associated with paranoia and are usually personally significant. Schizophrenic delusions usually involve 1) influence (e.g., thought control, broadcasting or insertion), 2) persecution or paranoia, and 3) self-significance (e.g., grandeur, reference, religious, guilt). Schizophrenic delusions are not consistently associated with lateralized or localized brain metabolic abnormalities.1 Delusional disorder is a psychotic syndrome with prominent non-bizarre delusions (e.g., my wife is having an affair) lasting for at least 1 month, including erotomania, grandiose, jealous, persecutory, somatic, and mixed. Simple delusions are unstructured, variable, and often paranoid. Complex delusions usually focus on specific content, with an elaborate web of explanations in patients with relatively preserved intellect, as intellect is recruited in the service of the delusion.2 Improved diagnostic techniques have increased identification of neurologic disorders among patients with delusions. For example, Capgras syndrome was a psychiatric disorder 50 years ago, but neurologic disorders are now recognized in most cases.3 A wide spectrum of systemic and neurologic disorders cause simple and complex delusions.4 Content-specific delusions
From the Departments of Neurology, Psychiatry, and Neurosurgery, NYU Epilepsy Center, New York, NY. Disclosure: The author reports no disclosures. 80 Copyright 2009 by AAN Enterprises, Inc.

Table 1
Syndrome

Content-specific delusions
Clinical features Comments

Delusional reduplicationmisidentification Loss of familiarity People Capgras3,5,6,13,14,20-22 Familiar persons are impostors or have doubles with different psychic identity No autonomic response to familiar faces despite conscious recognition (converse of prosopagnosia) Able to identify others in the mirror

Mirror sign (selfmisidentification)23,24 Places Foreign reduplicative paramnesia (Capgras for places)25 Disorientation for place26 Hyperfamiliarity People Fregoli6,14,22,30

Misidentification of oneself in the mirror

Familiar place such as home is considered a duplicate in another location A familiar place exists in another location

Sometimes labeled reduplicative paramnesia New York Hospital is in Manhattan but Manhattan is in Boston

A stranger is believed to be a familiar person Familiar or unfamiliar people change both physical and mental identity Familiar or unfamiliar person is mentally and physically transformed into the patient

A person takes on others appearances but retains psychic identity; often persecutes patient Usually transforms into someone familiar to the patient The patient considers this other person a double

Intermetamorphosis22 Subjective doubles22

Places Reduplicative paramnesia27-29 Other delusions Othello (delusional jealousy)31 Belief that a loved one is involved in a sexual or love relation Belief in a reciprocal love relation, often with a famous stranger Belief that one is dead or dying Patient may repeatedly interrogate, search for evidence, or stalk partner A place simultaneously exists in two or more physical locations For example, a strange hospital is duplicated in a hometown setting

de Clerambault (erotomania)32 Cotard syndrome

predominate in neurologic patients3,5,6 (table 1), usually misidentification and reduplication syndromes, with beliefs that places (reduplicative paramnesia), people (Capgras, Fregoli), or events are transformed in identity or duplicated. These delusional syndromes can coexist.
RELATED NEUROLOGIC DISORDERS: CONFABULATION AND ANOSOGNOSIA Confabulation and

anosognosia are close kin to delusions. Confabulation refers to incorrect or distorted statements made without a conscious effort to deceive. Confabulation is usually associated with memory (medial temporal or diencephalic) and executive (bifrontal) dysfunction.7-9 If retrieved information cannot be contextually and temporally labeled, past memories blur with current experience. Conscious guidance and maintenance of the search to link the results with past and current experi-

ence, or strategic recall, is often impaired.8 In contrast, the automatic and faster associative recall is intact. Deficits in self-monitoring and linking a memory with its source impairs reality monitoring and the ability to distinguish memory from internally generated thoughts.9 Although confabulation and content-specific delusions can coexist, they differ in features, time course, and neuroanatomy. Patients who confabulate can usually be redirected and corrected while those with delusions cannot. When confabulation and delusional misidentifications concur, confabulation usually resolves first.10,11 The occurrence and resolution of ventromedial and orbital frontal lesions parallels the development and disappearance of confabulation.7,12 Although confabulation and delusions are often caused by bilateral lesions, when pathology is unilateral, there is no lateralized predominance with confabulation,9 but right-sided lesions predominate with delusions.6,13,14 Anosognosia is unawareness of a neurologic defiNeurology 72 January 6, 2009 81

cit, usually affecting vision (Anton syndrome) or movement (anosognosia for hemiplegia). The inability of self to recognize blindness or hemiplegia is striking. Anton syndrome is usually caused by bilateral occipital lesions, but can result from anterior visual system lesions, especially when combined with frontal pathology.15 Patients often confabulate about their visual loss; for example, its just bad lighting. Babinski coined anosognosia to describe unawareness of left hemiplegia.16 Patients often insist their left arm and leg can move fine despite obvious weakness. Although anosognosia for hemiplegia after stroke often recovers over months, the initial false belief in the limbs strength is a delusion that resists rational explanations and visual demonstrations. Some patients deny ownership of their limb (asomatognosia), often claiming it belongs to someone else. Asomatognosia is a delusion in which the limbs relation to the self is lost, Capgras syndrome affecting the arm.18 Other patients personify the limb with names such as Floppy Joe or Silly Jimmy, hate the limb (misoplegia), or recognize the deficit but show no concern (anosodiaphoria).17-19 Anosognosia for hemiplegia usually results from large nondominant hemisphere strokes involving parietal, frontal, and temporal lobes as well as the insula and subcortex. Pick reported a 67-year-old woman who developed amnesia, confabulations, paranoid delusions, and seizures after a stroke.27 She believed that the Prague hospital and its doctors, attendants, and patients were simultaneously duplicated in her birth town. When the doctor asked how he could be there as well, she said was very pleased to see him here too. When asked about the peculiarities of the two clinics, she responded, todays clinic is perhaps a continuation of the previous one. Similarly, after a head injury, a man was repeatedly told and remembered that he was at the Jamaica Plain VA Hospital, but insisted the hospital was in his hometown of Taunton, Massachusetts.29 Some patients misidentify but do not reduplicate their environment. Reduplicative paramnesia most often occurs in patients with neurodegenerative disorders, stroke, and head trauma. Among psychiatric patients, it is associated with bilateral anterior cortical atrophy.28 Patients with reduplicative amnesia often confabulate, confusing memory and imagination. Like confabulation, reduplicative paramnesia is usually associated with executive and memory deficits, but visuospatial and geographic orientation skills are also often impaired.7,25 Patients with reduplicative paramnesia recognize familiar places, landmarks, and
82 Neurology 72 January 6, 2009

objects, suggesting that the ventral occipitotemporal cortex that recognizes these stimuli is preserved. The lesions usually involve bifrontal or right frontal areas.6,7,25 Frontal injury may disinhibit the intact familiarity and landmark areas in the temporal lobes while impairing self-monitoring, meta-memory, and error detection. Familiarity may be falsely triggered by novel stimuli but eludes error detection. Wish fulfillment and motivation may affectively color the choice of duplicated location. Virtually all patients believe that they are in or near their homes or hometowns.33 After resolution of the delusion that the hospital was in their home, four patients each claimed that their confabulated stories resulted from a desire to be at home.34 Rarely, patients believe that a familiar setting is reduplicated in a foreign place; i.e., foreign reduplicative paramnesia or Capgras for places. For example, a man with a right frontal hemorrhage returned home by taxi, recognized his wife and daughter, but was sure that his house was not his real home.25 He remarked how striking it was that the owners of this house had the same ornaments as he had in his house and what a coincidence it was that there were similar items beside the bed . . .
CAPGRAS SYNDROME Capgras and ReboulLachaux reported a 53-year-old paranoid megalomanic woman who transformed everyone in her entourage, even those closest to her, such as her husband and daughter, into various and numerous doubles.20 Delusional jealousy and grandeur began 8 years earlier, after four children died over a short period, leaving only a daughter. Later, she was convinced that her children were abducted and her surviving daughter was replaced by a look-alike. Thousands of doubles of her daughter passed before her eyes, every day, sometimes many a day. Her husband and even she were eventually replaced by doubles. Capgras attributed her delusions to an agnosia of identification, postulating that all sensory and memory images struggle between the emotional poles of familiarity and strangeness; he later proposed an Oedipal etiology.21 The Capgras delusion usually involves intimatesa spouse, sibling, child, or neighbor. The paradox of Capgras is the selective duplication of people with whom the patient has strong emotional bonds and associative memories. Capgras patients can consciously recognize familiar faces but cannot emotionally connect with them: the converse of prosopagnosia.35 Prosopagnosia results from bilateral or right-sided lesions in the facial recognition areas (fusiform gyri). Patients may fail to recognize their spouses or their own face but generate an uncon-

REDUPLICATIVE PARAMNESIA

scious autonomic skin resistance response to familiar faces. Nonconscious and conscious awareness are dissociated. Psychiatric and neurologic Capgras patients fail to generate autonomic responses when viewing photographs of familiar people or famous people.35,36 In some patients, visual information reaches the facial recognition area leading to conscious recognition, but a temporal lesion may disconnect this from the limbic areas that infuse the sparkle of familiarity. This explanation cannot explain most Capgras cases since the temporal lobes may be spared and most cases involve visual and auditory modalities.6 The delusional misidentification-reduplication syndromes can be viewed as pathologic poles of decreased and increased familiarity. Capgras syndrome, the mirror sign, and foreign reduplicative paramnesia cause familiar people or places to become foreign. By contrast, in, Fregoli, intermetamorphosis, and reduplicative paramnesia syndromes, novel people and places feel very familiar. Paranoid ideation is common with Capgras syndrome, often preceding the delusion of the imposter.6 In contrast, paranoia is uncommon among patients with reduplicative paramnesia.27-29
LESION LOCALIZATION IN DELUSIONAL MISIDENTIFICATION SYNDROMES Neurologic patients

with delusions usually have lesions in the right hemisphere and/or bifrontal areas.3,5-7,13,14,22-25,37-39 In some cases, a two-hit sequence may cause delusions: baseline generalized atrophy with a subsequent right hemisphere injury.37,38 In one study, nine patients with right hemisphere infarctions at a stroke rehabilitation unit had frequent delusions. While size of the stroke did not correlate with delusions when compared to a control group, the presence of brain atrophy was a significant predictor of delusions.38 When delusions occur in patients with widespread brain involvement, if pathology predominates in one hemisphere, it is usually the right. For example, delusional patients with Alzheimer disease have significantly more right frontal hypometabolism.40,41 Reduplicative paramnesia and Capgras syndrome cases with unilateral brain lesions strongly implicate the right hemisphere, usually frontal with variable temporal or parietal involvement. 6,13,14,37,39 Among 69 patients with reduplicative paramnesia, lesions were primarily in the right hemisphere in 36 (52%), bilateral in 28 (41%), and left hemisphere in 5 (7%)39a sevenfold increase of right- over left-sided lesions (p 0.0001). Similarly, among 26 Capgras patients, lesions were primarily in the right hemisphere in 8 (32%), bilateral in 16 (62%), and leftsided in 2 (7%)a fourfold increase of right-over left-sided lesions (p 0.06). For both delusional

syndromes, many bilateral cases had maximal damage in the right hemisphere. Among 29 cases of delusional misidentification syndromes (overlap with prior review), all had right hemisphere pathology, while only 15 (52%) had left hemisphere damage. 6 Fourteen had exclusively right hemisphere damage but none had isolated lefthemisphere damage. When lateralized lesions are found, right hemisphere lesions are more common in other delusional misidentification (Fregoli syndrome, mirror sign) and content specific delusions (Othello, erotomania).24,30-32 Frontal lesions are strongly implicated in misidentification syndromes. Exclusively frontal lesions were associated with delusions in 10/29 (34.5%) cases6: four with right frontal and six with bifrontal lesions. None had lesions sparing the frontal lobes.6 Notably, temporal lesions were present in 7/11 (64%) patients who mistook familiar people, places, or body parts as foreign and 2/14 (14%) who mistook foreign objects or people as familiar. While frontal pathology is often critical for delusions to develop, associated temporal lesions may cause familiar places to appear foreign while temporal sparing may cause foreign places to appear familiar. Observations in patients with bilateral neurodegenerative disorders supports that misidentification of familiar places as foreign is associated with temporal and frontal pathology. A patient with Alzheimer disease with foreign reduplicative paramnesia believed that his home was duplicated in another city; pathology was maximal in frontal and temporal association cortices.42 One study found neurodegenerative disorder in 38/47 (81%) Capgras patients, with Lewy body disease in 26 (55%).3 Many patients believed that there were multiple impostors, and held other delusions such as reduplicative paramnesia. Visual hallucinations, present in 30/38 (79%) of neurodegenerative patients, correlate with the severity of Lewy body deposition in the amygdala, and parahippocampal and inferior temporal cortices.43 Dysfunction in these areas can cause paranoia and visual hallucinations. Patients with Lewy body disease may identify familiar faces, but cannot link the face and emotion. Frontal lobe dysfunction, a neuropsychological hallmark of Lewy body disease,44 may prevent patients from recognizing their errors and changing their false beliefs. The frontotemporal lesions may lead patients to see familiar people or places as foreign; temporal lesions may cause paranoia.
PATHOGENESIS A spectrum of cognitive and behavioral disorders may cause or contribute to delusions (table 2).2-6,13-15,45-47 Nearly ubiquitous frontal pathology supports the role of impaired executive,
Neurology 72 January 6, 2009 83

Table 2

Pathogenesis of delusions: Possible cognitive and behavioral disorders

Executive function Reasoning Judgment Self-monitoring Reality monitoring Meta-memory (knowledge of memory) Theory of mind Attention (increased signal to noise, false alerts, and threats) Probability-based decision-making Prediction-error signaling Time estimation, context, and sequencing Familiarity versus foreignness Working memory Short-term declarative memory Autobiographical memory Visuospatial Spatial attention Self-representation Ego boundaries Attachment of emotional valence to stimuli Psychiatric-psychological comorbidity Anxiety Paranoia Wish fulfillment Denial Projection Disconnection Frontal executive and temporal emotional areas Facial recognition and emotional areas

theory of mind, decision and prediction making, time estimation and sequencing, meta-memory, and working memory functions. Frontal dysfunction impairs the ability to monitor self and to recognize and correct inaccurate memories and familiarity assessments. The resistance of delusions to change despite clear evidence that they are wrong likely reflects frontal dysfunction. The right frontal lobe likely monitors the appropriateness of familiarity decisions, consistent with right frontal lesions in almost all delusional misidentification syndromes, while the right temporal lobe creates the glow of emotional familiarity.48 The anterior parahippocampal cortex (perirhinal cortex, Brodmann areas 35 and 36) is activated by familiarity, while the hippocampus and posterior parahippocampal cortex mediate recollection.49 Perirhinal cortex stimulation evokes de ja ` vu and de ja ` ve cu (already experienced).50 Further, the right hemi84 Neurology 72 January 6, 2009

sphere dominates in familiarity decisions14,48; de ja ` vu is more common with right than left temporal lobe seizures or stimulation.48,51 Lesions that destroy or isolate stimuli from right perirhinal cortex may lead to loss of familiarity (e.g., Capgras syndrome) while hyperfamiliarity (i.e., misidentifying strange people as familiar [Fregoli syndrome]) may result from overactivity in right perirhinal cortex from stimulation or disinhibition. Two cases of nondelusional hyperfamiliarity for faces resulted from left-sided lesions (lateral temporal-occipital and anterior cingulate),52,53 possibly disinhibiting right hemisphere areas that imbue faces or places with familiarity. Delusions may develop preferentially after right hemisphere lesions because of 1) loss of self-related functions such as monitoring of reality, memory, and familiarity,6,14,48 and 2) left hemisphere overactivity after loss of right-sided influences. The right hemisphere dominates awareness and image of self and relating perceptual and emotional self to the external and internal environments.6,14,48 Thus, right hemisphere lesions may cause delusions by disrupting the relation between and the monitoring of psychic, emotional, and physical self to people, places, and even body parts.6,14 This explains why contentspecific delusions involve people, places, or things of personal significance and distort their relation to the self. The right hemisphere dominates selfrecognition,54 emotional familiarity,14,50 and ego boundaries.6,14,55 An example is the response-tonext-patient-stimulation, found in patients with acute right hemisphere strokes.55 When commands such as open your mouth were directed to the patient in the bed next to them, the stroke patients obeyed the commands. This inability to distinguish verbal information directed at others vs self is a disorder of the ego boundary. Left hemisphere hyperactivity may play a critical role in the pathogenesis of delusions. Jackson recognized many dramatic positive symptoms result from intact brain areas released from control by damaged areas.56 Lesions outside the right temporal lobe may cause nondelusional hyperfamiliarity syndromes by disinhibiting emotional familiarity. In corpus callosotomy patients, when an emotional visual stimulus is selectively presented to the right hemisphere, the left hemisphere may confabulate. One such patient blushed and giggled after her right hemisphere saw a nude picture. When asked what she saw, her left hemisphere responded, Nothing, just a flash of light. When asked why she was laughingOh doctor, you have some machine!57 Another callosotomy patient saw a snow scene in the right hemisphere (left visual field) and chicken claw in the left hemisphere (right visual field). Asked to choose a pic-

ture card that related to what was seen, the left hand chose a shovel and the right hand picked a chicken. When asked what he saw: I saw a claw and I picked the chicken, and you have to clean out the chicken shed with a shovel.58 Similarly, when a picture of a hurdler was flashed to the right hemisphere, the word athlete was offered, presumably by the right hemisphere. The left hemisphere then went on, a basketball guy . . . had a uniform.59 These confabulatory responses reflect the left hemispheres creative and incorrect filling in.59 Gazzaniga referred to the left hemispheres narrative talent that extends to fiction as the interpreter mechanism: . . . always hard at work, seeking the meaning of events. It is constantly looking for order and reason, even when there is nonewhich leads it continually to make mistakes. It tends to over-generalize, frequently constructing a potential past as opposed to a true one.59 Release of the left hemispheres creative narrator from the monitoring of self, memory, and familiarity by the right frontal lobe may lead to excessive or false explanations. In callosotomy patients, testing is often done months after surgery and the frontal lobes are usually intact, accounting for the lack of delusions. For visual perception and mental imagery, the right hemisphere is stronger at encoding overall patterns, representing specific instances, and mapping coordinate spatial relations. The left hemisphere encodes component parts, represents categories, and encodes categorical relations.60 Spatially, the left hemisphere dominates at dual categorical choices such as up vs down and left vs right. In callosotomy patients, the right hemisphere can point to a states location and then draw its shape, but the left hemisphere can tell whether state is north/south or east/ west of another. Thus, a left hemisphere released from the right may be overbiased to categorize, but without the right hemispheres gestalt and emotional familiarity. This combination may explain the bizarre invention of an impostor or duplicate. The left hemisphere recognizes she is my wife, but fails to receive other information (sparkle of familiarity, global gestalt, and relation to self) and concludes that she is not my wife. The conflict is resolved with a fabrication, a dual category: a duplicate or impostor. She looks like my wife, but she really isnt my wife. Right frontal injury impairs monitoring of feedback from others and self, leading to false memories and delusions that resist erasure, despite evidence that they are incorrect. Similarly, in anosognosic patients, the left hemisphere of an adult with superior verbal intelligence cannot understand evidence (i.e., the right side is weak) that a young child immediately grasps linguistically or visually. The left hemisphere just does not get it. Delusions result from right hemi-

sphere lesions. But it is the left hemisphere that is deluded. Our knowledge of delusions is limited by our ability to comprehend the patients irrational thought process. The hypothesis and mechanisms put forth in this article remain speculative. The presence of similar lesions without delusions argues against but is not a fatal flaw for the hypothesis. The pathogenesis of delusions likely includes many mechanisms that span overlapping psychological, cognitive, and neurologic disorders. Individual differences in the all three areas may interact to produce complex clinical patterns. Yet common elements may contribute to creation and persistence of delusions in diverse psychiatric and neurologic disorders. Future research should explore the psychological, cognitive, and physiologic-anatomic systems that change during the emergence and resolution of delusions, as well as strategies to treat delusions (e.g., reducing left hemisphere overactivity).
Received July 29, 2008. Accepted in final form September 22, 2008.

REFERENCES 1. Lahti AC, Weiler MA, Holcomb HH, Tamminga CA, Carpenter WT, McMahon R. Correlations between rCBF and symptoms in two independent cohorts of drug-free patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31:221230. 2. Cummings JL. Organic delusions. Br J Psychiatry 1985; 46:184197. 3. Joseph KA. Capgras syndrome and its relationship to neurodegenerative disease. Arch Neurol 2007;64:17621766. 4. Gorman DG, Cummings JL. Organic delusional syndrome. Semin Neurol 1990;10:229238. 5. Malloy PF, Richardson ED. The frontal lobes and contentspecific delusions. J Neuropsychiatry 1994;6:455466. 6. Feinberg TE, Deluca J, Giacino JT, Roane DM, Solms M. Right-hemisphere pathology and the self: delusional misidentification and reduplication. In: Feinberg TE, Keenan JP, eds. The Lost Self. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005:100 130. 7. Benson DF, Djenderedjian A, Miller BL, et al. Neural basis of confabulation. Neurology 1996;46:12391243. 8. Moscovitch M, Melo B. Strategic recall and the frontal lobes: evidence from confabulation and amnesia. Neuropsychologia 1997;35:10171034. 9. Johnson MK, Hayes SM, DEsposito M, Raye CL. Confabulation. In: Grafman J, Boller F. Handbook of Neuropsychology, 2nd Ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science; 2000: 383 407. 10. Box O, Laing H, Kopelman M. The evolution of spontaneous confabulation, delusional misidentification and a related delusion in a case of severe head injury. Neurocase 1999;5:251262. 11. Mattioli F, Miozzo A, Vignolo LA. Confabulation and delusional misidentification: A four year followup study. Cortex 1999;35:413422. 12. Deluca J, Diamond BJ. Aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery: a review of neuroanatomical and neuropsyNeurology 72 January 6, 2009 85

13.

14.

15.

16.

17. 18. 19. 20.

21. 22.

23.

24.

25.

26. 27. 28.

29. 30.

31.

32.

33.

chologic sequelae. Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995;17:100 121. Malloy P, Cimino C, Westlake R. Differential diagnosis of primary and secondary Capgras delusions. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1992;2:8396. Devinsky O. Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense of corporeal and emotional self. Epilepsy Behav 2000;1:6073. Feinberg TE, Roane DM. Anosognosia, completion and confabulation: the neutral-personal dichotomy. Neurocase 1997;3:7385. Babinski J. Contribution a letude des mentaux dans lhemiplegie organique (anosognosie). Rev Neurol 1914; 27:845848. Critchley M. Observations on anosodiaphoria. Encephale 1953;46:540546. Critchley M. Personification of paralyzed limbs in hemiplegics. BMJ 1955;30:284. Critchley M. Misoplegia or hatred of hemiplegia. Mt Sinai J Med 1974;41:8287. Capgras J, Reboul-Lachaux J. Illusion des sosies dans un delire systematise chronique. Bulletin de la societe clinique de medicine mentale 1923;2:616. Capgras J, Carette P. Illusion des sories et complexe dOedipe. Ann Medico-Psychologiques 1924;12:48. Fo rstl H, Almeida OP, Owen AM, Burns A, Howard R. Psychiatric, neurological and medical aspects of misidentification syndromes: a review of 260 cases. Psychol Med 1991;21:905910. Breen N, Caine D, Coltheart M. Mirrored-self misidentification: two cases of focal onset dementia. Neurocase 2001;7:239254. Postal KS. The mirror sign delusional misidentification symptom. In: Feinberg TE, Keenan JP. The Lost Self: Pathologies of the Brain and Identity. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005:131146. Kapur N, Coughlan AK. Confabulation and frontal lobe dysfunction. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1980;43: 461463. Fisher CM. Disorientation for place. Arch Neurol 1982; 39:3336. Pick A. Clinical studies. Brain 1903;26:242267. Joseph AB, OLeary DH, Kurland R, Ellis HD. Bilateral anterior cortical atrophy and subcortical atrophy in reduplicative paramnesia: a case-control study of computed tomography in 10 patients. Can J Psychiatry 1999;44:685689. Benson DF, Gardner H, Meadows JC. Reduplicative paramnesia. Neurology 1976;26:147151. de Pauw KW, Szulecka TK, Poltock TL. Fre goli syndrome after cerebral infarction. J Nerv Ment Dis 1987;175:433 438. Narumoto J, Nakamura K, Kitabayashi Y, Fukui K. Othello syndrome secondary to right orbitofrontal lobe excision. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006;18:560 561. El Gaddal YY. De Clerambaults syndrome (erotomania) in organic delusional syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 1980;154: 714716. Turnbull OH, Berry H, Evans CE. A positive emotional bias in confabulatory false beliefs about place. Brain Cogn 2004;55:490494.
January 6, 2009

34.

35.

36.

37.

38. 39.

40.

41.

42.

43.

44.

45.

46. 47.

48.

49.

50.

51.

52.

Ruff RL, Volpe BT. Environmental reduplication associated with right frontal and parietal lobe injury. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1981;44:382386. Ellis HD, Young AW, Quayle AH, de Pauw KW. Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras delusion. Proc Biol Sci 1997;264:10851092. Hirstein W, Ramachandran VS. Capgras syndrome: a novel probe for understanding the neural representation of the identity and familiarity of persons. Proc Biol Sci 1997; 264:437444. Alexander MP, Stuss DT, Benson DF. Capgras syndrome: a reduplicative phenomenon. Neurology 1979;29:334 339. Levine DN, Grek A. The anatomic basis of delusions after right cerebral infraction. Neurology 1984;34:577582. Feinberg TE, Shapiro RM. Misidentification-reduplication and the right hemisphere. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1989;2:3948. Sultzer DL, Brown CV, Mandelkern MA, et al. Delusional thoughts and regional frontal/temporal cortex metabolism in Alzheimers disease. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:341 349. Nakano S, Yamashita F, Matsuda H, Kodama C, Yamada T. Relationship between delusions and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimers disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:1621. von Gunten A, Miklossy J, Suva ` ML, Hof PR, Glannakopoulos P. Environmental reduplicative paramnesia in a case of atypical Alzheimers disease. Neurocase 2005;11:216226. Harding AJ, Broe GA, Halliday GM. Visual hallucinations in Lewy body disease relate to Lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain 2002;125:391403. McKeith IG, Perry RH, Fairbairn AF, Jabeen S, Perry EK. Operational criteria for senile dementia of Lewy body type (SDLT). Psychol Med 1992;22:911922. Corlett PR, Murray GK, Honey GD, et al. Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain 2007;130:23872400. Kunert HJ, Norra C, Hoff P. Theories of delusional disorders. Psychopathology 2007;40:191202. Maher BA. Anomalous experience and delusional thinking: The logic of explanations. In: Oltmanns TF, Maher BA, eds. Delusional Beliefs. New York: Wiley; 1988: 1533. Gainotti G. Face familiarity feelings, the right temporal lobe and the possible underlying neural mechanisms. Brain Res Rev 2007;56:214235. Bowles B, Crupi C, Mirsattari SM, et al. Impaired familiarity with preserved recollection after anterior temporallobe resection that spares the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2007;104:1638216387. Vignal J-P, Maillard L, McGonigal A, Chauvel P. The dreamy state: hallucinations of autobiographic memory evoked by temporal lobe stimulations and seizures. Brain 2007;130:8889. Mullan S, Penfield W. Illusions of comparative interpretation and emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1959;81:269 284. Vuilleumier P, Mohr C, Valenza N, Wetzel C, Landis T. Hyperfamiliarity for unknown faces after left lateral temporo-occipital venous infarction: a double dissociation with prosopagnosia. Brain 2003;126:889907.

86

Neurology 72

53.

Nente F, Carrillo-Mezo R, Mendez MF, RamirezBermudez J. Pathological hyperfamiliarity for others from a left anterior cingulate lesion. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007;19:345346. 54. Keenan JP, Wheeler M, Platek SM, Lardi G, Lassonde M. Self-face processing in a callosotomy patient. Eur J Neurosci 2003;18:23912395. 55. Bogousslavsky J, Regli F. Response-to-next-patient-stimulation: a right hemisphere syndrome. Neurology 1988;38:12251227. 56. Jackson JH. Evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. In: Taylor J, ed. Selected Writings of John Hughlings

57.

58. 59. 60.

Jackson, Volume 2. New York: Basic Books; 1884/1958: 4575. Sperry RW, Zaidel E, Zaidel D. Self recognition and social awareness in the deconnected minor hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 1979;17:153166. Gazzaniga MS. Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication. Brain 2000;123:12961326. Gazzaniga MS. The split brain revisited. Sci Am 1998;279: 5055. Brown HD, Kosslyn SM. Cerebral lateralization. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1993;3:183186.

Use Dendrite to Find Your Next Job


Looking to take the next important step in your career? Or are you hoping to reach more than 21,000 neuroscience professionals for your open positions in neurology? Look no further than AAN Dendrite: Careers in Neurology! Search hundreds of employment opportunities in neurology across the US and Canada. Employers: maximize your range and impact for job postings with a Dendrite ad and find the perfect fit for your posting. Print and online listings for one price. Make your next step in neurology count; go now: www.aan.com/neurologyjobs.

New Categories of Resident & Fellow Section


Clinical Reasoning: Case presentations to aid in developing clinical reasoning skills. Right Brain: Neurology and the medical humanities history, literature, and arts. Child Neurology: Patient case with detailed discussion about topic of focus. Pearls and Oy-sters: Clinical insights (pearls) and advice for avoiding mistakes (oysters). International: Educational exchanges, experiences in low and middle income countries. Emerging Subspecialities: History of fields such as Pain Medicine and Headache. Continue to submit articles about education research and educational topics, training videos, and teaching NeuroImages!

Neurology 72

January 6, 2009

87

You might also like