You are on page 1of 49

1/49

UNIT 12

THE CONCEPT OF GRAMMAR: A DISCUSSION ON
LANGUAGE AND LANGUAGE LEARNING. FROM
NORMATIVE GRAMMAR TO A GRAMMAR SYSTEM
BASED ON LANGUAGE USE AND COMMUNICATION.

OUTLINE

1. INTRODUCTION.
1.1. Aims of the unit.
1.2. Notes on bibliography.

2. THE CONCEPT OF GRAMMAR.
2.1. On the origin and nature of grammar.
2.2. On defining the term grammar.
2.3. A historical approach to grammar.

3. A GENERAL OVERVIEW OF GRAMMAR IN LANGUAGE AND LANGUAGE
LEARNING: FROM NORMATIVE GRAMMAR TO COMMUNICATIVE GRAMMAR.

3.1. On terminology: approach and method.
3.2. A brief definition of grammar models.
3.3. Normative Grammar.
3.3.1. Definition.
3.3.2. Historical background.
3.3.2.1. Early educational systems.
3.3.2.2. Ancient Greece.
3.3.2.3. Ancient Rome.
3.3.2.4. The Middle Ages (A.D. 400-1400s).
3.3.2.5. The Renaissance (late 1400s-1600).
3.3.2.6. The seventeenth century and beyond.
3.3.3. Main approaches.
3.3.4. Main methods.
3.3.4.1. The Grammar-Translation Method.

3.4. Descriptive Grammar.
3.4.1. Definition.
3.4.2. Historical background.
3.4.2.1. The early descriptivists.
3.4.2.2. Historical and comparative linguistic studies.
3.4.2.3. The early reformers.
3.4.3. Main approaches.
3.4.3.1. In Europe: The Reform Movement.
3.4.3.2. The behaviorist approach.
3.4.4. Main methods.
3.4.4.1. The Direct Method.


2/49
3.5. Structural grammar.
3.5.1. Definition.
3.5.2. Historical background.
3.5.3. Main approaches.
3.5.3.1. In the United States: American Structural Linguistics.
3.5.3.2. In Europe: the Oral Approach and Situational Language Teaching.
3.5.4. Main methods.
3.5.4.1. The Situational Method.
3.5.4.2. The Audiolingual Method.

3.6. Transformational-Generative Grammar.
3.6.1. Definition.
3.6.1.1. The notion of transformational.
3.6.1.2. The notion of generative: indefinite, explicit, discovery vs. evaluation,
competence vs. performance.
3.6.2. Historical background.
3.6.2.1. Syntactic Structures (1957).
3.6.2.2. Revisions of Syntactic Structures Model.
3.6.2.3. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (1965).
3.6.3. Main approaches.
3.6.3.1. A rejection to behaviourist approaches.
3.6.3.2. Psycholinguistics and cognitive approaches.
3.6.3.3. Natural approaches to second language acquisition research.
3.6.4. Main methods.
3.6.4.1. The Natural Method: Krashens Monitor Model.
3.6.4.2. The Total Physical Response.
3.6.4.3. The Silent Way.
3.6.4.4. Counseling Learning.
3.6.4.5. Suggestopedia.
3.7. Communicative grammar.
3.7.1. Definition.
3.7.2. Historical background.
3.7.3. Main approaches.
3.7.3.1. The Communicative Language Teaching (CLT) approach.
3.7.3.2. Other approaches: direct developments of CLT.
3.7.4. Main methods.

4. PRESENT-DAY DIRECTIONS REGARDING COMMUNICATIVE GRAMMAR.
4.1. Educational implications of communicative grammar.
4.2. Future directions on the teaching and learning of grammar.

5. CONCLUSION.

6. BIBLIOGRAPHY.



3/49
1. INTRODUCTION.

1.1. Aims of the unit.

Unit 12 is intended as an introductory survey on the concept of grammar and its relationship with
the use of language, as well as with language learning. Moreover, since the development of English
grammar systems has a long and continuing history, the aim of this study is to trace that history
within the linguistic scene from its earliest beginnings to the current situation, or in other words,
from normative grammar to a grammar system based on language use and communication.

Therefore, this study shall be divided into six main sections. Chapter 2 provides an account of the
concept of grammar from the proper philosophy about human nature and human use of language
which have reflected the culture and intellectual climate of the moment. Chapter 3 provides the
reader with a historical and theoretical approach to grammar in language and language learning,
which will account for English grammar in step from traditional English grammars to present-day
grammar systems based on language use and communication.

Chapter 3, then, is intended to present five major English grammar systems in their historical,
chronological order so as to offer the entire history of language study: Normative Grammar,
Descriptive Grammar, Structural Grammar, Transformational -Generative Grammar, and finally
Communicative Grammar. In order to consider how grammatical models have developed from
prescriptive, descriptive, structuralist, and generative approaches towards less formalistic
grammars, commonly known as communicative, this section offers (1) definition, (2) historical
background, (3) main approaches, and (4) the various methods to linguistics.

Chapter 4 examines the role of communicative grammar within our current educational system and
presents future directions regarding communicative grammar. In Chapter 5 a relevant discussion
shall be drawn from the considerable information gathered in this study, and in Chapter 6 all the
books and sources which have been involved to carry out this study are presented in alphabetical
order.


1.2. Notes on bibliography.

Numerous sources have contributed to provide an overall basis for the development of the unit. A
valuable introduction to the study of language, still indispensable, is given by Otto Jespersen,
Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin (1922); David Crystal, Linguistics (1985); and
Baugh and Cable, A History of the English Language (1993). For a historical overview of the
tradition of language teaching, see Howatt, A History of English Language Teaching (1984); Jack
C. Richards and Theodore S. Rodgers, Approaches and Methods in Language Teaching (1992);
Lydia White, Universal Grammar and Second Language Acquisition (1989); and Frederick
Newmeyer, Grammatical Theory: Its Limits and Its Possibilities (1983).



4/49
Among the many general works that incorporate an introduction to grammar, as well as a historical
background, we may find the still influential work of Lyda E. LaPalombara, An Introduction to
Grammar (1976), where we are presented the history of grammar from its earliest beginnings to the
current linguistic scene. Furthermore, other general works are Frank Palmer, Grammar (1971);
Geoffrey Horrocks, Generative Grammar (1987); Riley and Parker, English Grammar:
Prescriptive, Descriptive, Generative, Performance (1998); and more recently, Kolln & Funk,
Understanding English Grammar (1999).

The most complete record of current publications on new directions in teaching grammar is
published by B.O.E. RD N 112, 13 September (2002); and van Ek & and Trim, Vantage (2001).
Moreover, Three good places for grammar research on the Internet are: (1) http://www.swan.ac.uk;
(2) http://www.ling.lancs.ac.uk; and (3) http://www.geocities.com. Bibliography is fully presented
at the end of this work.




2. THE CONCEPT OF GRAMMAR.

This section accounts for the concept of grammar from the proper philosophy about human nature
and human use of language which have reflected the culture and intellectual climate of the moment,
and which later on, would have considerable influence on the main models on language teaching.
Therefore, we shall divide this section in (1) the origin and nature of grammar, (2) a definition of
grammar, and (3) a general historical approach to grammar, which will provide us with a
relevant framework for the next section, that is, the link between the concept of grammar and
language learning.


2.1. On the origin and nature of grammar.

In order to examine the origin and nature of grammar, we must trace back to the notion of
communication and language. Palmer (1971) states that for centuries men have been interested in
the language they speak, with the premise that language is a communication system where we find
a complex set of relations that link the sounds of the language (or its written symbols) with the
meanings, that is, the message they have to convey. This complex set, called grammar, is what
makes language so essentially a human characteristic.

Hence, there are three main characteristics of language that are important for the understanding of
the nature of grammar: it is complex, productive and arbitrary. First, complex in the sense that up to
now it has not proved possible to translate mechanically from one language to another, with really
satisfactory results; second, productive in the sense that we can produce millions of sentences that
we have never heard or uttered before; and third, arbitrary in the sense that there is no one-to-one
relation between sound a meaning.



5/49
Yet, every culture has something to communicate and they all have their own ways of
communication. When compared, the main difference is the enormous complexity in structuring the
systems from language to language, and it is within this complexity that we must look for grammar,
since they namely differ in their grammatical structure. This is why languages are different.
Therefore, grammar is said to refer to that complex set of relations between sounds and meaning,
and according to Palmer (1971), a device that specifies the infinite set of well-formed sentences,
and that assigns to each of them one or more structural description.

Moreover, Jespersen (1933) adds that since language consists of words, the way in which these
words are modified and joined together to express thoughts and feelings differs from one language
to another, for instance, English and French have many words in common but treat them in a totally
different way (i.e. excuse). Hence, since grammar deals with the structure of languages, the
grammar of each language constitutes a system of its own, where each element stands in a certain
relation to, and is dependent on, all the others. Finally, he states that language is nothing but a set
of human habits, the purpose of which is to give expression to thoughts and feelings, and especially
to impart them to others.


2.2. On defining the term grammar.

The concept of grammar emerges from the major concern of early Greek and Roman scholarship on
thought about language. In fact, the term grammar comes from the Greek word grammatike,
meaning to write or grammatike techne, meaning the art of writing. Greeks developed an
alphabet different in principle from previous writing systems, and considered to be the forerunner of
most subsequent alphabets. As we shall see, their permanent contribution in this area is nicely
indicated by the history of the term grammar, which in this early period implied understanding the
use of letters, that is, having the skill of reading and writing (Crystal 1985).

Plato, Aristotle, and the Stoics devoted a great deal of time to the development of specific ideas
about language, and in particular, to grammatical analysis. Hence, Plato was called by a later Greek
writer the first to discover the potentialities of grammar' and his conception of speech (logos) as
being basically composed of logically determined categories. This fairly study of the language, part
of the more general study of dialectic, was taken over by the Romans with very little change in
principle, and, through the influence of Latin on Europe, was introduced into every grammatical
handbook written before the twentieth century.

As we have seen, the Greek definition of grammar restricted the term to the written language,
although this connotation was to be changed by the Romans since the spoken language also has a
grammar. Hence, grammar was defined in popular Latin as ars bene dicendi et bene scribendi, that
is, the art of speaking and writing well (Crystal, 1985). Indeed, there are still hundreds of
languages in the world that have no written form although they all have spoken grammars in the
sense in which we are interested in the term.



6/49
There is, however, an implication here that the written form carries the grammar because it, unlike
speech, lasts over the centuries. Jespersen (1933) claims that in our so-called civilized life many
educated people forget that language is primarily speech while the written word is only a kind of
substitute. Also, elements that give expression to emotions, such as stress, pitch, and colour of the
voice, disappear in the medium of writing or are rendered by such means as italicizing, underlining
and punctuation.

For him, it should be remembered that this linguistic intercourse takes place not in isolated words
but by means of connected communications, namely in the form of sentences, which are mainly
found in writing, and even on that of everyday speech. As we may see, his words are still relevant
within our current educational framework, as nowadays grammar is considered from a
communicative approach.

Before providing a current definition of grammar, we must point out that this term can be
understood in a number of ways in everyday use . First, (1) grammar as something that can be good
or bad, correct or incorrect, or in other words, a whole lot of rules which would tell people on what
they should do or not. In this sense, grammar is understood as the art of speaking and writing a
language correctly, or as we shall see later, prescriptive grammar. A recent example is drawn from
the Beatles: I used to get mad at my school,/ the teachers who taught me werent cool,/ holding me
down, turning me round,/ filling me up with your rules .

(2) Second, grammar as a book on grammar. In this sense, the grammar of the language is no
more than the grammar as presented by the author of the book. And third, (3) grammar as the
science of describing how language is used or descriptive grammar as we shall see later. In other
words, when grammar is used in the technical sense within the linguistic field, and at the level of
analysis in linguistic structure dealing with the organisation of words into sentences.

Contemporary linguists define grammar as the rules of a language governing the sounds, words,
sentences, and other elements, as well as their combination and interpretation. In fact, it refers to the
underlying structure of a language that any native speaker of that language knows intuitively, that
is, as Jespersen (1933) stated, an implicit body of knowledge about how a language works that each
person has in his or her mind since grammar is part of each persons mental capacity, and it allows
the person to know implicitly the rules of that persons language.

The word grammar also denotes the study of these abstract features or a book presenting the rules
since it contains information about the lexicon (vocabulary and the meaning of words), the
languagess phonological system, syntactic structures and information about the semantics and
pragmatics of the language.






7/49
2.3. A historical approach to grammar.

After defining the term, we shall approach, broadly speaking, the concept of grammar in historical
terms. Our discussion traces back to those days when the history of language was bound up with
religious thought in its widest sense. In fact, it was the language of worship which triggered off the
preserving of early states of language from the effects of time by means of grammar. This decision
took place in ancient India around the 5
th
century B.C. when the Hindu priests realized that the
language of their oldest hymns, Vedic Sanskrit, was no longer the same, either in pronunciation or
grammar, as the contemporary language.

For an important part of their belief was that certain religious ceremonies, to be successful, needed
to reproduce accurately the original pronunciation and text of the hymns used. So, the solution
adopted was to determine exactly what the salient features of Vedic Sanskrit were, and to write
them down as a set of rules. The earliest evidence we have of this feat is the work carried out by
Panini in the fourth century B.C., in the form of a set of around 4,000 aphoristic statements about
the languages structure, known as sutras.

Also, there were other ways in which religious studies and goals promoted language study, since
later on, still under the aegis of the Church, missionaries often introduced writing by stating the first
grammars of languages, and priests and scholars translated works such as the Bible and the
Scriptures. Up to this point, with the background knowledge from the previous sections in hand, we
should be ready to explore the fascinating link of grammar to language teaching.



3. A GENERAL OVERVIEW OF GRAMMAR IN LANGUAGE AND LANGUAGE
LEARNING: FROM NORMATIVE TO COMMUNICATIVE GRAMMAR.

In this chapter we shall offer a general overview of grammar in language and language learning
from normative to communicative grammar. In order to discuss the concept of grammar within a
theory of language, we shall review the different approaches throughout history, and within a
language learning theory, we shall review the different methods proposed over the years to the ones
in use today.

Therefore, our study will be approached from a historical and theoretical point of view since they
both are interrelated. Thus, a historical approach will help us establish a chronological sequence of
events that will lead us to a description of the five major models of grammar from a theoretical
point of view, concerning approaches and methods. In so doing, we shall offer a general overview
of the underlying structures of language up to present-day, and the issues of using, learning, and
teaching out of which contemporary theories of language have been produced.

In order to clarify the description, analysis, and comparison of the five main models of grammar,
this chapter considers how learning and teaching methods and approaches to linguistics have


8/49
developed from prescriptive, descriptive, structuralist, and generative approaches towards less
formalistic grammars, that is functional grammars based on language usage and communication.

Special attention is given to functional grammar (also called communicative, competence,
pedagogic, teaching, and linguistic grammar), as functionally oriented approaches to linguistics
(Widdowson, 1978; Halliday, 1985) are nowadays a good basis for discussing the link between
linguistics and models of language learning and language acquisition, a further focus of our present
work.

Therefore, the present chapter on models of grammar and language learning shall include in each
section (1) definition, (2) historical background, (3) main approaches, and (4) teaching methods. In
order to do so, we shall mainly follow Jespersen (1933), Chomsky (1957), Hymes (1972),
LaPalombara (1976), Krashen (1983), Halliday (1985), Riley & Parker (1998), Richards & Rodgers
(2001), and Huddleston (2002) among others.



3.1. On terminology: approach and method.

Before carrying on with our analysis, just a brief note on terminology between the notions of
approach and method, which refer respectively to the concepts of theory and practice. So,
when approaching the analysis of grammar models, the term approach deals with the general
principles and theories concerning how languages are learned, how knowledge of language is
represented and organized in memory, or how language itself is structured. For our purposes, an
approach would deal with those principles for the selection and sequencing of vocabulary and
grammar.

Closely related, the term method refers to the practical realities of the classroom which are based
on a philosophy of language teaching at the level of theory and principles. A method is a set of
derived procedures for teaching a language, that is, the level at which theory is put into practice and
at which choices are made about the particular skills to be taught, the content to be taught, and the
order in which the content will be presented.

The history of language teaching throughout much of the twentieth century saw the rise and fall of a
variety of language teaching approaches and methods on different assumptions about how a second
language is learned. Their common belief is that, if language learning is to be improved, it will
come about through changes and improvements in teaching methodology. In fact, the most active
period in the history of approaches and methods was from the 1950s to the 1980s, which coincides
with the changing period from descriptive grammars to communicative ones, that is, from
structuralist views of language teaching to more eclectic and communicative approaches.

Therefore, in our analysis, we will present approaches before methods since a theory of language
will provide us with general principles and theories in language learning so as to establish the nature


9/49
of the different methods in language teaching. As stated before, the four main models are as
follows.



3.2. A brief definition of grammar models.

There are four main types of English grammars in history up to present-day. Among the many
definitions given, we have chosen that of Riley & Parker (1998) since it has been one of the most
influential readings within this field for the last few years. Therefore, we may distinguish (1)
prescriptive (also traditional, classical, vernacular and normative) grammar; (2) descriptive (or
explanatory) grammar; (3) structural grammar; (4) generative (or transformational) grammar; and
(5) performance (also called linguistic or communicative) grammar. Note that details on their way
of structuring language will be given in their corresponding chapters as this section is intended to be
an introductory presentation.

(1) Prescriptive grammars were the first grammars in history and they refered to the Latin teaching
methods of correct and incorrect. However, (2) descriptive grammars claimed that a language
should be described not according to a grammarians notion about what ought to be, but according
to the language practices that actually exist among the native speakers of a language. (3) Structural
grammar is an extension of descriptive grammar, but goes beyond the level of word structure
straightforward into the sound system until getting to the notion of smallest phonological unit,
phoneme. (4) The so-called notion of Transformational-Generative Grammar is based on
cognitive factors and is made up of a set of rules which make it clear which sequences of words and
sentences are possible. (5) Finally, communicative grammars, that is, grammars based on the usage
of language and communication, are the ones applied to language teaching nowadays.

Once we have briefly defined the four types of grammars, we shall related them to different
historical periods, approaches, and methods so as to get to the core of their origin and nature.



3.3. Normative Grammar.

3.3.1. Definition.

As stated before, prescriptive grammars, also labelled as traditional, and normative grammars, were
dominant from the earliest years up to approximately the middle of the eighteenth century. This
term establishes a straightforward connection between Latin and its teaching methods and the
notion of what is correct and incorrect. Grammar was seen as a set of rules of correct usage in
order to speak and write correctly (Jespersen, 1933) which did not take into account usage
practices.



10/ 49
Traditional grammars make use of a fairly wide technical vocabulary to describe the concepts they
use, for instance, words like noun, verb, agreement, plural, clause and even word itself.
Unfortunately the usual practice in this kind of grammar is to give some kind of definition of most
of the words, but never to question the whole justification of their use. Also, traditional grammars
restricted grammar knowledge to just written texts.



3.3.2. Historical background.

3.3.2.1. Early educational systems.

The history of grammar within the framework of language learning goes back to the earliest
educational systems whose main aim was to teach religion and to promote the traditions of the
people. These practices trace back to the temple schools of ancient Egypt where the principles of
writing, the sciences, mathematics, and architecture were taught. Grammar was, until then, part of
those writing principles, and it was in ancient India that Hindu priests mentioned the concept of
grammar for the first time, as a set of grammar rules called sutras. Much of this education was
carried on by priests with the Buddhist doctrines that later spread to the Far East.

The ancient Jewish traditions of the Old Testament also played an important role in formation of
later education systems. The foundation of Jewish education is the Torah (the Biblical books of
mosaic law) and the Talmud, which set forth the aims and methods of education among Jews.
Jewish parents were urged by the Talmud to teach their children such subjects as ethics, vocational
knowledge, swimming, and a foreign language, where grammar was implicit. During the Middle
Ages (15th-16th century), the early educational systems of the nations of the Western world
emanated from the Judea-Christian religious traditions, which were combined with traditions
derived from ancient Greece philosophers like Socrates, Plato, and Aristotle.


3.3.2.2. Ancient Greece.

The notion of grammar in Ancient Greece was developed under the theory of natural logic, and
therefore, there were speculations about words and their meanings. Among the most relevant Greek
philosophers, we shall consider Plato, Aristotle, and Thraxs contributions. Thus, Plato concluded
that a given word bears an inherent, natural, and therefore logical relationship to the thing or
concept for which it stands (LaPalombara, 1976) and, as a result, he devised what is possibly the
first word-classification system in the western world. His system, based on meaning, had but two
word classes: onoma and rhema, which bear a striking resemblance to traditional grammars noun
and verb classes.


11/ 49
Later on Aristotle, Platos most gifted pupil, continued with the investigation on words and their
meanings
1
, and among his most important contributions we find a developed system of natural
logic, based on the concept of the syllogism, representing the universal system of human thinking.
After Aristotle, the next important work in language study is that of the Stoics, who made their
philosophical inquiries around 300 B.C. by expanding Aristotles word classes to four, adding
articles to nouns, verbs, and conjunctions.

The Stoics were also concerned with inquiring into the nature of language, their goal being to
demonstrate that the outer forms of language reveal inner truths about human nature. Thus, the
present-day linguistic interest in outer and inner forms of language, what transformational
grammarians refer to as surface structure and deep structure, may have its first faint beginnings with
the work of the Stoics.

The last philosopher under review, Dionys ius Thrax, must be mentioned. In his book Techne
Grammatike, written around the first century B.C., he still based his grammar classifications on
meaning, expanding the word classes to eight
2
, for which he gave a detailed definition and provided
many examples. So, Thrax was influential in two ways, first, in establishing the basis for descriptive
grammars and second, in studying language in his search for universals.

In the context of language teaching and learning, a clear influence of the Greek and Latin language
is present. In Greece, Plato, Aristotle, and the Stoics examined carefully the structure of language as
part of the general study of dialectic. This study had a major influence on subsequent grammatical
thinking which was taken over by the Romans with very little change.


3.3.2.3. Ancient Rome.

In ancient Rome, we know that very large sums were often paid for the services of Greek slaves
who were grammar scholars. When Roman scholars wrote their first Latin grammars, they patterned
them after the earlier Greek models since they were inflected languages with many grammatical
similarities. We must remember that they had as a theoretical background the idea that words in
particular and language in general are natural and logical.

Thus, Quintilian, a Latin scholar (around the second century A.D.) stressed the importance of
including the study of grammar and rhetoric in the education of the cultured Roman. He established

1
Following LaPalombara (1976), among Aristotles most important contributions to language study, we find (1) the
addition of a third word class, syndesmoi (equivalent to traditional conjunction class); (2) the addition of certain structural
word features, such as that nouns possess case and that verbs possess tense; (3) finally, he probably provided the earliest
definition of the concept word, as the smallest meaningful language unit. This definition is very close to the modern
structuralists definition of morpheme.

2
Thraxs eight classes were roughly equivalent to nouns, pronouns, verbs, participles, articles, adverbs, conjunctions, and
prepositions. In this sense, Thrax seems to have been influential in establishing as linguistic gospel that the best way to
describe a language was to begin with a description of words. Although unaware, he set a basis for descriptive grammar
hundred of years later.


12/ 49
the proper usage of language under three criteria: reason, authority, and antiquity. Two other Latin
grammarians of continuing influence were first, Donatus (about A.D. 350), who wrote two books,
one on parts of speech, and another (Ars Minor) in which he summarized the basics of Latin
grammar; and second, Priscian (about A.D. 500), who wrote an eighteen-volume Latin grammar.

Yet, Roman grammarians came to regard written classical Latin as their sacred duty to preserve the
purity of Latin from whatever decay the vernacular might impose, as spoken vernacular represented
corrupt practices of language. This attitude, as we shall see later, influenced language study in the
centuries that followed on what is known in theoretical grounds as prescriptive grammar.

One more point should be made before we continue. It is worth noting that there was an important
difference between the Greek and Roman cultures regarding grammar: whereas Greek scholars had
studied grammar as a part of their larger philosophical concern with the nature of humans and the
universe, the educated Roman, on the other hand, studied the grammar of languages in terms of
proper usage. Thus, they studied two languages: their native Latin, to develop their oratory powers
since the ability to sway people with ones rethoric came to be highly admired, and Greek, as a
foreign language since it had become a valued cultural accomplishment.


3.3.2.4. The Middle Ages (A.D. 400-1400s).

The Medieval period is usually dated from about A.D. 400 until the late 1400s. It should be
remembered, however, that no era begins or ends overnight, but rather undergoes gradual changes
shaped by a variety of events. Broadly speaking, scholars defined the Middle Ages as a time when
respect for learning died and when no new ideas emerged. In fact, the Medieval Period was referred
to as the Dark Ages.

To have a better understanding of developments in language study during this period, we should try
to get at least a general impression of the times. The inevitable result of the expansion of the Roman
Empire was the widespread disemination of Roman culture and, therefore customs, laws, religion,
and of course the Latin language. Then, for a number of complex reasons, among which we may
mention overextension, decentralization, and troubles at home, the Empire began slowly to crumble
down. Consequently, social organization changed, and so did values.

A new power structure emerged from war and due to the multiplying attacks, safety and security
demanded personal protection in return for properties. As a result, nearly every man of the period,
powerful families or peasants, became a warrior. In these new circumstances, scholarly learning
was not in demand, and in fact, many feudal knights and lords were crude men with little or no
education.

The only Roman institution which not only survived but indeed extended its influence during the
Middle Ages was the Catholic church. The aexis of Church represented the only power greater even
than that of the feudal lords and became the medium of educated discourse and communication by

13/ 49
the end of the first millenium. Moreover, respect for Greek and Latin learning started to increase
and was preserved and nourished by the Church, namely in the monasteries scattered throughout
Europe.

It is worth noting for our current purposes that two different levels of Latin co-existed: the literary
Latin of the classical scholars, and the common vernacular Latin of the people. Literary Latin,
according to Crystal (1985), became the official language within the Catholic Church, and
therefore, the emphasis in language study was for a while almost exclusively concerned with the
description of the Church Latin in the context of language teaching. On the contrary, vernacular (or
Roman) Latin of the common people was carried with the Roman armies in the invasions, with two
important consequences: first, Latin borrowings affected the development of many European
vernacular languages (i.e. Italian, Spanish, French), and second, the impact of local vernaculars on
common Latin made it disappear as a spoken European language.

Then, when it had come to be recognized that Latin was no longer a native language for the
majority of its prospective users, the grammar books became less sets of facts and more sets of
rules, and the concept of correctness became even more dominant. This approach brought about a
massive codification of Latin grammars such as those of Aelius Donatus (fourth century) and
Priscian (sixth century) Thus, Donatus grammar was used right into the Middle Ages, and became
a popular grammar known as being the first to be printed using wooden type, and providing a
shorter edition for children.

Therefore, throughout this period, we may observe a high standard of correctness in learning. The
Benedictine Rule, for example, heavily punished the mistakes of children in Latin classes. It is
worth noting that this use of grammar rules promoted the development of written skills in language
teaching, as we may observe in a popular Latin definition of grammar, that is, ars bene dicendi et
bene scribendi, which means the art of speaking and writing well.

Later, in the age of humanism, it was common to hear people identify the aim of learning grammar
with the ideal of being able to write Latin like Cicero. A similar attitude had also characterized
Greek language teaching, especially after the Alexandrian school (third century B.C.), considered to
be the language of the best literature, was held up as a guide to the desired standard of speech and
writing. Grammars were considered, then, to tell people authoritatively how to speak and write, in
other words, this is the historical framework for prescriptive or normative grammars.

Among the Medieval scholars who have been recently considered to be precursors of current
grammar theories, we may mention St. Anselm. He was a mid-eleventh century French Benedictine
abbot, who wrote a treatise entitled De Grammatico, with considerable interest in grammatical
distinctions such as that expressed by the concepts of signifier and thing signified (linked in time
to Saussures semiotics). However, this concern was not new since other scholars before him
already discussed semantic differences in word meanings.


14/ 49
Yet, during the last quarter of the thirteenth century, Peter of Spain (later to become Pope John
XXI) was also interested in the grammatical and semantic implications of different meanings
attributable to a single word or expression. Peter also detected differences between significatio and
the suppositio of a word. Another relevant figure, who is said to have had a strong influence on the
genesis of scholarly seventeenth century philosophical grammar, is the work of Peter Helias, a
Parisian mid-twelfth century scholar. His main contribution was to stress the real significance and
importance of grammar study.

It is worth pointing out that more and more work of Medieval scholars is coming to the attention of
contemporary linguists since they are becoming aware, after years of neglect, of yet another
important development: the growing body of written grammars of the European vernaculars
languages (LaPalombara, 1976). It has been now documented that written grammar of Hebrew,
Arabic, Old Irish, and Old Norse existed before the middle of the twelfth century in contrast to
other European vernacular languages which had not been written until the end of the sixteenth
century.

Following LaPalombara (1976), one of the most fascinating of these vernacular grammars was the
First Grammatical Treatise (mid-twelfth century), an Old Norse grammar written in the vernacular
by a now unknown scholar who has since then been called the First Grammarian. This work is
especially interesting to modern linguists because, as has only recently discovered, many of his
language study techniques (i.e. phonology in Icelandic or minimal pairs) were very much like
those developed independently by nineteenth and twentieth century historical and structural
linguists.


3.3.2.5. The Renaissance (late 1400s-1600).

Again, it is impossible to assign exact dates for the Reinassance period, but approximately this
period dates from sometime in the late 1400s up to the seventeenth century. Renaissance means
reawakening and, therefore, it refers to the return to normality in Medieval Europe. As peoples
fortunes improved, their attention could once more be turned to other matters. Gradually, interest in
scholarship and in cultural things was renewed. Once again, men of leisure focused their attention
on the work of earlier Greek and Roman scholars for enlightenment.

Following LaPalombara (1976), among those scholars whose work has been recently examined by
twentieth century linguists, was a sixteenth century Spanish classical scholar called Francisco
Snchez de las Brozas, or simply Sanctius. He was a professor of Greek and Rethoric in Salamanca
and his book Minerva (around 1587) was considered the standard work on Latin grammar. As we
know, in the sixteenth century the status of Latin changed from a living language that learners
needed to be able to read, write in, and speak, to a dead language which was studied as an
intellectual exercise (Richards & Rodgers, 1992).



15/ 49
Sanctius believed that all languages, in spite of their superficial differences, shared a single
universal set of underlying principles which were common to all human language. However, two
hundred years before those language similarites were own up to descend from the same source.
More recently, Noam Chomsky suggested in Language and Mind (1968) that Sanctius already
devised the theory as a method for the interpretation of literary texts.

Other influential scholars whose works are thought to have contributed to the thinking of
seventeenth century philosophers and grammarians, are (1) the Spanish physician Huarte, who
argued that human language has two powers: first, that it is rooted in the senses, and second, that it
implies a generative or creative ability; and (2) Peter Ramus, who wrote grammars of Greek, Latin,
and French, and who, in his work Scholae stressed the current usage of native speakers as the best
guide to usage practices.


3.3.2.6. The seventeenth century and beyond.

Moreover, the seventeenth century is characterized by a philosophical debate between the
rationalists and the empiricists. The rationalist position emerged from the philosophical writings
of Ren Descartes who held that certain human abilities, capacities, and ideas were innate. He also
argued that the acquisition of knowledge is determined by certain abstract, built-in principles
which are present in every normal person from the moment of birth, and which allow the creative
use of language. We are dealing with the notion of grammar.

On the other hand, the empiricists John Locke and David Hume among others, insisted on language
as a explainable, sense-oriented, and learned behaviour. They denied the existence of innate ideas.
This controversy was so important that it dominated the thinking of scholars in many disciplines,
including linguistics, and therefore the notion of grammar, for several decades.

Rationalist (or Cartesian) grammarians followed Greek philosophers, Descartes, and Sanctius in
that language had both an outer and an inner form. The most important work of rationalist thinking
was done by a group of monks at the Port Royal monastery in Port Royal, France, who, intrigued by
the great many similarities among languages, believed that there were basic universal language
principles.

Elaborating Descartes hypothesis of the inner and outer aspects of language, they developed two
terms from the old ones: surface structures and deep structures. Since they reasoned that language
is phrase-rather than word-oriented, surface structure would refer to the observable outer
grammatical form of a sentence whereas surface structure represents the implicit body of abstract
ideas or thought relationships which are present in the human mind. As we shall see later, these two
notions were reviewed again some centuries later by generative grammar.

The mid- and late eighteenth century reflected the changing climate of the times which took place in
relation to a new social situation and the rise of the English language. After years of neglect, and

16/ 49
due to political, industrial, economical, and social reasons (i.e. colonization, immigration from rural
areas to cities), English gradually became a perfectly respectable, healthy, and thriving world
language by the middle of the eighteenth century.

Consequently, a new and growing merchant class felt the need for acquiring some city polish in
their manners, dress, and speech in a hope of gaining social acceptance in urban circles. As
prescriptivist grammars dominated, a few were written in the middle of the eighteenth century.
Actually, the first widely respected English dictionary and the first detailed vernacular English
grammar which were published in those years are, respectively, Dr. Samuel Johnsons Dictionary of
the English Language (1755), and Bishop Robert Lowths A Short Introduction to English
Grammar (1762).



3.3.3. Main approaches.

Although Port Royal monks tried to replace Latin with French as the scholarly language in France,
the analysis of the grammar and rhetoric of Classical Latin became the model for foreign language
teaching from the seventeenth to the nineteenth centuries. As the status of Latin diminished from
that of a living language to that of an occassional subject in the school curriculum, the study of
Latin took on a different function.

Teaching approaches, then, were traditional and extremely rigorous when introducing Latin
grammar. This was taught through rote learning of grammar rules, study of declensions and
conjugations, translation, and practice in writing sample sentences. One basic proficiency was
established, whereby students were introduced to the advanced study of grammar and rhetoric, and
brutally punished for lapses in knowledge (Richards & Rodgers, 2001).

Most traditional school grammars began by defining and classifying English words into part-of-
speech categories, and proceeded from there to more inclusive sentence components until they
arrived at a discussion of the sentence itself. Although there are many drawbacks to this kind of
approach, this was the kind of grammar taught in many American schools for many years, and it
proved impossible to evaluate the usefulness of a particular grammar system without first
examining it carefully.

Although other attempts to promote alternative teaching approaches were made (Roger Ascham and
Montaigne in the sixteenth century, and Comenius and John Locke in the seventeenth century), the
establishment of reforms in the curriculum and changes in the way of teaching Latin was not
successful since Latin was regarded as a classical and ideal form of language.

In the eighteenth century, Latin continued to be used by European scholars to such extent that, when
modern languages began to enter the curriculum of European schools, they were taught using the
same basic procedures that were used for teaching Latin. Thus, textbooks consisted of statements of

17/ 49
abstract grammar rules, lists of vocabulary, and sentences for translation. Those sentences
illustrated the grammatical system of the language and consequently bore no relation to the
language of real communication.

Moreover, as seen before, Bishop Robert Lowth stated that the most reliable grammatical authority
was reason or logic, and rules of correctness based on the practices of the best educated among the
language community. Meanwhile, the term traditional grammar came to refer exclusively to their
brand of schoolroom grammar, and for more than a century, the prescriptivist approach remained
basically unquestioned.

By the nineteenth century, this approach which was based on the study of Latin became the
standard way of studying foreign languages in schools, and became known as the Grammar-
Translation Method.



3.3.4. Main methods.

3.3.4.1. The Grammar-Translation Method.

The Grammar-Translation Method was the dominant foreign language teaching method in Europe
from the 1840s to the 1940s, and a version of it continues to be widely used in some parts of the
world. As the names of some of its leading exponents suggest (Johann SEidenstcker, Karl Pltz,
H.S. Ollendorf, and Johann Medidinger), this method was the offspring of German scholarship, and
in fact it was first known in the United States as the Prussian Method.

As Richards & Rodgers (2001) points out, it is still used nowadays where understanding literary
texts is the primary focus of foreign language study in order to benefit from mental discipline and
intellectual development. Its main characteristics are: (1) a detailed analysis of grammar rules
(translation, memorizing) in order to understand and manipulate the morphology and syntax of the
foreign language; (2) a focus on reading and writing; (3) vocabulary selection from texts and words
which are taught through bilingual lists or dictionary study; (4) the sentence is considered to be the
basic unit of teaching and language practice; (5) emphasis on accuracy; (6) deductively teaching
methods; and (7) the students native language as a medium of instruction.

The grammar-translation methods main failures are that it does not sound natural to a native
speaker, produces difficult mistakes to eradicate, it is a tedious experience since students had to
memorize endless lists of unusable grammar rules and vocabulary, and finally, there was little or no
stress on accurate pronunciation and oral skills.

The mid- and late nineteenth century brought about a serious opposition to this method, known as
the Reform Movement, which was gradually developed in several European countries. It laid the


18/ 49
foundations for the development of new approaches to the teaching of languages which raised
controversies that have continued to the present day.



3.4. Descriptive Grammars.

3.4.1. Definition.

Descriptive grammars claimed that a language should be described not according to a grammarians
prescription about what ought to be, but according to the language practices that actually exist
among the native speakers of a language. Jespersen (1933) defines them as those grammars which
instead of serving as a guide to what should be said or written, aim at finding out what is actually
said and written by the speakers of the language investigated, and thus may lead to a scientific
understanding of the rules followed instinctively by speakers and writers. Spoken language, until
now rejected, was included in the definition of grammar.

Descriptive grammars have always been related to traditional grammars as a binary contrast
between prescriptive and descriptive. They are considered both traditional and pre-structuralist
since they were based on traditional grammar but had already envisaged in early years a description
of language in terms of structure. In fact, they prepared the ground for structural linguistics. Yet,
they were in vogue until the most influential school of linguistics described language as structural
in the early nineteen-thirties.



3.4.2. Historical background.

During the late seventeenth and early eighteenth centuries, prescriptive grammars were still written
but, as English gained social acceptance in urban circles and language scholars became interested in
etymologies of words, a major linguistic controversy was developed, that between the descriptivists
and the prescriptivist writers of particular grammars.


3.4.2.1. The early descriptivists.

Among the early descriptivists, we find, first, the work of Panini, an ancient Indian scholar, who
had written an extremely detailed grammar of Sanskrit sometime during the fifth century B.C. Like
the earliest Greek language scholars, Panini also devised a word classification system but, instead of
classifying words according to their meanings and semantic functions, Panini analyzed and
classified words and word parts into roots, prefixeds, and suffixes. Secondly, we may mention the
Port Royal grammar scholars who wrote about rules of correct usage rather than rules of
correctness.

19/ 49
Some centuries later, during the late eighteenth and early nineteenth centuries in western Europe,
travel to foreign countries was by then relatively common, and as an inevitable outcome of such
sidening horizons, scholars began to take note of the vast number of similarities among European
vernacular languages. In the 1780s and Englishman, Sir William Jones, contributed invaluable
linguistic information since he came across Paninis work.


3.4.2.2. Historical and comparative linguistic studies.

Sir William Jones, a government official with the East India Company, became fascinated with
Sanskrit. As Jones examined Paninis classifications, he became convinced that the enormous
number of similarities between the roots of Sanskrit and those of Greek and Latin provided strong
evidence that all three of these languages were in some way related. Jones, then, hypothesized that a
great many other European and Asian languages probably had histories which could be traced back
to the same original parent language.

Hence, a great interest on these speculations aroused in many European scholars and they began to
conduct their own investigations on comparative linguistics. Thus, in 1816 Franz Bopp, a German
language scholar who is often called the founder of historical and comparative linguistics, published
Uber das Conjugationssystem. In his comparative studies of verb inflections of Sanskrit, Greek,
Latin, Persian, and Germanic languages, he offered convincing support that it was quite possible to
recover enough empirical historical linguistic evidence to be able to reconstruct a fairly close
approximation of that ancient Indo-European language which had been the source of those
languages.

Also, one of the most important of these new theories had a naturalistic basis. So, in 1859 Charles
Darwin published On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, where he argued that
humans had gradually evolved. In the years between the late 1810s and the early 1870s, great
numbers of archeologists, anthropologists, paleontologists, and philologists spent time in the field
unearthing vast bodies of evidence on the sometimes incomprehensible mysteries of language.

New methods, developed by historical and comparative linguists, became meticulous and refined,
and one of the common agreements they had was on phonological aspects, that is, the view that
language was a continuous, open-ended changing process that never stops so long as a language
continues to be a viable spoken tongue. There was a growing interest on the sounds of language and
their nature.

Some scholars revived speculations about the origin of language, such as that the first words had
been imitations of animal sounds (the Bow Wow Theory) or that language had originated with
song as a need to express such feelings as love, rather than by a need for practical communication.
In conducting their research, they found themselves relying heavily on the comparative sounds of
languages, always working chronologically backwards.


20/ 49
In England, the work of the historical-comparative linguists culminated in the publication of the
massive twelve-volume Oxford English Dictionary, which was first planned in 1857. Not until
1888, thirty years after its conception, was the first of four volumes published. The final volume
appeared in 1928, seventy years after the project had begun.

So, the interest of the great majority of nineteenth century linguists in England and Europe was
focused on the exciting work in historical and comparative linguistics, and in the meantime, the
prescriptive grammars of the pedagogues were becoming ever more firmly entrenched in English
and American schools.


3.4.2.3. The early reformers.

As seen before, towards the mid-nineteenth century the Grammar-Translation method was rejected
and questionned, for instance, the American lexicographer Noah Webster (American Dictionary,
1864) spoke out against the prescriptivists slavish reliance on the authority of Latin. Several
factors, such as the increased opportunities for communication among Europeans, created a demand
for oral proficiency in foreign languages.

Initially, this created a market for private study, but language teaching specialists also turned their
attention to the way modern languages were being taught in secondary schools. Increasingly, public
education failed in its responsabilities, and new approaches to language teaching were developed by
individual language teaching specialists, such as Claude Marcel, Thomas Prendergast and Franois
Gouin.

The Frenchman Claude Marcel (1793-1896) emphasized the importance of meaning in learning,
proposing a rational method, and referring to child language learning as a model for language
teaching. The Englishman Thomas Prendergast (1806-1886) created a mastery system on a
structural syllabus to work on basic structural patterns occurring in the language. He was one of the
first to record the observation of children in speaking. The Frenchman Franois Gouin (1831-1896)
is perhaps the best known of these reformers because of his approach to teaching based on his
observations of childrens use of language.

It would be inaccurate to suppose, however, that the dominance of the prescriptivists eliminated the
tradition of the descriptive grammarians, for linguists like Sir William Jones (1746-1794), Henry
Sweet (A New English Grammar, 1891), and Otto Jespersen (Language, 1922) kept the tradition of
scholarly descriptive grammar alive. Most of these linguists failed to gain wide public acclaim in
their own time, but their work continued unabated. Their voices would ultimately be heard with
great respect (LaPalombara, 1976).





21/ 49
3.4.3. Main approaches.

The discoveries of the European historical-comparative linguists were influential both in shedding
much light on the understanding of language development and in ridding language scholars on some
of their earlier ideas about the nature of language. In addition, their newly developed empirical
methods paved the way for new approaches to language study within a Reform Movement which
developed principles for language teaching on naturalistic principles of language learning, such as
are seen in first language acquisition.

Perhaps the most significant works which made use of these new field-study methods, based on
natural and sound methodological principles, were those begun in England by a group of applied
linguists, and those begun in the United States by a small group of American anthropologists around
the turn of the century. According to Richards & Rodgers (1992), this would led to what have been
termed natural methods and ultimately led to the development of what came to be known as the
Direct Method, and later on, to the oral approach both in Europe and in the United States.


3.4.3.1. In Europe: The Reform Movement.

Towards the end of the nineteenth century, teachers and linguists began to write about the need for
new approaches to language teaching, and through their pamphlets, books, speeches, and articles,
the foundation for more widespread pedagogical reforms was set up. This Reform Movement, as it
is known, laid the foundations for the development of new ways of teaching languages within the
Direct Method and raised controversies that have continued to the present day.

From the 1880s, an intellectual leadership gave greater credibility and acceptance to reformist ideas
thanks to linguists like Henry Sweet (1845-1912) in England, Wilhelm Vitor (1850-1918) in
Germany, and Paul Passy in France. Among the earliest goals of the association, we find the
leading role of phonetics within the teaching of modern languages, as there is a great emphasis on
the development of oral skills.

Sweet (1899) set forth principles for the development of teaching methods based on sound
methodological principles (an applied linguistic approach). For Vitor, whose name is directly
associated with a phonetic method, speech patterns were the fundamental elements of language,
stressing the value of training teachers in the new science of phonetics. In general the reformers
believed that grammar had to be taught inductively, translation avoided, and a language learning
based on hearing the language first, before seeing it in written forms.

These principles provided the theoretical foundations for a principled approach to language
teaching, one based on a scientific approach to the study of language. However, none of these
proposals assumed the status of a method. They reflect the beginnings of a new discipline of
applied linguistics: the naturalistic approach, which envisaged second language learning as in first
language acquisition.


22/ 49
As we have stated before, these early reformers, who included Henry Sweet of England, Wilhelm
Vitor of Germany, and Paul Passy of France, believed that language teaching should be based on
scientific knowledge about language, that it should begin with speaking and expand to other skills,
that words and sentences should be presented in context, that grammar should be taught inductively,
and that translation should, for the most part, be avoided.
In fact several attempts to make second language learning more like first language learning had
been made throughout the history of language teaching. For instance, if we trace back to the
sixteenth century, we find out that the Frenchman Montaigne described his own experience on
learning Latin for the first years of his life as a process where he was exclusively addressed in Latin
by a German tutor.

3.4.3.2. The behaviorist approach.
Since language is not an isolated phenomenon, we are committed to relate it to other aspects of
society, behavior and experience through the development of a theory between linguistics and other
fields of study, such as anthropology, sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, philosophical linguistics,
biological linguistics, and mathematical linguistics. Among all the interdisciplinary subjects, two of
them have strongly contributed to the development of the study of language teaching in the early to
mid-1900s, thus, sociology and psychology.
We shall deal with the latter for our purposes in this section. Psycholinguistics focuses on how
language is influenced by memory, attention, recall and constraints on perception, and the extent to
which language has a central role to play in the understanding of human development. In the field
of psychology, behaviorism had a great effect on language teaching as various scientists in the early
to mid-1900s did experiments with animals, trying to understand how animals behaved under
certain stimulus.
Theorists as Ivan Pavlov and Skinner, believed that languages were made up of a series of habits,
and that if learners could develop all these habits, they would speak the language well. Also, they
believed that a contrastive analysis of languages would be invaluable in teaching languages, and
from these theories arose the Audiolingual Method, examined in subsequent sections.

3.4.4. Main methods.
3.4.4.1. The Direct Method.
When the emphasis on oral skills was spread as well as natural language learning principles, they
both consolidated in what became known as the Direct Method, the first of the "natural methods,
both in Europe and in the United States. In general, it was quite successful in private language
schools, and difficult to implement in public secondary school education.


23/ 49
Among those who tried to apply natural principles to language classes in America were L. Sauveur
(1826-1907) and Maximiliam Berlitz who promoted the use of intensive oral interaction in the
target language. Saveurs method became known as the Natural Method and was seriously
considered in language teaching. In his book An Introduction to the Teaching of Living Languages
without Grammar or Dictionary (1874), Saveur described how their students learnt to speak after
a month on intensive oral work in class, avoiding the use of the mother tongue, even for grammar
explanations.
Berlitz, however, never used the term natural and named his method The Berlitz method
(1878), and it was known for being taught in private language schools, high-motivated clients, the
use of native-speaking teachers, and no translation under any circumstances. In spite of his success,
this method lacked a basis in applied linguistic theory, and failed to consider the practical realities
of the classroom.
In Europe, one of the best known representatives of language teaching was Gouin who, in 1880
attempted to build a methodology around observation of child language learning when publishing
L'art d'enseigner et d'tudier les langues. He developed this technique after a long struggle trying to
learn to speak and understand German through formal grammar-based methods. However, their
total failure and his turning to observations of how children learn a second language is one of the
most impressive personal testimonials in the recorded annals of language learning.
According to Richards & Rodgers (1992), although the Direct Method enjoyed popularity in
Europe, not everyone had embraced it enthusiastically. In the 1920s and 1930s, the British applied
linguist Henry Sweet and other linguists recognized its limitations. They argued for the
development of sound methodological principles as the basis for teaching techniques.


3.5. Structural grammar.
3.5.1. Definition.
Structural grammar emerges from the basis of descriptive grammar, and in fact, it is an extension of
it, from at least the early nineteen-thirties until the late nineteen-fifties. However, despite describing
syntax and utterances as in descriptive grammars, structural grammar goes beyond the level of word
structure straightforward into the sound system until getting to the notion of smallest phonological
unit, phoneme. This meant not only that the phonemes had to be found without reference o the
grammar (the morphemes), but that both had to be discovered without reference to semantics. Yet,
structural grammar is made up of a set of collected utterances made by native speakers of a
particular language (Palmer, 1971).
As its name suggests, the main thesis of this school was that language had a structure. But in itself
this word structure does not mean very much. In one sense all linguists are structuralist in that
they are looking for regularities, patterns or rules. In particular, it was associated with the


24/ 49
phoneme as the unit of phonology which is described as distinctive sounds of a language (the
sound system) and the morpheme as the unit of grammar. The essential sense, however, in which
the approach is structural is that the language is supposed to be actually composed of morphemes in
sequence, as a string of morphemes and similarly, though at a different level, of strings of
phonemes.
For many years, the most influential school of linguistics, described as structural, was mainly
associated with the name of several American linguists, such as Franz Boas, Edward Sapir, and
Leonard Bloomfield. These linguists attempted to record the dying languages of the Native
American Indians, and analyse them by studying the patterns of phonology as well as the syntax of
the utterances. But let us examine the framework for this analysis to take place.

3.5.2. Historical background.
The discoveries of the European historical-comparative linguists had a great influence on American
anthropologists on the nature of language. However, while European scholars were mainly
interested in dia chronic language studies, scholars on the American side of the Atlantic found
themselves engaged in synchronic investigations of particular languages, such as those of American
Indian cultures (LaPalombara, 1976).
This study on Indian cultures proved different from those of the Indo-European family, and
European language-analysis methods were almost worthless in this new situation. Assumptions
about word and phrase structure seemed simply not to apply. Thus, the method of analyzing the
sounds of a language, which was so valuable as a tool for the historical linguists, became a practical
necessity for the American anthropologists, who became the first American structuralists. Their
goal was to identiy the significant sounds (phonemes) of a language, and then, determine which
particular sound sequences made up the words of the language. Finally, languages sentences were
analyzed and recorded.
Therefore, structural grammar was based on syntactical analysis and attempted to describe language
grammar in terms of a structure of combining systems
3
. The most important contributions of
structural linguistics were in the areas of phonology (sound structure) and morphology (word
structure), particularly in the former. In fact, phonology came to be regarded as the indispensable
foundation upon which all of structural grammar analysis rested.

3
The structural description of English begin with an analysis of the sounds of the language in general, and then goes on to
isolate mutually exclusive groups of sounds which have semantic significance, the phonemes. From there, the grammar
description proceeds to the next highest level, the word structure (morphology) of English, which involves the isolation of
the smallest meaning-bearing units, the morphemes, which make up the words of the language. Finally, structural
grammar analyzes the phrase structure, or syntax, of English. At this level, the grammarian looks for the various ways in
which words can be combined to produce grammatical English sentences (LaPalombara, 1976).


25/ 49
On analysing Indian languages, not only did structural linguists discover that dialects had social
status connotations, but also that they were based on structured and complex system with
complicated grammar rules. Such discoveries modified attitudes about language use. Prior to these
developments, the notions of grammatical and ungrammatical were synonymous with correct and
incorrect within the teaching of Latin grammar in the schools of Europe. Yet, the structuralists were
then convinced by their own research that every language has its own complex grammar rules, and
that the task of the linguist is to discover what these rules are.

3.5.3. Main approaches.
3.5.3.1. In the United States: American Structural Linguistics.
But let us sum up some of the assumptions of the structural linguists. Fundamental to their approach
was the assumption that language is, first and above all, speech, reinforced by the discovery that
most Indian languages were still spoken only. Franz Boas, and later his student Edward Sapir,
established the basic framework. Boas was the first to formally propose that structural analysis
should be conducted on three successive levels, beginning with the sound level, proceeding next to
word structure analysis, and only then to the analysis of syntax or sentence structure. And Sapir
further developed the system proposed by Boas.
Leonard Bloomfield is the linguistic scholar usually considered the major developer of twentieth-
century structuralism in America. First, in his Introduction to the Study of Language (1914), and
later in Language (1933), he presented a detailed outline of the principles of structural language
analysis within the prescriptive grammar trend. His most important contribution was the
establishment of a binary principle, that is, at any level, from simplex to complex, a sentence can be
divided into a pair of units (immediate constituents), each of which applies its whole grammaticall
or structural meaning to the other.
In the 1940s, another important figure was Charles Fries. His most important contributions were
that (1) he devised a word-classification system based on the forms or structures of isolated words;
(2) he listed five structural grammatical devices which serve, in English, to signal grammatical
clues; and (3) he invented a system of grammatical analysis by means of test frames.
Among other important structuralists during the forties, fifties and beyond, we may include George
L. Trager and Henry Lee Smith on the phonological field, and many others who made their own
important contributions: Nelson Francis, Mary Haas, Archibald Hill, Zellig Harris, and Charles
Hock. Note that many of them are still in the linguistic picture.
Regarding the teaching of foreign languages, there were many studies on the teaching of second
languages. Particularly impressive were the results achieved by those structuralists who, during the
period of World War II, devised effective new methods for teaching second languages to adults. It
became necessary to teach the languages of the occupied countries as thoroughly and efficiently as
possible in the shortest possible time. Among those who achieved notable success in this area, the

26/ 49
name of Charles Fries especially stands out, the methods of second-language teaching he developed
having since been widely adopted by foreign language teachers in Americas public schools and
universities.
For nearly three decades, the structural linguists were in the forefront of language studies. During
this same period, the methods of empirical science which had originated with the nineteenth-century
comparativists extended to many other disciplines as well. Sociology, anthropology, psychology,
and political science, as well as linguistics, came to be referred to by many scholars as behavioral
sciences. In fact, the methods of the empirical behavioral sciences dominated the American
intellectual scene for almost thirty years.

3.5.3.2. In Europe: the Oral Approach and Situational Language Teaching.
This approach has its origins in the 1920s and 1930s and was developed from the 1930s to the
1960s by British applied linguists, as a more scientific foundation for an oral approach than the one
evidenced in the Direct Method. Its most prominent figures are the British applied linguists Harold
Palmer and A.S. Hornby, who developed the basis for a principled approach to methodology in
language teaching.
The terms Oral Approach or Situational Language Teaching are not commonly used today, but the
impact of the Oral Approach has been long lasting, and it has shaped the design of many widely
used textbooks and courses, including many still being used today. Therefore it is important to
understand the principles and practices of this oral approach which resulted from a systematic study
of the lexical and grammatical content of a language course.
This approach involved principles of selection, organization and presentation of the material based
on applied linguistic theory and practice. Thus, the role of vocabulary was seen as an essential
component of reading proficiency, and parallel to this syllabus design was a focus on the
grammatical content, viewed by Palmer as the underlying sentence patterns of the spoken language.
This classification of English sentence patterns was incorporated into the first dictionary for
students of English as a foreign language, and some grammatical guides which became a standard
reference source for textbook writers.
Palmer (1971) had emphasized the problems of grammar for the foreign learner. Much of his work
in Japan (from 1922 until World War II) was directed toward developing classroom procedures
suited to teaching basic grammatical patterns through an oral approach. His view of grammar was
very different from that of the Grammar-Translation Method. However, he saw grammar as the
underlying sentence patterns of spoken language.
The theory of language underlying Situational Language Teaching is referred to as a type of British
structuralism since speech was regarded as the basis of language, and structure was viewed as
being at the heart of speaking ability. However, the theory of learning underlying Situational

27/ 49
Language Teaching is a type of behaviorist habit-learning theory since it addresses primarily the
processes rather than the conditions of learning.
Like the Direct Method, Situational Language Teaching adopts an inductive approach to the
teaching of grammar. This is how child language learning is believed to take place, and the same
processes are thought to occur in second and foreign language learning, according to practitioners
of Situational Language Teaching.
This approach was the accepted British approach to English language teaching by the 1950s. One of
the most active proponents of the Oral Approach in the 1960s was the Australian George Pittman.
He and his colleagues were responsible for developing an influential set of teaching materials based
on the Situational Approach.

3.5.4. Main methods.
By the 1920s, in the United States foreign language specialists attempted to have the Direct Method
implemented in American schools and colleges, although they decided to move with caution.
Largely dependent on the teachers skill, and on native speakers, this new approach proved
counterproductive, since teachers were required to avoid using their native language, despite the
fact it would have been a more efficient route to grammar comprehension.
An American study, published as the Coleman Report (1923), argued that a more reasonable goal
for a foreign language course would be a reading knowledge of a foreign language, achieved
through the gradual introduction of words and grammatical structures in simple reading texts. The
main result of this recommendation was that reading became the goal of most foreign language
programs in the United States (Coleman, 1929). This emphasis on reading continued to characterize
foreign language teaching in the United States until World War II (Richards & Rodgers, 2001).
These linguists systematized the principles stated earlier by the Reform Movement and so laid the
foundations for what developed into the British approach to teaching English as a foreign language.
This would led to Audiolingualism in the United States and the Oral Approach or Situational
Language Teaching in Britain.

3.5.4.1. The Situational Method.

As said before, the principles and practices of this oral method which resulted from a systematic
study of the lexical and grammatical content of a language course, involved principles of selection,
organization and presentation of the material based on applied linguistic theory and practice. Thus,
language teaching begins with the spoken language, where material is taught orally before it is
presented in written form.


28/ 49
Similarly, new language points are introduced and practiced situationally, and in particular, items of
grammar are graded following the principle that simple forms should be taught before complex
ones. Also, reading and writing are introduced once a sufficient lexical and grammatical basis is
established.
In fact, the objectives of this method are to teach a practical command of the four basic skills of
language, goals it shares with most methods of language teaching. But the skills here are
approached through structure. Accuracy in both pronunciation and grammar is regarded as crucial,
and errors avoided at all costs.
The role of vocabulary was seen as an essential component of reading proficiency, and parallel to
this syllabus design was a focus on the grammatical content, viewed by Palmer as the underlying
sentence patterns of the spoken language. This classification of English sentence patterns was
incorporated into the first dictionary for students of English as a foreign language, and some
grammatical guides which became a standard reference source for textbook writers.
The Oral Approach was the accepted British approach to English language teaching by the 1950s,
but in the sixties, another active proposal from Australia and termed situational, entered this
approach developing an influential set of teaching materials based on the notion of situation,
linking structures to situations. Its main leader was George Pittman, and its main characteristics
were as follows: material is taught orally before it is presented in written form; introduced and
practiced situationally; and reading and writing are introduced only when sufficient lexical and
grammatical basis is established. The skills are approached through structure.
In the words of Richards & Roberts (1992), this theory that knowledge of structures must be linked
to situations has been supported by British linguists, giving a prominent place to meaning, context,
and situation. Prominent figures such as M.A.K. Halliday and Palmer emphasized the close
relationship between the structure of language and the context and situations in which language is
used.


3.5.4.2. The Audiolingual method.

As stated, the origins of this method trace back to the entry of the United States into World War II
since the government aimed to teach foreign languages to avoid Americans becoming isolated from
scientific advances in other countries. The National Defense Education Act (1958) provided funds
for the study and analysis of modern languages based on the earlier experience of the army
programs such as the so-called ASTP (Army Specialized Training Program). This program was
established for military personnel in 1942 in American universities, and its main objective was for
students to attain conversational proficiency in different foreign languages through significant drills.
This fact had a significant effect on language teaching in America, and in fact, new approaches on
language teaching were soon developed, and toward the end of the 1950s a new approach emerged
under the name of Audiolingualism (term coined by Professor Nelson Brooks in 1964). It is based

29/ 49
in structural linguistics (structuralism) and behavioristic psychology (Skinners behaviorism).
Therefore, it is primarily an oral approach to language teaching and there is little provision for
grammatical explanation or talking about the language.
The audio-lingual method aims at teaching the language skills in the order of listening, speaking,
reading, and writing, and is based on using drills for the formation of good language habits. Thus
students are given a stimulus, which they respond to. If their response is correct, it is rewarded, so
the habit will be formed; if it is incorrect, it is corrected, so that it will be suppressed. As Rivers
(1981) states, material is presented in spoken form, and the emphasis in the early years is on the
language as it is spoken in everyday situations.
It was a methodological innovation which combined structural linguistic theory, contrastive
analysis, aural-oral procedures, and behaviorist psychology. Therefore linguists such as Leonard
Bloomfield, developed training programs within an anthropological and linguistic tradition. The
best known of these programs was the informant method, based on a strict timetable (ten hours a
day during six days a week), fifteen hours drill with native speakers and almost thirty hours of
private study over nearly three six-week sessions. Statistics show that excellent results were often
achieved in small classes of mature and highly motivated students.


3.6. Transformational-Generative Grammar.
3.6.1. Definition.
In 1957 Noam Chomskys Syntactic Structures was published and it was this book that first
introduced to the world the most influential of all modern linguistic theories, that is,
transformational-generative grammar, or TG for short. The theory was ashtonishingly
revolutionary, but as with all revolutions, some of it had already been foreshadowed in earlier
works, as for instance, that of Chomskys own teacher, Zellig Harris (Palmer, 1971).

The name transformational-generative suggests that there are two aspects of the theory. The
grammar it provides is both transformational and generative. These two aspects are not logically
dependent upon each other, though the theory gains plausibility from the interaction of the two.
However, although the two aspects will be considered separately, we shall start by the most
fundamental and perhaps more revolutionary in linguistic terms, that of transformation.


3.6.1.1. The notion of transformational (1957).

Concerning the notion of transformation, we shall say that, as we have seen, the grammatical
theories which we have so far been discussing are mainly concerned with the analysis of sentences
in the sense that they must be divided into parts and that the functions of the various parts must be

30/ 49
stated. However, the notion of transformation allows the linguist to show how one sentence was
related to another in that their descriptions would be partly alike in meaning and partly different in
syntax (i.e. from active to passive, direct speech to reported speech, paraphrasing, writing
sentences in affirmative, negative and interrogative forms, etc). In fact, it is a theory that will not
merely allow us to replace one element by another or by a number of others, but also to take the
sentence and completely rearrange it.

Also, transformational grammar may resolve ambiguity in sentences. Thus, in a statement like The
shooting of the hunters was terrible, we cannot tell whether this means that the way in which the
hunters shot was terrible or that the fact that the hunters were shot was terrible. The transformation
involved here is nominalizing a sentence into a nominal phrase.


3.6.1.2. The notion of generative: indefinite, explicit, discovery vs. evaluation, competence vs.
Performance (1957).

Concerning the notion of generation, we shall say that it is the second characteristic of TG. It means
that grammar must generate all and only the grammatical sentences of a language (Palmer, 1971).
Note that it does not mean that a grammar will literally bring all these sentences into existence, but
that the grammar must be so designed that by following its rules and conventions, we could produce
all or any of the possible sentences of the language. To generate then is to predict what could be
sentences of the language or to specify precisely what are the possible sentences of a language.

Yet, there are two important aspects of a generative grammar both lacking, in some degree, in the
previous traditional and structuralist grammars. First, a generative grammar is not concerned with
any actual set of sentences of the language but with the possible set of sentences, since we can
produce sentences ad infinitum (indefinitely). We must remember, however, that to say that the
number of sentences may be infinite does not mean that the grammar itself is infinite. On the
contrary, grammar has a finite number of rules just as a finite set of figures (from 0 to 9) allows us
to generate an infinite set of numbers.

Secondly, to say that grammar is generative is to say that it is explicit, that is, it explicitly indicates
what are the possible sentences of the language. This, too, is a reaction to previous approaches since
a grammar that looks for patterns in a body of texts could fail to be explicit even if it allowed for
considerable extrapolation. Thus, in the Latin grammar (traditional grammar), the conjugations of
the verbs and the declensions of the nouns are set out in long paradigms (i.e. each conjugation
taking examples from one verb or noun).

There are two other important points that are related to the whole question of generation. The first
concerns the contrast between discovery and evaluation, and the second contrast between
competence and performance. First, the structuralists were largely concerned with the problem
of how to discover the phonemes, morphemes, etc. of the language, and under the requirement that
in the interest of empirical and scientific linguistics, we must begin with the observed data from


31/ 49
the sound system to the grammatical system, where the grammar depends on the phonology and the
phonology on the grammar, so neither precedes the other. Then, a linguistic theory has to provide a
set of evaluation procedures for evaluating all the possible descriptions, and a discussion on why
one solution was better than another.

The second important point concerns the distinction drawn between a native speakers competence
and his performance, which is central to Chomskys approach to the study of language. The former
is defined as the speakers internalised grammar of his language, his tacit or unconscious
knowledge of the system of rules and principles which underlies his capacity to speak and
understand the language of his speech community.

The latter is the speakers actual use of language on particular occasions, and includes not only
directly observable utterances, spoken and written, but also the speakers use of language to clarify
his thoughts, and other phenomena observable only by introspection, such as his ability to pass
judgements on the acceptability of utterances in terms of their sound, form and meaning, and his
awareness, perhaps partly subconscious, of the existence of various systematic structural and
semantic correspondences between ceretain utterance types, as reflected in his ability to form
questions corresponding to statements, passive analogues to active sentences, and so on.

It is important to note that competence is viewed not as a skill but as a system of knowledge which
underlies various skills. It is what the speaker must know in order to be able to perform (Horrocks,
1987). According to the theory, the native speaker of a language would have internalized a set of
rules which forms the basis of his ability to speak and to understand his language. It is the
knowledge of these rules that is the object of the linguists attention, not the actual sentences he
produces.

Yet, if we investigate what he says, this would be part of his performance. In fact, the distinction
between competence and performance is important in accounting for the fact that when we speak
we often do no speak grammatically, and also, for understanding the point about the infinite
number of sentences in a language.


3.6.1.3. Deep vs. Surface structures (1965).

Chomskys Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (1965) established the distinction between deep and
surface structures to refer to the base component of a standard theory (phrase structure rules and
lexicon) by which grammar generates sentences and assigns a syntactic structure to each. Chomsky
proposed, then, that the phrase structure rules (and lexicon) would generate the deep structures of
sentences (meaning) and that the rules of the transformational component of the syntax would place
them into surface structures (phonology).



32/ 49
As we have seen, Transformational-Generative grammar is based on cognitive factors and is made
up of a set of rules which make it clear which sequences of words and sentences are possible where
the rules of grammar are explicit, giving a structural description to each phrase. This woud allow
students to generate an unlimited number of new phrases in a language, provided that the necessary
vocabulary was known.


3.6.2. Historical background.

3.6.2.1. Syntactic Structures (1957).

Following LaPalombara (1976), in 1957, at the height of structuralisms influence on linguistic
studies, a young professor of Modern Languages at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology
published a book which challenged many of the basic beliefs of structuralism. The professor was A.
Noam Chomsky, and his book, a 108 page monograph entitled Syntactic Structures, was soon to
have a profound effect on language studies
4
since it was the first grammar to force psychologists to
reconsider their whole approach to the study of language behaviour, and so heralded the
psycholinguistic revolution.

This volume was a criticism to the structuralist approach to language study, as Chomsky considered
the entire structuralist theory to have been built upon wrong assumptions on their methods. He felt
their research should concern the logic of language regularities. In fact, Harris and his student
Noam Chomsky worked together to develop a phrase-structure grammar which, although modeled
along the rigorous structuralist lines, took some scholarly traditional notions.

Chomskys own thinking has been much more closely allied from the beginning with that of the
philosophical traditional grammarians from Plato through Humboldt. In his first work (1957), he
examined a number of generative grammars to demonstrate that no particular grammar thus far
formulated meets his standards of adequacy. Then, he presented a new formulation, phrase structure
rules plus transformational rules, which he believed to be more accurate and more useful.

Thus, he took from traditional grammarians the terminology like subject, object, complement,
singular and plural. From structuralists, he took the notions of phoneme, morpheme, and so on, and
his two major contributions (1957) were these: (1) he introduced a precise, mathematical method of
writing grammar rules; (2) he added a third level, the tranformation level to grammar theory. Hence,
his model generative grammar proposed a three-level, rule-based system: phonemic, morphemic,
and phrase-structure rules.




4
According to LaPalombara (1976), Chomskys book was based on his Ph.D. dissertation, written in 1955 at the
University of Pennsylvania under the direction of Zellig Harris.

33/ 49

3.6.2.2. Revisions of Syntactic Structures Model.

After a slow start, transformational-generative grammar finally took hold. Chomsky and others
worked at extending and refining the early theoretical model, since Chomsky admitted that there
were certain defects in his early model. Eventually, they arrived at enough modifications and
revisions on the original theory, and soon new inquiries showed that the linguist must rely on the
linguistic intuitions of native speakers (LaPalombara, 1976), and therefore, cognitive theories on
learning and language acquisition.

Chomskys answer was that, as a result of millions of years of evolution, human beings are
endowed genetically with a faculty of language acquisition, and therefore, the linguistic theory of
universal grammar is a model of this genetic endowment. It is because human infants enjoy the
benefit of this language-acquisition faculty that they are able to acquire a highly complex system of
knowledge long before they have reached intellectual maturity and on the basis of exposure to
primary data that is both limited in quantity and often degenerate in quality.

Chomsky, then, claimed that native speakers have knowledge of grammatical principles that simply
could not have been learned by each and every native speaker on the basis of generalisation from
samples of primary data. Structuralist claimed that language learning takes place by the processes of
observation, imitation, and cultural reinforcement. Chomsky, however, although he admitted the
importance of such environmental influence, said that exposure alone was simply not enough to
explain language acquisition processes.

Chomsky went further by saying that there exist in the mind innate structures which determine, in
advance of its acquisition, certain of the specific forms of the acquired knowledge. In other words, a
child must possess some kind of inherent linguistic theory that generally limits and specifies the
possible form of any human language. Chomskys theory, then, offers an explanation of how first
language acquisition is possible by attributing to the human infant as a species-specific biological
endowment a language acquisition device which makes available only a very small set of possible
primary data.


3.6.2.3. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (1965).


In the decade from 1955 to 1965 the foundations of generative grammar were laid and a complex
technical formalism was developed. In his book Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (1965), Chomsky
presented the standard theory of transformational generative grammar, whose main components
were the syntactic, the semantic and the phonological, since in 1957 he failed at dealing satisfactory
with the problem of meaning and meaning relationships.


34/ 49
Here, the syntactic component generates an infinite set of structures which are then related by the
semantic and phonological components to meaning and sound. Notice first of all that the rules of
the semantic and phonological components are taken to be interpretative of syntactic structures, and
that the rules of syntax are at the heart of the system (phrase-structure rules, transformational rules
and lexicon).

As stated before, Chomsky proposed, then, that the phrase structure rules (and lexicon) would
generate the deep structures of sentences (meaning) and that the rules of the transformational
component of the syntax would place them into surface structures (phonology). The deep structure
semantics approach may seem attractive, but if faces most of the criticisms of traditional notional
grammar. Deep grammar, in fact, is to give a name to semantics.

So, Chomsky conceived finally a revised model with a base component called, for the first time, the
deep structure, whose base component would include syntactical rules, semantic and phonological
information represented by feature matrixes of lexical items, and phrase markers. All sentences
would then be generated directly from the deep structure, or base, by means of various
transformation operations, to become actual sentences or surface structures (LaPalombara, 1976).

By the time he wrote Aspects of the Theory of Syntax some ten years later, he had come to regard
linguistics as a branch of cognitive psychology. To be sure, he still thought a grammar theory
should contain a syntax as the principal mediating component, but he was no longer willing to
ignore the influence of semantics.



3.6.3. Main approaches.

Transformational-Generative Grammar was a response to behaviouristic approaches to language
acquisition processes. Chomskys view of linguistics was an important contribution to the study of
the human mind, as a branch of cognitive psychology, apart from showing the weaknesses of
structural grammar. Regarding the teaching of languages, the psychological approach is related to
questions such as when and how children develop their ability to ask questions syntactically, or
when they learn the inflectional systems of their language.
Another interdisciplinary overlap, as Crystal (1985) states is psycholinguistics. It is a distinct area
of interest developed in the early sixties and in its early form covered from acoustic phonetics to
language pathology. Most of its researchers have been influenced by the development of generative
theory where the most important area is the investigation of the acquisition of language by children.
Linguists such as R. Ellis or Stephen Krashen and Tracy Terrells contribution show an approach
focusing on teaching communicative abilities and emphasizing the primacy of meaning when
second language acquisition is on study.

35/ 49
Therefore, we shall review the behaviorist approach since cognitive approaches are considered a
rejection to them. Also, we shall state the basis for naturalistic approaches which explain children
language acquisition processes as an introduction to next section on communicative grammar.


3.6.3.1. A rejection to behaviourist approaches.

Before the 1960s, views of how language was acquired started to change. In the behaviorist theory
of stimulus-response learning, particularly the operant conditioning model of Skinner, all learning is
regarded as the establishment of habits as the result of reinforcement or reward. According to this
theory, infants acquire their native-language habits through different stages (i.e. babbling, making
sounds which resemble appropriate words, and so on). As they acquire more of the syntactic and
morphological variations of the language, they create new combinations by generalization or
analogy, sometimes making mistakes (Rivers, 1981).

However, the behaviorist view of native-language learning was rejected by a number of theorist,
notably Chomsky and Lenneberg. They maintained that certain aspects of native-language learning
made it impossible to accept the habit-formation-by-reinforcement theory. They also argued that
child language learning does not appear to be a process of pure imitation since it seemed to involve
active selection from what is heard and personal construction of forms, according to the childs
developing system.

Lenneberg and Chomsky maintained that man has certain innate propensities for acquiring a
language, and for acquiring a language with a complicated grammar by some process of imitation
and generalization.


3.6.3.2. Psycholinguistics and cognitive approaches.

Chomsky theorized that the innate logical structure of the childs nervous system conforms with the
abstract universal categories and organization underlying language. Consequently, children identify
the basic syntactic system of the language to which they are attending, and mastery of the language
from identification and not from repetition and reinforcement. This theory of language learning,
then, was approached from a psycholinguistic and cognitive view to learning processes, such as
habit formation, induction, inferencing, hypothesis testing, and generalization.
Chomskys view of linguistics is another important contribution to the study of the human mind, as
a branch of cognitive psychology. Apart from showing the weaknesses of structural grammar,
Chomsky demonstrated that creativity and individual sentences formation were fundamental
characteristics of language, not part of the structural theories of language. His approach provides a
humanistic view of teaching where priority is given to interactive processes of communication.

36/ 49
He maintained that human beings come into the world with innate langauge-learning abilities in the
form of a langauge acquisition device which proceeds by hypothesis testing, that is, children make
hypotheses and compare these with their innate knowledge of possible grammars based on the
principles of universal grammar. Language use, then, is rule-governed behaviour which enables
speakers to create new utterances which conform to the rules they have internalized. This is the so-
called Chomskys creative aspect of language use.
Yet, in the development of generative theory, the most important area is the investigation of the
acquisition of language by children, and most researchers were influenced by this view. Hence, the
most prominent figures in this field are, among others, Stephen Krashen, and Tracy D. Terrell.

3.6.3.3. Natural approaches to second language acquisition research.
An approach to language teaching that constantly recurs through the centuries is the attempt to
achieve a language-learning situation which resembles as closely as possible the way children learn
their first language. Basically the natural approach involves setting up informal situations where
students communicate with each other and their teacher and, through communicating, acquire the
new language. This is an active, inductive approach.
Hence, Stephen D. Krashen developed a second language acquisition research as a source for
learning theories. He distinguishes two concepts here, acquisition and learning, where acquisition is
seen as the basic process involved in developing language proficiency. For him, it is the
unconscious development of the target language system as a result of using the language for real
communication. Learning would be related to the conscious representation of grammatical
knowledge and non spontaneous processes. He developed the Monitor Model on which the Natural
method was built.
Another theorist, Tracy D. Terrell is closely related to Krashen, since they both wrote a book named
The Natural Approach (1983), and their theories emphasize the nature of the human and physical
context in which language learning takes place. Their learning theory is supported by three main
principles. Firstly, they claim that comprehension precedes production (commonly known as
input); secondly, they state that production may emerge in stages and students are not forced to
speak before they are ready; and thirdly the fact that the course syllabus consists of communicative
goals, thus classroom activities are organized, by topic, not grammar (Krashen & Terrell, 1983).

3.6.4. Main methods.
The period from the 1970s through the 1980s witnessed a major paradigm shift in language
teaching. The quest for alternatives to grammar-based approaches and methods led in several
different directions, and therefore, different classroom methods. Whereas Audiolingualism and
Situational Language Teaching were mainstream methods developed by linguists and applied
linguists, the methods described in this section were either developed outside of mainstream


37/ 49
language teaching or represent an application in language teaching of educational principles
developed elsewhere.
The former case is represented by such innovative methods of the 1970s as Total Physical
Response, Silent Way, Counseling Learning, and Suggestopedia. A different case is represented by
the Natural Method that, although proposed in the 1970s, emerged within mainstream education in
the nineteenth century and have been later applied and extended to second and foreign language
teaching, with some changes on terminology. Let us examine all this methods starting by the
Natural Method.

3.6.4.1. The Natural Method: Krashens Monitor Model.
Sweet, in 1899, in discussing the claims for the natural method of his day, said that it almost
necessarily implied a residence in the country where the language is spoken. Even the, he
advocated systematic study to offset the problems of what one has misheard or confused. Studies by
Krashen and Seliger of foreign students learning English in the United States support Sweets
contention. They have found that formal instruction plays an important role in language proficiency.
In second-language acquisition Krashens Monitor Model (1981) has enjoyed considerable
prominence as far as it is a relevant point concerning the differences between what is learned in an
informal environment where the language is spoken and what is learned through systematic study.
Krashen proposed (1983) that the first (learned informally in context) would be termed acquisition
whereas the second (learned through systematic study) would be termed learning.
He believed acquisition takes place when linguistic abilities are internalized naturally in informal
situations, and that language learning, on the other hand, is a conscious process taken out of a
language learning situation or a self study program. The present approach to language learning,
then, is to be set in the language laboratory, where students study the language systematically on
their own and class sessions are devoted entirely to natural interaction, providing opportunities to
create new utterances.
Thus, the Monitor Model consists of five central hypotheses: (1) the acquisition vs learning
hypothesis, already discussed; (2) the natural order hypothesis, whereby students are said to follow
a more or less invariant order in the acquisition process; (3) the Monit or hypothesis, whereby the
monitor is the device learners use for their language performance; (4) the input hypothesis, whereby
acquisition takes place as a result of the learner having understood input that is a little beyond the
current level of his competence; and (5) the affective filter hypothesis, whereby the filter controls
how much input the learner comes into contact with, and how much input is converted into intake
(Ellis, 1985).



38/ 49
3.6.4.2. Total Physical Response.
Total Physical Response (1977) centers on both processes and conditions aspects of learning. Thus,
coordinating language production with body movement and physical actions is believed to provide
the conditions for success in language learning. Total Physical Response is linked to several
traditions, such as psychology, learning theory, and humanistic pedagogy. This method is built
around the combination of speech and action and was developed by James Asher, a professor of
psychology.
For him, including movements within the linguistic production reduces learner stress, creating a
positive mood which facilitates learning. This emphasis on comprehension and the use of physical
actions to teach a foreign language is not new. In the nineteenth century, Gouin acknowledged a
situationally based teaching strategy in which action verbs served as a basis for practicing new
language items.
This method owes much to structuralist or grammar-based views of language as most of vocabulary
items and grammatical structures are learned through an instructor. Asher still sees a stimulus-
response view as reminiscences of the views of behavioral psychologists, directed to right-brain
learning. The main goal is to teach oral proficiency at a beginning level through the use of action-
based drills in the imperative form.
This method is updated with references to more recent psychological theories and supported by
prominent theorists as Krashen because of its emphasis on the role of comprehension in second
language acquisition. However, Asher himself, points out the need for this method to be used in
association with other methods to be fully successful.

3.6.4.3. The Silent Way.
The Silent Way method, developed by Caleb Gattegno, was also built on a conscious control of
learning to heighten learning potential. Caleb Gattegno introduced this classroom technique wherein
the teacher remains silent while pupils output the language through simulated experiences using
tokens and picture charts as central elements. For instance, a color-coded phonics (sound) chart
called a fidel, with both vowel and consonant clusters on it, is projected onto a screen to be used
simultaneously with a pointer, thus permitting the pupil to output continually the target language in
a sequence of phonemes.
This method works effectively to promote small group discussion, where students are encouraged to
produce as much language as possible and to self-correct their pronunciation errors through manual
gesticulation on the part of the instructor. The greatest strength of this method lies in its ability to
draw students out orally, while the teacher listens. This inner criteria allow learners to monitor and
self-correct their own production. It is here where this method differs notably from other ways of
language learning.



39/ 49
3.6.4.4. Counseling Learning.
The Counseling-Learning (1972), focused mainly on creating the conditions necessary for
successful learning, such as a good atmosphere of the classroom, where intimacy and security are a
crucial factor together for students when producing language. Also labelled, Community Language
Learning, as the name indicates, this method follows a humanistic approach which was supported
by Charles A. Curran, a specialist in counseling and a professor of psychology at Chicago
University.
His method redefines the roles of the teacher (counselor) and learners (the clients) in the language
classroom. He developed a holistic approach to language learning, since human learning is both
cognitive and affective. For him, learning takes place in a communicative situation where teachers
and learners are involved in an interaction. One of its main tenets is for the student to develop his
relationship with the teacher.
This process is divided into five stages and compared to the ontogenetic development of the child.
Thus, feelings of security are established; achievement of independence from the teacher; the
learner starts speaking independently; a sense of criticism is developed; and finally, the learner
improves style and knowledge of linguistic appropriateness.
Curran wrote little about his theory which was to be developed by his student, La Forge. He built a
theory on basic sound and grammatical patterns which started with criteria for sound features, the
sentence, and abstract models of language in order to construct a basic grammar of the foreign
language. Since these humanistic technique of counseling students engage the whole person,
including the emotions and feelings (affective part) as well as linguistic knowledge and behavioral
skills, this method has been linked to bilingual and adult education programs.

3.6.4.5. Suggestopedia.
In the 1980s and 1990s, an extremely esoteric method was developed by a Bulgarian psychiatrist-
educator called Georgi Lozanov. The most outstanding features of this mystical method are,
according to Rivers (1981), its arcane terminology and neologisms, and secondly, the arrangement
of the classroom to create an optimal atmosphere to learning, by means of decoration, furniture, the
authoritative behavior of the teacher and specially, through the use of music. Therapy theories are
the reason of using music in the classroom as Lozanov calls upon in his use to relax learners as well
as to structure, pace, and punctuate the presentation of linguistic material.
Lozanov acknowledges following a tradition on yoga and Soviet psychology, borrowing techniques
for altering states of consciousness and concentration, and the use of rhythmic breathing. In fact,
teachers are trained in a special way to read dialogues, using voice quality, intonation, and timing.
Lozanov also claims that his method works equally well whether or not students spend time on
outside study and promises success to the academically gifted and ungifted alike.


40/ 49
In the own words of Lozanov (1978), Suggestopedia prepares students for success by means of
yoga, hypnosis, biofeeback or experimental science. Its main features such as scholarly citations,
terminological jargon, and experimental data have received both support and criticisms. However,
Suggestopedia is acknowledged to appear effective and harmonize with other successful techniques
in language teaching methodology.



3.7. Communicative grammar.

3.7.1. Definition.

Communicative grammars, that is, grammars based on the usage of language and communication,
are also labelled as functional, competence, linguistic, pedagogical, situational and teaching
grammars. This type of grammar draws attention to those rules of grammar which will help students
learn and understand a language by means of language use and communication. In linguistic terms,
we are dealing with a mixture of prescriptive and descriptive grammar.

As seen before, the period from the 1970s to the 1980s was highly productive concerning different
alternatives to grammar-based approaches and methods, where language teaching embraced a
growing interest in communicative approaches. The communicative movement sought to move the
focus away from grammar as the core component of language, to a different view of language, of
language learning, of teachers, and of learners, one that focused on language as communication and
on making the classroom an environment for authentic communication.

Yet, in this final chapter, we shall reflect on the history of approaches and methods in the recent
history of language teaching which have had, more or less, a lasting impact on language acquisition
on what has been defined as the post-methods era.



3.7.2. Historical background.

This section provides a historical framework for approaches and methods up to the present time and
describe some of the directions mainstream language teaching has followed since the emergence of
communicative methodologies in the 1980s. To start with, it is relevant to say that Communicative
Language Teaching (CLT) marked the beginning of a major paradigm shift within language
teaching in the twentieth century, one whose ramifications continue to be felt today (Richards &
Rodgers, 2001).
Communicative Language Teaching has its origins in two sources. First, the changes in the British
and American linguistic theory in the mid-late sixties and secondly, changes in the educational
realities in Europe. Teaching traditions until then, such as Situational Language Teaching in Britain

41/ 49
and Audiolingualism in the United States started to be questioned by applied linguists who saw the
need to focus in language teaching on communicative proficiency rather than on mere mastery of
structures.
First, changes in the British and American linguistic theory in the mid-late sixties was partly a
response to the sorts of criticisms the prominent American linguist Noam Chomsky had leveled at
structural linguistic theory in his book Syntactic Structures (1957). He demonstrated that
structuralism was unable to account for the fundamental characteristics of language, thus the
creativity and uniqueness of individual sentences.
British applied linguists, on the other hand, emphasized another fundamental dimension of language
that was inadequately addressed in approaches to language teaching at that time: the functional and
communicative potential of language. Among some of the scholars who followed this view of
language we shall include Christopher Candlin and Henry Widdowson, who drew on the work of
British functional linguists (i.e. John Firth, M.A.K. Halliday), American work in sociolinguistics
(i.e. Dell Hymes, John Gumperz, and William Labov), as well as work in philosophy (i.e. John
Austin and John Searle).
Secondly, different approaches to foreign language teaching emerged from changes in the
educational realities in Europe. The role of the European Common Market and the Council of
Europe had a significant impact on the development of functional and communicative language
teaching since there was an increasing need to teach adults the major languages of the European
Common Market.
Education was one of the Council of Europes major areas of activity since it envisaged a regional
organization for cultural and educational cooperation. Then, it sponsored international conferences
on language teaching, published books about language teaching, and was active in promoting the
formation of the International Association of Applied Linguistics in order to develop alternative
methods of language teaching.
In the early 1970s, a group of experts began to investigate the possibility of developing language
courses on a unit-credit system, and in 1971, a preliminary document in which learning tasks were
broken down into portion of units was launched into the market by a British linguist, D.A.
Wilkins. He proposed a functional or communicative definition of language in which language was
described in terms of communicative approaches rather than through traditional concepts of
grammar and vocabulary.
This approach attempted to demonstrate the systems of meanings that a language learner needs to
understand and express wit hin two types: notional categories (time, sequence, quantity or
frequency) and categories of communicative function (requests, offers, complaints ). The rapid
application of these ideas by textbook writers and its acceptance by teaching specialists gave
prominence to what became the Communicative Approach or simply Communicative Language
Teaching.
Later on, in 1977 Tracy Terrell proposed a new philosophy of language teaching called the Natural
Approach based on comprehensible and meaningful practice activities rather than production of

42/ 49
grammatically perfect utterances and sentences. Other more general instructional and interactive
approaches were the Cooperative Language Learning approach, based on the maximum use of
cooperative activities involving pairs and small groups of learners in the classroom; Content-Based
approaches in language teaching offered unlimited opportunities for teachers to match students
interests and needs with interesting and meaninful content; and more recently, Task-Based
Language Teaching approaches with a reassessment of the role of formal grammar instruction in
language
Now, once we have briefly offered a historical overview of the development of communicative
approaches, we shall consider how the general principles of Communicative Language Teaching are
molded into quite diverse teaching practices, but no specific methods. The role of grammar has
considerably changed from those traditional views of language teaching to these days, where it is
considered from a content-based approach, although there is no evidence that the type of grammar-
focused teaching activities used in many language classrooms reflects the cognitive learning
processes employed in naturalistic language learning situations outside the classroom.


3.7.3. Main approaches.

3.7.3.1. The Communicative Language Teaching Approach.

The Communicative Approach in language teaching starts from a theory of language as
communication, and has been approached from different perspectives, thus notional, functional,
and situational. The origins of Communicative Language Teaching (CLT) trace back to the late
1960s, when some changes in British language teaching tradition were taking place. Until then,
Situational Language Teaching represented the major British approach since language was taught
by practicing basic structures in meaningful situation-based activities. But, when the linguistic
theory underlying Audiolingualism was rejected in the United States in the mid-1960s, British
applied linguists began to do the same with Situational Language Teaching underlying theory.

In the mid-1960s, there were a variety of theoretical challenges to the audio-lingual method.
Linguists such as Hymes, the American linguist Noam Chomsky, Halliday, and Labov challenged
previous assumptions about language structure and language learning, taking the position that
language is creative (not memorized by repetition and imitation) and rule governed (not based on
habits).

For Hymes (1972), the goal of language teaching is to develop a communicative competence, that
is, the knowledge and ability a learner needs to be communicatively competent in a speech
community. However, for Chomsky, the focus of linguistic theory was to characterize the abstract
abilities speakers possess that enable them to produce grammatically correct sentences in a
language. Yet, in Hymess view, a person who acquires communicative competence acquires both
knowledge and ability for language use, as cognitive and behavioral approaches.


43/ 49
This theory of language knowledge offers a much comprehensive view than Chomskys view of
competence, which deals primarily with abstract grammatical knowledge. Another linguistic theory
of communication is a functional account of language use, in which Halliday (1970) elaborated a
functional theory of the functions of language, which complements Hymess view of
communicative competence. Moreover, Henry Widdowson in his book Teaching Language as
Communication (1978) presented a view of the relationship between linguistic systems and their
communicative values in text and discourse. Grammar, then, was to be part of grammatical
competence.

A more pedagogically influential analysis of communicative competence is found in Canale and
Swain (1980), who identified four dimensions of communicative competence: grammatical,
sociolinguistic, discourse and strategic competence. Grammatical competence makes reference to
what Chomsky calls linguistic competence and what Hymes intends by what is formally possible,
that is, the domain of grammatical and lexical capacity (Richards & Rodgers, 2001).

This communicative view is considered an approach rather than a method which provides a
humanistic approach to teaching where interactive processes of communication receive priority. Its
rapid adoption and implementation resulted from a strong support of leading British applied
linguists and language specialist, as well as institutions, such as the British Council. However, some
of the claims are still being looked at more critically as this approach raises important issues for
teacher training, materials development, and testing and evaluation (Richards & Rodgers 1992).
Today, Communicative Language Teaching thus continues in its classical form, as is seen in the
long run of course books and other teaching resources based on the principles of CLT. In addition,
it has influenced many other language teaching approaches and methods that subscribe to a similar
philosophy of language teaching. Those approaches are examines as follows.

3.7.3.2. Other approaches: direct developments of Communicative Language Teaching.
As stated before, the Natural Approach emerged in the late 1970s, and belongs to a tradition of
language where teaching methods are based on observation and interpretation of how learners
acquire both first and second languages in nonformal settings. Such methods, then, rejected the
formal (grammatical) organization of language as a prerequisite to teaching, and saw
communication as the primary function of language.
The Natural Approach grew out of Terrells experiences teaching Spanish in California and it is
well-known that he joined forces with Stephen Krashen by writing a book called The Natural
Approach (1983). In this work, they combined statement of both principles and practices on their
approach to language teaching, which was defined as natural, psychological, phonetic, new, reform,
direct, analytic, imitative and so forth. Anyway, we must differentiate between The Natural
Approach and the older Natural Method from traditional grammars.


44/ 49
In the 1980s, from more general instructional and interactive views, we find the Cooperative
Language Learning approach, based on an interactive view of language structure. It was heavily
based on the theoretical work of developmental psychologists, such as Jean Piaget (1965) and Lev
Vygotsky (1962), both of whom stress the central role of social interaction in learning.
This approach has its origins in proposals for peer-tutoring and peer-monitoring that trace back
hundreds of years and longer. The early twentieth.century American educator John Dewey
promoted the idea of cooperation into regular classrooms, and in the 1960s and 1970s it was further
developed in the United States as a response to forced integration of public schools. Yet, it is
viewed as a learner-centred approach to teaching which offers advantages over teacher-fronted
methods.
It is an approach to teaching that makes maximum use of cooperative activities involving pairs and
small groups of learners in the classroom, where grammar is seen as a teaching content in class,
together with the four skills, pronunciation, and vocabulary. However, although it has been
extensively researched and evaluated, it was never conducted in Second Language Learning
classrooms. No methods are advocated to it.
Since the 1980s, Content-Based approaches in language teaching have been widely used in a
variety of different settings, such as English for Specific Purposes, immersion programs for
immigrants, university foreign language programs and also, in business and vocational purposes.
However, since Content-Based Instruction refers to an approach rather than a method, no specific
techniques are associated to it. Yet, this approach offers unlimited opportunities for teachers to
match students interests and needs with interesting and meaninful content.
The earliest proponent of this appoach was Saint Augustine, who attempted to give priority to
meaning in language teaching in order to learn or communicate through language by means of
teaching content (i.e. the use of objects, pictures, miming, imitiation, and so on). More recently, in
the mid-1970s Language across the Curriculum emerged as a proposal for native-language
education from a British governmental commission.
The report of the comission recommended a focus on reading and writing in all subject areas in the
curriculum, and not merely in the subject called language arts. Language skills should be taught in
the content subjects. This report influenced American education, but did not have the expecte
impact. However, since it is widely used as the basis for several kinds of successful language
programs, it is expected to lead curricular approaches in language teaching, as for instance, the
current Spanish curriculum.
In recent years, vocabulary has been considered to play a more central role in second language
learning than was traditionally assumed. Vocabulary includes lexical phrases, sentence stems,
prefabricated routines, and collocations, so the concept of vocabulary is seen as significant units of
linguistic lexical analysis and language pedagogy. Hence, the development of approaches based on
tasks, that is, Task-Based Language Teaching approaches.


45/ 49
Tasks, then, were proposed as useful vehicles for language teaching, considered as potential
building blocks of second language acquisition, whose research focused on the strategies and
cognitive processes employed by second language learners. This research has suggested a
reassessment of the role of formal grammar instruction in language teaching.


3.7.4. Main methods.

As stated before, Communicative Language Teaching approaches and its further developments did
not reach the status of method at any time. All of them and, in particular, CLT have been
extensively evaluated and examined with productive research findings. However, little of this
research was turned into methods into the classroom setting, although CLT procedures often require
teachers to acquire less teacher-centered classroom management skills. The classroom is regarded
as the setting for communication and communicative activities.

There are numerous textbooks designed to direct and suppor Communicative Language Teaching.
Yet, some of them are written around a largely structural syllabus, where grammar is presented as a
task analysis for thematic development. Learners, then, learn language through using it to
communicate. CLT and its further developments are best considered approaches rather than
methods, since they refer to a diverse set of principles that reflect a communicative view of
language and language learning, which can be used to support a wide variety of classroom
procedures.

The notion of methods was widely criticized in the 1990s. By the end of the twentieth century,
mainstream language teaching no longer regarded methods as the key factor in accounting for
success or failure in language teaching. Some spoke of the death of methods and approaches and the
term post-methods era was sometimes used (Richards & Rodgers, 2001).




4. PRESENT-DAY DIRECTIONS REGARDING COMMUNICATIVE GRAMMAR.

4.1. Educational implications of communicative grammar.

Nowadays, grammatical competence is approached from an eclectic point of view, and currently
applied to language teaching as part of our present educational system, L.O.G.S.E., based on
communicative methods. It is the current perspective on communicative approaches which drag our
attention to grammar since it is one of the main sub-competences in students linguistic competence
(the grammatical one), and helps establish a link between structure and meaning when
communicating.


46/ 49
As stated before, in recent years, grammar and lexical items have been considered to play a more
central role in second language learning than was traditionally assumed, since it helps students
handle lexical phrases, sentence stems, prefabricated routines, and collocations. So far, the notion of
grammar is envisaged in task-based teaching approaches as an appropriate classroom activity in
order to derive output from input.
Yet, the development of approaches based on tasks (i.e. Task-Based Language Teaching) has
favoured the introduction of grammar structures in curriculum plans as part of a broader set of
educational planning decisions. In fact, current curriculums for E.S.O. and Bachillerato envisage the
teaching of grammar through blocks of contents (B.O.E., 2002), as for instance, Present Simple,
Passive Voice, Modal verbs, and so on.
Tasks, then, are considered as useful vehicles for practicing these contents, considered as potential
building blocks of second language acquisition, whose research focused on the strategies and
cognitive processes employed by second language learners. This research has suggested a
reassessment of the role of formal grammar instruction in language teaching.

So far, we have also reviewed the role of communicative grammar within our current educational
system, in order to present future directions regarding communicative grammar.


4.2. Future directions regarding communicative grammar.

According to Hedge (2000), since the introduction of communicative approaches, the ability to
communicate effectively in English has become one of the main goals in European Language
Teaching. The Council of Europe (1998), in response to the need for international co-operation and
professional mobility among European countries, has recently published a document, Modern
languages: Learning, Teaching, Assessment. A Common European Framework of reference, in
which the acquisition of communicative competence, and for our present purposes, of
communicative grammar.

Similarly, the Spanish Educational System states (B.O.E., 2002) that there is a need for learning a
foreign language in order to communicate with other European countries, and a need for
emphasizing the role of a foreign language which gets relevance as a multilingual and multicultural
identity. Within this context, getting a proficiency level in a foreign language implies educational
and professional reasons which justify the presence of foreign languages in the curricula at different
educational levels.
The European Council (1998) and, in particular, the Spanish Educational System within the
framework of the Educational Reform, establishes a common reference framework for the teaching
of foreign languages, and claims for a progressive development of communicative competence in a
specific language. Students, then, are intended to be able to carry out several communication tasks
with specific communicative goals within specific contexts. It is here that we find the notion of


47/ 49
communicative grammar within the grammatical competence, since these communication tasks
involve the knowledge of grammar and the ability to use it in specific contexts.
Future directions in second language teaching reflect current trends of language curriculum
development at the level of cognitive strategies, grammar, phonetics or technological innovative
methods. The Internet Age anticipates the development of teaching and learning in instructional
settings by means of an on-line collaboration system, perhaps via on-line computer networks or
other technological resources by means of functional approaches to grammar.
The aim is for students to acquire a communicative competence, where their knowledge and ability
in the foreign language will help them get the meaning of a sentence, even if the different functions
of language make it difficult. Finally, students are provided with strategies and techniques to
overcome their communicative problems in an attempt to make communication as real as possible
in a formal setting, by means of chat, e-mail, and keeping in contact with friends in the European
framework.





5. CONCLUSION.

As we have seen, this study has aimed to serve as the core of a survey on the concept of grammar,
and in particular on its relationship to language and language learning. We have also examined
history within the linguistic scene from its earliest beginnings to the current situation, or in other
words, from normative grammar to a more communicative grammar system based on language use.

The conclusion here is not the old clich, but rather the old saying of Theres nothing new under
the sun, since tracing back in history proves successful in establishing amazing similarities
between old theories of language and grammar, such as the Old Norse First Grammatical Treatise
from the mid-twelfth century with nineteenth and twentieth century historical and structural
linguists.

We have also examined the concept of grammar and language learning in an attempt to offer a
relevant background to discuss the the phenomenon of language and its nexus between
philosophical theories on human nature and human use of language. Within a broad historical
overview, different cultures have been examined in order to offer a relevant chronological
framework for the section which describes the main grammar systems ranging from traditional to
present-day perspectives.







48/ 49

6. BIBLIOGRAPHY.

- Algeo, J. and T. Pyles. 1982. The origins and development of the English language. Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich, Inc.

- Bauer, L., and P. Trudgill. 1998. Language Myths. Penguin.

- Baugh, A. & Cable, T. 1993. A History of the English Language. Prentice-Hall Editions.

- B.O.E. RD N 112/2002, de 13 de septiembre por el que se establece el currculo de la Educacin
Secundaria Obligatoria/Bachillerato en la Comunidad Autnoma de la Regin de Murcia.

- Chomsky, N. 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

- Council of Europe (1998) Modern Languages: Learning, Teaching, Assessment. A Common
European Framework of reference.

- Crystal, D. 1985. Linguistics. Harmondsworth, England. Penguin Books.

-Ellis, R. 1985. Understanding Second Language Acquisition. Oxford University Press.

-Halliday, M.A.K. 1985. An Introduction to Functional Grammar. London: Edward Arnold.

- Hedge Tricia (2000) Teaching and Learning in the Language Classroom (OUP).

- Horrocks, Geoffrey. 1987. Generative Grammar. Longman Group UK Limited.

- Howatt, A.P.R. 1984. A History of English Language Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

- Hymes, D. 1972. On communicative competence. In J. B. Pride and J. Holmes (eds.),
Sociolinguistics, pp. 269-93. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Press.

- Jespersen, O. 1933. Essentials of English Grammar. George Allen & Unwin Ltd. London.

- Kolln, M., and R. Funk. 1999. Understanding English Grammar. Allyn & Bacon (5
th
ed.):
Needham Heights, New Jersey.

- Krashen, S., and T. Terrell. 1983. The Natural Approach: Language Acquisition in the Classroom
(1983). Oxford: Pergamon.

- LaPalombara, Lyda E. 1976. An introduction to grammar: Traditional, Structural,
Transformational. Winthrop Publishers, Inc. Massachusetts.


49/ 49
- Newmeyer, Frederick J. 1983. Grammatical Theory. Its Limits and Its Possibilities. The
University of Chicago Press. Chicago.

- Palmer, F. 1971. Grammar. Penguin Books Ltd, Harmondsworth.

- Quirk, R & S. Greenbaum. 1973. A University Grammar of English. Longman.

- Richards, J., & Rodgers, T. (2001). Approaches and methods in language Teaching (2nd ed.).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

- Riley, K., and F. Parker. 1998. English Grammar: Prescriptive, Descriptive, Generative,
Performance. Allyn & Bacon: Needham Heights, New Jersey.

- Rivers, W. 1981. Teaching Foreign-Language Skills. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

- Rutherford, William E. 1987. Second Language Grammar: Learning and Teaching. Longman
Group UK Limited.

- van Ek, J.A., and J.L.M. Trim, 2001. Vantage. Council of Europe. Cambridge University Press.

- Widdowson, H.G. 1978. Teaching Language as Communication. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.

- White, L. 1989. Universal Grammar and Second Language Acquisition. John Benjamins
Publishing Company. Amsterdam/Philadelphia.

You might also like