You are on page 1of 26

Clarifying conditions and decision points

for mediational type inferences in


Organizational Behavior
y
JOHN E. MATHIEU
*
AND SCOTT R. TAYLOR
University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut, U.S.A.
Summary Although mediational designs and analyses are quite popular in Organizational Behavior
research, there is much confusion surrounding the basis of causal inferences. We review
theoretical, research design, and construct validity issues that are important for drawing
inferences frommediational analyses. We then distinguish between indirect effects, and partial
and full mediational hypotheses and outline decision points for drawing inferences of each
type. An empirical illustration is provided using structural equation modeling (SEM)
techniques, and we discuss extensions and directions for future research. Copyright #
2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Introduction
Over 20 years ago, Baron and Kenny (1986) and James and Brett (1984) published papers that have had
a profound inuence on Organizational Behavior research and theory. Those authors advanced a
theoretical foundation and analytic guidelines for drawing mediational inferencestheories, methods,
and analyses that elucidate the underlying mechanisms linking antecedents and their consequences. At
issue in this approach are research questions that seek to better understand how some antecedent (X)
variable inuences some criterion (Y) variable, as transmitted through some mediating (M) variable. In
this sense, mediators are explanatory variables that provide substantive interpretations of the
underlying nature of an X!Y relationship.
Mediational designs have become ubiquitous in the organizational literature. Wood, Goodman,
Cook, and Beckman (in press) reviewed ve leading management journals over the years 19812005
and identied 381 studies that tested mediational relationships. Of these, over 60 per cent of the studies
followed prescriptions offered by Baron and Kenny (1986) or James and Brett (1984). However, the
state of the art in mediational analysis is far from consistent. The fact that mediational designs have
developed in different disciplines has only exacerbated the situation (Alwin & Hauser, 1975; Baron &
Kenny, 1986; Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004; Holmbeck, 1997; James & Brett, 1984). Indeed,
Journal of Organizational Behavior
J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
Published online 7 September 2006 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/job.406
* Correspondence to: John E. Mathieu, University of Connecticut, 2100 Hillside road, Unit 1041, Storrs, CT 06269-1041, U.S.A.
E-mail: JMAthieu@business.uconn.edu
y
This article was published online on 7 September 2006. An error was subsequently identied and corrected by an Erratumnotice
that was published online only on 13 October 2006; DOI: 10.1002/job.426. This printed version incorporates the amendments
identied by the Erratum notice.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Received 29 April 2006
Accepted 5 May 2006
MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, and Sheets (2002) noted that Reecting their diverse
disciplinary origins, the procedures [for testing mediating variables] vary in their conceptual basis, the
null hypothesis being tested, their assumptions, and statistical methods of estimation (p. 84).
Mediational designs come in a variety of forms that differ in terms of the nature of the variables
involved in this X!M!Y chain. For example, Judd and Kenny (1981) discussed the inuence of
psychological treatments on individuals behavior as mediated by their knowledge. Saks (1995) studied
the inuence of training on newcomer adjustments, as mediated by their post-training self-efcacy.
Mayer and Gavin (2005) considered how employees trust in their management inuenced their in-role
performance and organizational citizenship behaviors, as mediated by their ability to focus their
attention on work activities. Chen, Gully, Whiteman, and Kilcullen (2000) tested the inuence of
individuals psychological traits on their behavior, as mediated by their psychological states.
Claessens, Eerde, Rutte, and Roe (2004) considered the inuence of individuals planning behavior and
work characteristics on their work outcomes such as strain, job satisfaction, and performance, as
mediated by their perceived control of time. In all instances, mediational models advance an
X!M!Y causal sequence, and seek to illustrate the mechanisms through which X and Yare related.
However, there are important nuances in such designs that are often not appreciated, such as whether a
mediator variable partially or full accounts for an X!Y relationship, or whether it merely serves as a
linking mechanism between variables. This important distinction along with other aspects of
mediational designs constitutes our focus.
Given the diversity of approaches and statistical techniques that currently exist for testing mediation,
our aims for this paper are to: (1) revisit the research design and measurement preconditions that must
be met in order for tests of mediational relations to be meaningful; (2) review denitions of mediators
and related concepts, and in so doing distinguish between indirect effects, and partial and full
mediational models; (3) distinguish the different statistical tests and decision points that apply,
depending on what type of relationship is hypothesized; and (4) provide an empirical example that
illustrates such differences. We conclude with a discussion of directions for future research and theory
incorporating these distinctions. We note at the onset that many of the points we make below have been
voiced previously. However, a quick perusal of the literature will reveal that some conventional bits of
wisdom have been routinely ignored by many authors, and there remains a lack of consensus regarding
how mediational hypotheses should be framed and tested (Wood et al., in press). Our hope is that this
presentation can serve as a guide for those wishing to advance and to test mediational type relations.
Preconditions for Mediation Tests
The basic mediational design tests whether some antecedent condition X has a relationship with some
criterion Y through some intervening mechanism M. In other words, mediational design advance an
X!M!Y style causal chain. Later we will distinguish different types of such designs and argue that
they advance different a priori hypotheses. Nevertheless, the important point to emphasize here is that
mediational designs implicitly depict a causal X!M!Y chain. Whereas substantial development
has occurred surrounding statistical tests of mediated relationships, far less attention has been devoted
to conditions for strong causal inference in such designs. We submit that inferences of mediation are
founded rst and foremost in terms of theory, research design, and the construct validity of measures
employed, and second in terms of statistical evidence of relationships. The greatest challenges for
deriving mediational inferences relates to the specication of causal order among variables, and the
construct validity of the measures employed to operationalize X, M, and Y. In this sense, the
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1032 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
preconditions for mediation tests are quite similar to those involved in specifying and testing causal
(i.e., structural) models (see James, Mulaik, & Brett, 1982; Kenny, 1979).
1
Causal sequence
Perhaps most fundamentally, inferences concerning mediational X!M!Y relationships hinge on
the validity of the assertion that the relationships depicted unfold in that sequence (Stone-Romero &
Rosopa, 2004). In other words, as with structural modeling techniques, multiple qualitatively different
models can be t equally well to the same covariance matrix. Using the exact same data, one could as
easily conrm a Y!M!X mediational chain as one can an X!M!Y sequence (MacCallum,
Wegener, Uchino, & Fabrigar, 1993; Stezl, 1986; Stone-Romero & Rosopa, 2004). Despite passionate
pleas to the contrary by Mitchell (1985) and others, a clear trend away from the use of experimental
designs and a parallel increase in correlational designs has been evident in organizational research in
the past two decades (Scandura & Williams, 2000). Indeed, commenting upon that current state of the
literature, Spencer, Zanna, and Fong (2005, p. 845) lamented that this [correlational mediational]
analysis strategy is overused and has perhaps been elevated as the gold standard of tests of
psychological processes and may even be seen in some quarters as the only legitimate way to examine
them. In short, no statistical analysis can unequivocally differentiate one causal sequence from
another. Theorists and researchers must then rely on other means to justify the sequence of effects. The
most valuable bases to advance such inference come from: (1) experimental design features; (2)
temporal precedence; and (3) theoretical rationales.
Experimental designs
Naturally, experimental designs afford the strongest foundation for making causal inferences.
Hallmarks of randomized experimental designs include random assignment of participants to
conditions, control of extraneous variables, and experimenter control of the independent variable.
Indeed, the philosophy of experimental designs is to isolate and test, as best as possible, X!Y
relationships from competing sources of inuence. In mediational designs, however, this focus is
extended to a three phase X!M!Y causal sequence. The benets of conducting randomized
experiments for testing such sequences has long been recognized. For example, Baron and Kenny
(1986) described a design (based on Smith, 1982) whereby one introduces two experimental
manipulations: (1) one presumed to inuence the mediator and not the criterion; and (2) one presumed
to inuence the criterion yet not the mediator. Analyses of such designs would permit one to distinguish
factors that exert inuence directly on a criterion versus those that are carried through an intervening
mechanism.
More recently, Stone-Romero and Rosopa (2004, p. 283) argued that the only way that one can
make credible inferences about mediation is to perform two or more experiments. In the rst, the cause
[i.e., X] is manipulated to determine its effect on the mediator [i.e., M]. In the second, the mediator [i.e.,
M] is manipulated to determine its effect on the dependent variable [i.e., Y]. Certainly such an
approach affords a solid foundation for making causal inferences, but may not be feasible or even
desirable in many applied circumstances. There may well be ethical, logistical, nancial, and other
considerations that limit the extent to which researchers can employ randomized experimental designs.
1
We should further note that mediation inferences from such designs are predicated not only on the assumptions that X, M, and Y
are causally ordered in that fashion, and their relationships are not attributable to other variables or processes, but also that they are
related linearly (see Bollen, 1989; Kenny et al., 1998; Pearl, 2000 for further details). Whereas non-linear relationships, such as
moderation, can be incorporated in mediational frameworks, that takes us beyond the current discussion (see Baron & Kenny,
1986; James & Brett, 1984; Muller et al., 2005).
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1033
Spencer et al. (2005) outlined circumstances when experiments offer desirable features for drawing
mediational inferences, as well as instances when they may be less applicable. In short, however,
randomized experimental designs offer the strongest basis for drawing causal inferences and should not
be abandoned so prematurely by applied researchers (Mitchell, 1985; Scandura & Williams, 2000).
Quasi-experimental designs also afford means by which causal order can be established (Cook &
Campbell, 1979; Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). Because researchers may not be able to randomly
assign participants to conditions, the causal sequence of X!M!Y is vulnerable to any selection
related threats to internal validity (Cook & Campbell, 1979; Shadish et al., 2002). To the extent that
individuals status on a mediator or criterion variable may alter their likelihood of experiencing a
treatment, the implied causal sequence may also be compromised. For example, consider a typical:
training !self-efcacy !performance, mediational chain. If participation in training is voluntary, and
more efcacious people are more likely to seek training, then the true sequence of events may well be
self-efcacy !training !performance. If higher performing employees develop greater self-efcacy
(Bandura, 1986), then the sequence could actually be performance !efcacy !training. If efcacy
and performance levels remain fairly stable over time, one could easily misconstrue and nd substantial
support for the training !efcacy !performance sequence when the very reverse is actually
occurring.
Researchers also have less control over contaminating variables in quasi-experiments as
compared to randomized experiments. Whereas concerns about contaminating inuences and other
threats to internal validity are extensive and well discussed elsewhere (see Cook & Campbell, 1979;
Shadish et al., 2002; West, Biesanz, & Pitts, 2000), our primary focus here concerns threats to the
implied causal sequence of effects in a mediational design. Revisiting our training example, a
misspecication of causal sequence can emanate from the inuence of an omitted (sometimes
referred to as unmeasured, third, contaminating, hidden, or confounding) variable. For example, if
employees with greater seniority are rst eligible to receive training, and if they also tend to have
higher self-efcacy, then there would be an illusionary training !efcacy relationship unless
seniority is also controlled. The issue here is that one must carefully consider what other variables
might confound the relationships under consideration and account for their inuence when
evaluating the specied causal sequence and variable relationships as outline above. Otherwise,
such inuences might mask real effects (see MacKinnon, Krull, & Lockwood, 2000) or generate
artifactual relationships.
In summary, our discussion about the advantages of experimental and quasi-experimental designs
converges on the larger issues of justifying the presumed causal order of variables and minimizing the
inuence of unmeasured variables. Randomized experiments certainly provide the strongest case for
minimizing the inuence of such potential effects but are difcult to implement. Quasi-experiments
offer much as well, but are susceptible to a variety of threats to causal inferences. James (1980) and
James et al. (1982) have well chronicled this issue. They submitted that an omitted variable poses a
threat to causal inferences if it: (a) has a signicant unique inuence on an effect (i.e., mediator or
criterion); (b) is stable; and (c) is related to at least one other predictor included in the model. In other
words, in mediation analyses, omitted variables represent a signicant threat to validity of the X!M
relationship if they are related both to the antecedent and to the mediator, and have a unique inuence
on the mediator. Moreover, omitted variables represent a signicant threat to inferences involving the
prediction of the criterion, if they have a unique inuence on the outcome variable and either the
antecedent or mediator.
Temporal precedence
A mediational framework proposes that the antecedent preceded the mediator, which in turn preceded
the criterion. Implicitly, therefore, mediational designs advance a time-based model of events whereby
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1034 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
X occurs before M which in turn occurs before Y. To the extent that the measures and operations
employed to operationalize variables in a study are aligned with that sequence, one can have more
condence that the chain of relationships is not compromised. Let us emphasize one point here: it is the
temporal relationships of the underlying phenomena that are at issue, not necessarily the timing of
measurements. Certainly, to the extent that the two different sequences are aligned is of concern.
However, the literature is replete with designs whereby researchers collect a set of observations and
then correlate variables with some record of last years performance, whether that was derived from
performance appraisals, performance outputs, sales, or some nancial index. In effect, this reverses the
presumed sequence of events and more likely models Y!X!M than it does X!M!Y.
Simply assessing a presumed antecedent before measuring a presumed mediator and criterion in no
way assures that the true underlying causal order is consistent with the order of measurement (James
et al., 1982; Kenny, 1979). Indeed the synchronization of measurement timing and the development of
phenomena over time are critical to the basis of causal inferences (Mitchell & James, 2001). Given that
X!M!Y relationships are presumed to unfold over time, it begs the question of how long does it
take each variable to develop and to change? Consider a work redesign effort intended to empower
employees and thereby to enhance their work motivation with the aim of increasing customer
satisfaction. How long does it take to establish the new work design? Over what duration should we
track employees subsequent motivation? If employees are indeed more motivated to perform, how
long will it take for customers to notice and for them to become more satised? These questions are not
easy to answer, and in fewinstances would phenomena readily align with the 3 or 6 month intervals that
organizations are willing to tolerate, even if they are open to multiple data collections. Even worse,
consider the fact that employee motivations (M) in this instance are likely to begin changing before the
work redesign (X) intervention is fully entrenched. And, the appropriate window for sampling
customer reactions may vary widely depending on their frequency of encounters with employees and
other factors. In sum, the guiding point here is that the passage of time between the assessment of X, M,
and Y helps to further strengthen inferences about the causal sequence. To the extent that such
assessments are aligned with the underlying developmental phenomena being studied will strengthen
causal inferences.
Theoretical guidance
Theoretical frameworks usually prescribe a distinct ordering of variables. In fact, it is a hallmark of
good theories that they articulate the how and why variables are ordered in a particular way (e.g., Sutton
& Staw, 1995; Whetten, 1989). This is perhaps the only basis for advancing a particular causal order in
non-experimental studies with simultaneous measurement of the antecedent, mediator, and criterion
variables (i.e., classic cross-sectional designs). For example, Fishbein and Aijzens (1975) Theory of
Reasoned Action has long posited that individuals attitudes give rise to intentions, which in turn
inuence their actual behaviors. This theoretical foundation has been applied extensively to the study
of employees absence and turnover behaviors (Hom & Kinicki, 2001; Tett & Meyer, 1993). The job
characteristics model argues that work design features give rise to psychological states which in turn
inuence individuals reactions (e.g., satisfaction) and behaviors (Hackman &Oldham, 1980). Mathieu
(1991) used Lewinian Field Theory (Lewin, 1943) to submit that variables more psychologically
removed from oneself (i.e., distal effects such as perceptions of work characteristics), would inuence
more psychologically proximal variables (e.g., role states) and thereby affect work attitudes (e.g.,
satisfaction, organizational commitment). Absent an experimental or longitudinal design, one might
test a mediational model on the basis of the theoretical ordering of variables. Naturally the case would
be stronger if one could also leverage features from a design perspective, but clearly the theory must
articulate a certain causal sequence. And, in cross-sectional studies one often has little else to justify
any particular order.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1035
To summarize, the specication of the causal order of variables is absolutely critical to inferences
about mediational relationships. This is rst and foremost a theoretical exercise. Research design
features in terms of experimental control and temporal precedence provide additional justication for
particular sequences. Notably, there is no panacea for justifying causal sequence. Learned scholars
differ on what they believe is sufcient grounds upon which to claim causal order. On one extreme,
Stone-Romero and Rosopa (2004) submitted that anything short of a randomized experiment is
insufcient to claim justied causal order. Their position is tests of mediation models that are based
upon data from non-experimental studies have little or no capacity to serve as a basis for valid
inferences about mediation (Stone-Romero & Rosopa, 2004; p. 250). On the other extreme, a perusal
of journal articles will quickly reveal numerous instances of authors claiming causal connections
from mediational analyses of cross-sectional data collected in a single survey as related to last years
performance indices. Reasonable people can disagree, and we personally believe that both of the above
extreme positions are probably overstated. In any case, to the extent that ones work is: (a) grounded in
strong theory; (b) employs true or quasi-experimental designs; and (c) assesses variables over time in
the proper sequence and intervals, condence in the causal sequence of variables in a particular model
is enhanced.
Measurement related issues
As with any research investigation, the construct validity of measures employed are of concern in tests
of mediation. Schwab (1980) submitted Construct validity is dened as representing the
correspondence between a construct (conceptual denition of a variable) and the operational
procedure to measure or manipulate that construct (pp. 56). Of note in particular for mediational
analyses, attention should be directed at the convergent and discriminant validity of measures.
Convergent validity
Convergent validity essentially concerns the extent to which different measures of the same construct
hold together or converge on the intended construct. Usually convergent validity is assessed using
techniques such as factor analyses and other approaches that evaluate how well different observations
relate to a latent variable. Naturally, this concept is related to reliability concepts such as internal
consistency estimates, alternative forms/methods, interrater, or test-retest. Depending on the nature of
the constructs involved in the X!M!Y relationship, any combination of reliability estimates may
be applicable (see Nunnally, 1978). Of note for the present discussion is the fact that measurement
unreliability, particularly that of the mediator, can bias mediational analyses. As Hoyle and Kenny
(1999) have demonstrated, assuming all positive paths, to the extent that a mediator is measured with less
than perfect reliability, the M!Yrelationship would likely be underestimated, whereas the X!Ywould
likely be overestimated when the antecedent and mediator are considered simultaneously. Whereas latent
variable modeling can help to compensate for measurement shortcomings, the technique is certainly not a
panacea. Consequently, the message here is clear: it is critical to use reliable measures when testing
mediation, particularly when it comes to the mediator variable.
Discriminant validity
Discriminant validity of measures is another concern for all research investigations, yet particularly for
tests of mediation. Discriminant validity refers to the extent to which measures of different constructs
are empirically and theoretically distinguishable. Note that discriminant validity must be gauged in the
context of the larger nomological network within which the relationships being considered are believed
to reside. Discriminant validity does not imply that measures of different constructs are uncorrelated;
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1036 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
indeed if that were the case there would be no mediational covariance to be modeled. The issue is
whether measures of different variables are so highly correlated as to raise questions about whether
they are assessing different constructs.
If measures of an antecedent variable and a mediator are not sufciently distinguishable, then they
are in effect tapping the same underlying domain. Consequently, any attempt to parse their independent
contributions to a criterion variable will be futile. For example, if Xand M fail to evidence discriminant
validity, any sequential analysis of their substantive relationship will conclude that the mediator carries
the inuence of X on Y. The same conclusion would follow in situations where the mediator and
criterion and not distinguishable. This problemis akin to the notion of multi-collinearity between either
the X and M variables, or between the M and Y variables. Consequently, it is incumbent on researchers
to demonstrate that their measures of X, M, and Yevidence acceptable discriminant validity before any
mediational tests are justied. This may be done in a variety of fashions ranging fromexploratory factor
analyses to more powerful multi-trait, multi-method approaches, and conrmatory factor analyses. In
sum, a lack of discriminant validity between either X and M, or between M and Y, will lead to an
illusionary mediational relationship that amounts to nothing more than correlating some measure of a
construct with another measure of the same construct.
Distinguishing indirect and mediating relationships
Up to this point we have employed the term mediating variable in a very general sense. Unfortunately,
different authors dene mediation in many different ways and often use terms such as indirect effects,
intervening variables, intermediate endpoint, and so forth interchangeably with mediators. (see
MacKinnon et al., 2002 for a review). Further, although mediational models are pervasive in applied
research and elsewhere, there is some debate concerning the requisite statistical evidence for drawing
inferences about mediation (cf., Baron & Kenny, 1986; Collins, Graham, & Flaherty, 1998; Frazier
et al, 2004; Holmbeck, 1997; James & Brett, 1984; James, Mulaik, & Brett, 2006; Kenny, Kashy, &
Bolger, 1998; MacKinnon, et al., 2000, 2002; Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). We
believe that root causes of such controversies lie in differences of opinion regarding: (1) denitions of
mediators and related concepts; (2) the necessity of rst demonstrating a signicant total X!Y
relationship; and (3) the appropriate base model for tests of different forms of mediation.
The rst two points of contention are closely intertwined. Some have submitted that a precondition
for tests of mediation is that the antecedent must exhibit a signicant total relationship with a criterion
when considered alone (i.e., X!Y, see Baron & Kenny, 1986; Judd & Kenny, 1981; Preacher &
Hayes, 2004). Others have relaxed this precondition, and argued that mediation inferences are justied
if the indirect effect carried by the X!M and M!Y paths is signicant (e.g., Kenny, et al., 1998;
MacKinnon et al., 2002). Advocates of this latter view often equate mediator variables with indirect
effects (e.g., Alwin & Hauser, 1975; Bollen, 1987; MacKinnon et al., 2002). However, there is an
important distinction between indirect and mediator variables.
For example, MacKinnon et al. (2002, p. 83) suggested that An intervening variable (Mediator)
transmits the effect of an independent variable to a dependent variable [emphasis added]. In contrast,
Baron and Kenny (1986, p. 176) submitted that a given variable may be said to function as a mediator
to the extent that it accounts for the relation between the predictor and the criterion [emphasis added].
Preacher and Hayes (2004, p.719) explicitly drew a distinction between the two concepts and argued
that mediation is a special, more restrictive, type of intervening relationship.
A conclusion that a mediation effect is present implies that the total effect X!Y was present
initially. There is no such assumption in the assessment of indirect effects. It is quite possible to nd
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1037
that an indirect effect is signicant even when there is no evidence for a signicant total effect.
Whether or not the effect also represents mediation should be judged through examination of the
total effect.
In other words, mediator variables are explanatory mechanisms that shed light on the nature of the
relationship that exists between two variables. If no such relationship exists, then there is nothing to be
mediated. While a chain of events whereby X!M and M!Y may well be of interest, along with
the extent to which variance in Y can be attributed to the indirect effect of X, we submit that sequence
represents a qualitatively different phenomenon than mediation. We prefer to label such relationships
as indirect effects.
We readily acknowledge that there is another reason why some researchers (e.g., MacKinnon et al.,
2000) advocate dropping the X!Y precondition for mediational inferences. This second position
centers around the fact that confounding, suppression, and interactive effects could attenuate overall
X!Y relationships (see MacKinnon et al., 2000).
2
Similarly, others have argued that competing
effects might mitigate total X!Y relationships, when opposite signed direct and indirect effects are
present (e.g., when X and M are both positively related to Y, yet X and M are negatively related).
Notably, the common thread through all of these positions is that some other variable, including
perhaps the mediator, serves to contaminate the total X!Y relationship when viewed in isolation. In
other words, the opposite signs and mediator as a suppressor arguments both suggest that the true
underlying model is a partially mediated one whereby the direct effect of X!Ycan only be interpreted
in the context of a model that also includes the M!Y path. This raises a central point about the
importance of the base model that one hypothesizes.
James et al. (2006) underscored the importance of the base model that one adopts for tests of
mediation. In the case of full mediation, M is hypothesized to fully account for the signicant total
effect of X!Y. In other words, the direct effect of X!Yis no longer signicant once M!Yhas been
included. In contrast, in the case of partial mediation, M is believed to account for a signicant portion
of the total X!Y, but a signicant direct effect also remains. In other words, both M!Yand X!Y
are signicant when considered simultaneously. Of course, both partial and full mediation models are
predicated on a signicant X!M relationship. James et al. (2006) and Shrout and Bolger (2002) noted
that full and partial mediational inferences rely on different types of statistical tests. Consequently,
which model one hypothesizes may lead to different conclusions in many instances.
James et al. (2006) noted that the Baron and Kenny (1986) approach implicitly advocates partial
mediation as the base model for tests of mediation. Alternatively, James and Brett (1984), and James
et al. (2006) prefer the axiom of parsimony and advocate the full mediation base model. All agreed that
substantive reasoning should guide which is adopted in any given circumstance; but the important point
is that the a priori model that one advances has important implications for conrmatory and
disconrming statistical evidence. We extend this logic and submit that the specication of ones
hypothesized base model has implications for indirect effects along with partial and full mediation.
Moreover, such relations can be examined in the context of larger structural models where the
inuences of other variables of interest are also considered. Nevertheless, the evidential basis for
drawing inferences of each type remains consistent and is outlined below.
In summary, we believe that there are different types of relationships that fall under the general
heading of intervening effects. Accordingly, we use the term intervening effects to describe any type of
2
Concerns about the inuence of interactive or confounding variables imply the presence of non-linear relationships which
violates an assumption of testing indirect or mediated relations, unless one is also hypothesizing moderation (see Footnote 1).
Further, extraneous, omitted, or 3rd variables represent specication errors that always must be accounted for, through theoretical,
methodological, or empirical means, whenever a causal sequence of effects is advanced (James et al., 1982; Stone-Romero &
Rosopa, 2004). We elaborate more fully on this and related points below.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1038 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
linking mechanism M that ties an antecedent with a criterion. Indirect effects are a special form of
intervening effect whereby X and Y are not related directly (i.e., are uncorrelated), but they are
indirectly related through signicant relationships with a linking mechanism. In contrast, mediation
refers to instances where the signicant total relationship that exists between an antecedent and a
criterion, is accounted for in part (partial mediation) or completely (full mediation) by a mediator
variable.
Estimation Guidelines and Decision Rules
We submit that different statistical rules of evidence apply depending on whether one anticipates an
indirect effect versus partial or full mediation. We argue that researchers are obliged to specify, a priori,
which type of intervening process that they anticipate. Importantly, the nature of the hypothesized
relationship leads to different sources of conrmatory and disconrming evidence. In this sense, what
we are advocating is an approach that is similar to that of structural equation modeling (SEM).
Accordingly, a failure to reject a hypothesized model hinges on two types of tests: (1) conrmation of
hypothesized relations (i.e., relationships that were hypothesized to exist are indeed signicant and in
the hypothesized directions); and (2) disconrmation of non-hypothesized paths (i.e., sufcient model
t indices, which indicate that the paths that were hypothesized to be absent are indeed not signicant).
Moreover, because different competing models can be t to the same data, we advocate contrasting
ones hypothesized model against viable alternative models (see Anderson &Gerbing, 1988, for a good
background on this general approach).
Figure 1 presents three alternative models containing intervening effects and their respective
parameters. In this sense, the indirect effects model is the most constrained or parsimonious, as it
implies that the only signicant relationships observed are the combined effect (b
mx
b
ym
). This
implies that both the X!M(b
mx
) and M!Y(b
ym
) paths are signicant, although the combined effect
is best tested using approaches such as the Sobel test (see MacKinnon et al., 2002; Shrout & Bolger,
Y M X
Y M X
Y M X
n o i t a i d e M l a i t r a P
n o i t a i d e M l l u F
t c e f f E t c e r i d n I
x m
x m
x m
ym
ym
y x . m
y m . x
x y
Figure 1. Alternative intervening models
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1039
2002 for details). Importantly, an indirect effect hypothesis also implicitly suggests that the total X!Y
relationship (b
yx
) is absent. The full mediation model is the next most parsimonious. This model also
includes signicant X!M(b
mx
) and M!Y(b
ym
) paths. However, the dashed line fromX!Yin this
model is meant to imply a signicant total X!Y (b
yx
) relationship that becomes non-signicant when
M!Y (b
ym
) is included. In other words, a hypothesis of full mediation also requires a non-signicant
b
yx.m
effect. Last, the partial mediation model is the least parsimonious and implies that X!M (b
mx
),
as well as both M!Yand X!Y will be signicant when considered simultaneously (b
ym.x
and b
yx.m
,
respectively).
The three panels of Figure 2 specify sequences of effects to be considered in order for each of the
hypothesized models to be supported. Later we will describe analytic techniques which provide
information regarding these effects. The columns of rectangles in Figure 2 depicts the various
conditions that must hold for each model to be accepted, whereas the branches containing circles depict
guided alternative hypotheses that might be considered if a hypothesized condition is disconrmed.
Notably, by accepted models we mean that the data fail to reject the hypothesized model. As is always
the case, this does not mean that a hypothesized model has been proven; simply that it is not
inconsistent with the data. Similarly, once any facet of a hypothesized model is rejected, one enters an
exploratory mode as alternative models are considered. Consequently, any conclusions that are derived
from such searches are tentative at best and need to be validated on a new sample.
Indirect effects
As shown in Panel 1 of Figure 2, the pivotal test of the indirect model is simply (b
mx
b
ym
) using
methods such as the Sobel (1982) test or more sophisticated approaches employing bootstrapping
techniques (see, MacKinnon et al., 2002; Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). If such a
test is not signicant, then one should reject the indirect effect hypothesis and consider viable
alternatives. A more parsimonious alternative in such instances would be to consider simply a direct
Figure 2. Decision tree for evidence supporting different intervening effects
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1040 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
X!Y (b
yx
) relationship. Moreover, even if the indirect effect was signicant, researchers should
consider whether alternative models such as a partially or fully mediated model are suggested by the
data. For example, if the overall X!Y relationship was signicant (b
yx
), and X does not contribute to
the prediction of Yonce M has been considered (b
yx.m
), then the hypothesis of an indirect effect would
be rejected in lieu of an alternative model of full mediation.
This approach echoes our earlier comments about researchers casual dismissal of the precondition
of a total X!Yeffect for tests of mediation. Our reading of the literature suggests that there are very
few instances where researchers actually hypothesized a priori that the total X!Y relationship would
be non-signicant. Rather, it appears as though many evoke a waiver of the X!Yprecondition when it
fails to materialize in their data, and then simply proceed to test the signicance of the indirect effect.
This has occurred even when there was no evidence to suggest suppression or counter-acting signs of
direct and indirect effects. Such tactics, in our opinion, moves one away from conrmatory hypothesis
testing and into the exploratory realm. In summary, we submit that the presence of a signicant total
X!Y relationship leads to the rejection of a hypothesis of an indirect effect and should trigger a
consideration of an alternative partial mediation hypothesis (if suppression or counter-acting effects are
suspected), and thereby perhaps, to a full mediation explanation.
Full mediation
As depicted in the second panel of Figure 2, a hypothesis of full mediation is predicated on a signicant
total X!Y (b
yx
) relationship. Failing that, one might consider an alternative hypothesis of an indirect
effect. If suppression is evident, then one might consider the alternative hypothesis of a partially
mediated relationship. Assuming the total effect was present, one proceeds to test the X!M (b
mx
) and
M!Y (b
ym
) relationships. If either fails to exist, then the evidence is consistent with the alternative
hypothesis of a direct effect. Moreover, full mediation depends on the non-signicance of direct effect
of X!Y when the M!Y path is included (i.e., a non-signicant b
yx.m
). If the direct X!Y path is
signicant in this context, then the hypothesis of full mediation should be rejected and the researcher
should consider the alternative hypothesis of partial mediation. We should note that there may be cases
where adding the b
yx.m
parameter attenuates the M!Y relationship (b
ym.x
) to a non-signicant level.
If the X!Y relationship (b
yx.m
) is signicant in such an instance, then the full mediational hypothesis
should be rejected in lieu of an alternative hypothesis of a direct effect. Alternatively, if neither b
yx.m
or
b
ym.x
are signicant, and the previous conditions were satised, then the data are consistent with the
hypothesis of full mediation. This follows from the fact that the relevant M!Y parameter for the full
mediation hypothesis is b
ym
not b
ym.x
(see James et al., 2006).
Partial mediation
A partial mediation hypothesis, as shown in Panel 3 of Figure 1, is the least constrained and rests on the
signicance of all three paths: X!M (b
mx
) and both X!Y (b
yx.m
) and M!Y (b
ym.x
) when
considered simultaneously. Given the presumed causal order of variables, if the X!Y (b
yx.m
) path is
not signicant in this model, then the hypothesis of partial mediation should be rejected and one should,
perhaps, consider an alternative hypothesis of full mediation. Alternatively, if the X!M (b
mx
) or the
M!Y (b
ym.x
) paths are not signicant, then the partial mediation hypothesis should be rejected in lieu
of the alternative hypothesis of simply a direct effect. The partial mediation hypothesis would only be
supported if all three hypothesized paths are signicant.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1041
Caveats
We should highlight two other related concerns for the tests outlined above. First, our various decision
points pivot on tests of statistical signicance, just as any SEM nested model comparisons do (see
Anderson &Gerbing, 1988). Nevertheless, tests of statistical signicance must be considered in light of
related issues such as sample size and measurement reliability (Hoyle & Kenny, 1999). In other words,
signicance tests must be tempered by considerations of power and effect size estimates. Enormous
sample sizes can yield statistically signicant results that are virtually meaningless in practice and also
easily lead to the rejection of full mediation hypotheses. Alternatively, small sample sizes can easily
lead to inferences of full mediation in instances where there is not sufcient power to adequately test for
partial mediation. The summary point is that sufcient power must exist to adequately test various
relationships, and researchers should balance conclusions about statistical signicance with those
about practical signicance.
A second, and related caveat, concerns the relative power of different tests. MacKinnon et al. (2002)
have argued that a direct test of intervening effects (b
mx
b
ym
) has greater power as compared to causal
steps approaches such as those outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986). However, recall that MacKinnon
et al. (2002) equated indirect effects with mediation, and argued that overall, the step requiring a
signicant total effect of X on Y led to the most Type II errors, (p. 96). In other words, the
distinguishing feature between indirect and mediator relations is what accounts for the fact that tests of
the latter are more conservative than the former. The fact that mediators rely on the presence of a total
direct effect represents a greater statistical burden, however, so we believe that the corresponding lower
power is totally appropriate. In other words, the combined tests of indirect effects appear to have greater
statistical power simply as a consequence of comparing them with qualitatively different types of
relationshipsnamely, mediation.
Summary
Tests of intervening effects are predicated on the assumption that the causal sequence of variables is
sufciently justied and the measures employed to represent the constructs possess sufcient construct
validity. While not particularly controversial, we believe these preconditions are often overlooked and
should be afforded more attention by scholars. Less clarity surrounds the rules of evidence for
mediational type inferences and associated statistical tests. A key to most of this confusion is the fact
that X!M!Y models may represent full mediation, partial mediation, or indirect effectsall of
which are conrmed or disconrmed in slightly different ways. We submit that researchers are obliged
to a priori specify the nature of the relationship(s) that they anticipate, and then to conduct the
corresponding tests to demonstrate both conrmatory and disconrming evidence. To better illustrate
how this works in practice, we offer the following example.
Empirical Illustration
The purpose of this illustration is to demonstrate the steps and evidential basis involved in testing
indirect effects, and partial and fully mediated relationships. Our example focuses on the concept of
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1042 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
self-efcacy as a mediator of the inuences of individual differences and situational cues on
individuals performance and is illustrated in Figure 3. Self-efcacy is dened as peoples judgments
of their capabilities to organize and execute courses of action required to attain designated types of
performances (Bandura, 1986, p. 391). The positive relationship between self-efcacy and
performance has been demonstrated time and again and summarized in narrative reviews (e.g.,
Bandura & Locke, 2003) and in meta-analyses (e.g., Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998). Hence, there is
abundant support for viewing it as an inuence on performance.
Bandura (1977) has long theorized that self-efcacy is determined mostly by the cognitive appraisal
and integration of information cues. Two of the most inuential of such cues are enactive mastery and
vicarious experience. Enactive mastery develops through repeated performance accomplishments in
the same or similar situations. In other words, to the extent that individuals have performed well in a
particular situation in the past, it is reasonable to expect that their efcacy expectations will be higher.
Further, naturally one would expect that individuals past performance would correlate positively with
their future performance for reasons other than simply their efcacy expectations (e.g., because ability
inuences both). Consequently, as depicted in Figure 3, we would hypothesize that self-efcacy would
partially mediate the relationship between previous (i.e., baseline) performance and subsequent
performance.
Vicarious experience is gained through direct observation or information about how well others
have performed in a situation. However, there is no reason to expect that vicarious experience
would inuence individuals performance unless they internalized such information in terms of
their efcacy expectations. Consequently, we hypothesized that self-efcacy would fully mediate
the relationship between normative information and individuals performance. Indeed, previous
research has been consistent with this expectation (e.g., Mathieu & Button, 1992; Weiss, Suckow, &
Rakestraw, 1999).
Finally, recent theorizing and research have argued that relatively stable individual differences may
inuence efcacy expectations. For example, Phillips and Gully (1997) found support for a positive
correlation between individuals learning goal orientation and their self-efcacy in an academic setting.
Individuals who are high on learning goal-orientation strive to understand something newor to increase
their level of competence in a given activity (Button, Mathieu, &Zajac, 1996). Whereas a learning goal
orientation may contribute directly to performance, it is more likely to help shape specic task-related
perceptions such as self-efcacy. Although some previous researchers have found results that are
consistent with a hypothesis of full mediation (e.g., Phillips & Gully, 1997), others have found
relationships more consistent with an indirect effect inference (e.g., Chen et al., 2000; Diefendorff,
2004; Potosky & Ramakrishna, 2002). Which interpretation is most appropriate is debatable. However,
for present illustration purposes, we hypothesized that learning goal orientation would exhibit a
positive indirect effect with performance via self-efcacy.
Figure 3. Hypothesized intervening effects
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1043
Method
Participants
Two hundred and one undergraduates were recruited from introductory psychology courses at a large
northeastern University and received extra credit toward their course grade for participation. The
sample was 61 per cent female, and their average age was 18.65 (SD1.97). Participants were invited
to attend experimental sessions where they were randomly assigned to one of the three normative
information conditions (n 67 per condition) as described below.
Task and procedure
The task was identical to the one used by Mathieu and Button (1992). It involved the creation of words
containing three or more letters drawn from a list of 10 letters: four vowels (worth 1 point each), two
consonants (worth 1 point each), and four additional consonants worth 2, 3, 4, and 10 points,
respectively. The point values associated with each letter correspond to those used in the game
Scrabble. The object of the task was to score as many points as possible during a 10-minutes session
by forming words containing three or more letters from the list provided, excluding proper nouns and
slang. Points were awarded according to the point values associated with letters used in each word
generated.
Upon arrival at the experimental session, participants completed an informed consent form and a
survey that contained demographic items and a measure of learning goal orientation from Button et al.
(1996). Once the survey was completed, the experimental task was explained to participants and they
completed a 5-minutes practice exercise. They then calculated their own score on the practice exercise
and were told that they would perform a 10-minutes experimental trial after answering some survey
questions. The rst page of the survey instrument presented the normative information manipulation
using the following statement. In previous testing we have found that students like you score about 115
[175, 235] points on the task you are about to complete [emphasis in instructions]. The middle value
corresponded to pilot subjects average performance, whereas the low- and high-point values were set
one standard deviation from the mean, based on pilot data. They then completed several survey items
that included their self-efcacy and a manipulation check, and then completed the 10-minutes task,
were debriefed and given their extra credit slips, and dismissed.
Measures
Manipulation check
A manipulation check identical to that used by Mathieu and Button (1992) was administered after the
survey items. It asked participants how many points they thought most people would score on the task
they were about to complete. As anticipated, their responses differed signicantly across the normative
information conditions (F(2,194) 189.16, p <0.001) and all means differed signicantly from each
other in the anticipated fashion.
Performance
Participants practice and task performances were simply the total number of points they earned during
each of the timed periods. Individuals scored their own practice trail in order to provide clear and
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1044 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
immediate feedback. Their calculations were later checked by a coder, blind to manipulated normative
information, and found to be highly accurate.
Self-efcacy
Self-efcacy was assessed using a nine-item graduated scale identical to that employed by Mathieu and
Button (1992). The 9-point scale spanned two standard deviations above and below the mean point
obtained in the pilot sessions. Participants rated the extent to which they believed that they could score
at least each of the point values using a Likert-type 7-point scale, ranging from 1 (virtually no
possibility) through 4 (about a 50/50 chance) to 7 (complete certainty). Thus, higher scores represent
greater levels of self-efcacy (a0.95). We created three parcels (i.e., subscales) for use in the SEM
analysis by averaging the highest, lowest, and midpoint rating to form one indicator, the next highest,
lowest, and middle value to form a second, and the remaining three ratings to form a third.
3
Learning goal orientation
Learning goal orientation was assessed using six items from Button et al. (1996). An example item is I
prefer to work on tasks that force me to learn new things. Participants responded to each item using a
17 agreement scale with higher values representing greater learning orientation (a 0.77). We also
created three parcels for this measure, by rst tting them to a single factor model and then averaging
the highest and lowest loading items to form one composite, and so forth as described above.
Analytic overview
We employed Anderson and Gerbings (1988) two-step SEM strategy to test the model depicted in
Figure 1 using LISREL 8.54 (Joreskog, Sorbom, du Toit, & du Toit, 2000). SEM techniques have long
been advocated as preferable to regression techniques for testing mediational relationships because
they permit one to model both measurement and structural relationships and yield overall t indices
(cf., Baron & Kenny, 1986; James & Brett, 1984; James et al., 2006; Kenny et al., 1998). Accordingly,
we rst t a conrmatory factor analytic (CFA) measurement model followed by a series of structural
models testing our hypothesized relationships.
In order to gauge model t, we report the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMSR),
Goodness of Fit index (GFI; Joreskog et al., 2000), and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990).
We also report x
2
values which provide a statistical basis for comparing the relative t of nested models.
SRMSR is a measure of the standardized difference between the observed covariance and predicted
covariance. Usually, SRMSR values 0.08 are considered a relatively good t for the model, and
values 0.10 considered fair (Browne & Cudeck, 1989). The CFI is an incremental t index that
contrasts the t of a hypothesized SEM model against a baseline (uncorrelated indicators) model.
Historically, SEM model incremental t indices such as GFI and CFI <0.90 have been considered
wanting and likely to be improved substantially. More recently, however, Hu and Bentler (1999)
proposed that use of combined cutoffs such as CFI 0.95 and SRMSR0.08 results in better
balance of rejection rates for misspecied models under different conditions. In contrast, Marsh,
Kit-Tai, and Wen (2004), Beauducel and Wittmann (2005), Fan and Sivo (2005) illustrated that
deciding on the most appropriate index and cutoff is a complex function of the nature of model
3
This parceling approach helps to reduce the ratio of estimated parameters to sample size in SEM analyses (Hagtvet & Nasser,
2004; Hall, Snell, & Foust, 1999; Landis, Beal, & Tesluk, 2000). It is also true that graduated self-efcacy items such as these
yield notoriously positively skewed distributions for the highest levels rated and negatively skewed distributions for the lowest
levels rated. Combining the ratings in this fashion yields parcels that better fulll the normal distribution assumptions of SEM
indicators.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1045
misspecications, sample sizes, balances between Type I and Type II tradeoffs, and a host of other
factors. All authors also emphasized that the acceptability of models rests heavily on the extent to
which hypothesized parameters are signicant and in the anticipated directions, as well as issues such
as parsimony. Given such controversy and the complexity of issues surrounding cutoff values for model
t indices, along with our study characteristics, we will consider models with CFI values <0.90 and
SRMSRvalues >0.10 as decient, those with CFI 0.90 to <0.95 and SRMSR>0.08 to 0.10 ranges
as acceptable, and ones with CFI 0.95 and SRMSR<0.08 ranges as excellent.
Results
Conrmatory factor analysis model
Descriptive statistics and variable correlations are presented in Table 1. Table 2 presents a summary of
the t indices for the various models that we tested. Using the covariance matrix, we estimated a ve-
factor CFA model. In effect, this model amounts to testing the discriminant validity of three self-
efcacy and three learning goal orientation parcels, as the remaining three latent variables (i.e.,
normative information manipulation, baseline, and nal performance) were set equal to their observed
scores. The ve-factor CFA evidenced excellent t indices [x
2
(20) 19.04, n.s.; GFI 0.98;
CFI 1.00; SRMSR0.019] with all six parcels exhibiting signicant (p <0.05) relationships with
their intended latent variable.
To further test the discriminant validity of the self-efcacy and learning goal orientation measures,
we t a four-factor CFA model by constraining their latent variables to unity. This four-factor model,
which is nested in the ve-factor model, exhibited a decient and signicantly worse model t [Dx
2
(1) 27.24, p <0.001; x
2
(21) 46.28, p <0.01; GFI 0.95; CFI 0.98; SRMSR0.11]. Finally, we
t a null latent CFA model (which constrains the correlations among the latent variables to zero) to the
data [x
2
(30) 157.42, p <0.001; GFI 0.85; CFI 0.89; SRMSR0.19] and obtained a
signicantly worse t [Dx
2
(10) 138.38, p <0.001] as compared to the ve-factor model.
Table 1. Variable descriptive statistics and correlations
Variables
LLGO LSE
Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Baseline performance 69.93 31.26 0.07 0.49

0.08 0.50

2. Normative information
a
2.00 0.82 0.07 0.16

0.05 0.16

3. Performance 84.55 32.31 0.49

0.16

0.03 0.39

4. LGO-1 5.36 0.93 0.04 0.02 0.05 0.18

5. LGO-2 5.48 0.79 0.06 0.08 0.04 0.53


6. LGO-3 5.95 0.70 0.06 0.01 0.04 0.44

0.55


7. Self-efcacy-1 5.07 1.04 0.47

0.17

0.38

0.09 0.17

0.12
8. Self-efcacy-2 4.94 1.17 0.50

0.16

0.38

0.09 0.14

0.11 0.96


9. Self-efcacy-3 5.46 1.19 0.49

0.14

0.39

0.09 0.15

0.12 0.91 0.92

p <0.05.
a
Coded: 1 Low, 2 Moderate, 3 High.
Notes: Lower diagonal contains indicator correlations, upper diagonal contains latent variable correlations.
LLGOLatent learning goal orientation, LSELatent self-efcacy
N201.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1046 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
Collectively, these results indicate that the measurement properties t quite well and there is sufcient
covariance among the latent variables to warrant examining the different intervening effects. We should
also highlight that the t of the ve-factor CFA is equivalent to a saturated structural modelor one
that includes direct paths from all antecedents to both the mediator (i.e., self-efcacy) and to the
criterion (i.e., performance). This saturated model provides a useful comparison against which to gauge
the t of other models.
Structural models
Below we t different structural models to test the three different types of intervening effects that were
hypothesized. In effect, we isolate the direct and indirect effects for each of the three antecedents.
However, we rst t only directs and no directs models to serve as additional bases of comparison.
The only directs model estimates direct relationships from all antecedents to performance, with no
paths leading to or stemming from the self-efcacy mediator (although self-efcacy remains as a latent
variable in the model). This model exhibited decient t indices [x
2
(24) 96.36, p <0.001;
GFI 0.92; CFI 0.94; SRMSR0.170] and differed signicantly from the CFA model
[Dx
2
(4) 77.32, p <0.001]. This indicates that at least one of the antecedents has a signicant
direct effect with self-efcacy, or efcacy related signicantly with performance. In other words, these
results attest to the importance of the mediator variable. In the context of this model, both normative
information (b
yx
0.12, p <0.05) and baseline performance (b
yx
0.48, p <0.01) related signicantly
to performance, whereas learning goal orientation (b
yx
0.02, n.s.) did not. These ndings are
consistent with the anticipated forms of intervening effects.
The no direct effects model estimated paths from each of the antecedents to self-efcacy, and from
efcacy to performance, but contained no direct effects from the antecedents to performance. This
model exhibited acceptable t indices [x
2
(23) 51.32, p <0.01; GFI 0.95; CFI 0.98;
SRMSR0.052] but did differ signicantly from the CFA model [Dx
2
(3) 32.28, p <0.001]. This
lack of t indicates that one or more of the antecedents has a signicant direct effect with performance.
In the context of this model, all three antecedents related signicantly with self-efcacy (learning goal
orientation b
mx
0.22, p <0.05; normative information b
mx
0.14, p <0.05; and baseline
performance b
mx
0.51, p <0.01), and self-efcacy exhibited a signicant relationship with
performance (b
ym
0.19, p <0.05). Therefore, the X!M relationship (b
mx
) is evident for all three
intervening effects, and the M!Y relationship holds, at least when considered alone. Notably, self-
efcacy retained its signicant relationship with performance in all models that we examined. In
Table 2. Summary of structural equation modeling analyses
Models
Fit indices
DF x
2
GFI CFI SRMSR
Saturated model 20 19.04 0.98 1.00 0.019
Learning goal direct 22 48.79

0.95 0.98 0.047


Normative information
a
direct 22 49.20

0.95 0.98 0.050


Baseline performance direct 22 21.90 0.98 1.00 0.024
No directs 23 51.32

0.95 0.98 0.052


Only directs 24 96.36

0.92 0.94 0.170


Null latent 30 157.42

0.85 0.89 0.190

p <0.01.
a
Coded: 1 Low, 2 Moderate, 3 High.
Notes: N201.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1047
addition, the indirect effects of all the antecedents to performance via self-efcacy (b
mx
b
ym
) were
signicant in this model: learning goal orientation: b0.09, Sobel 4.70, SE1.69, p <0.05;
normative information b0.05, Sobel 2.14, SE0.99, p <0.05; and baseline performance
b 0.20, Sobel 0.21, SE0.04, p <0.01.
In summary, these two base models provide us with valuable information about the signicance of
the parameters associated with the different intervening effects. From the only directs model we
ascertained that the mediator variable plays an important role in the context of our model. From the no
directs model we learned that the indirect effect of each antecedent with performance was signicant,
as transmitted through the self-efcacy mediator. We now turn to additional models that complete the
picture for the three different relationships that were hypothesized.
Indirect effect
Recall that learning goal orientation was hypothesized to have only an indirect effect with performance
via self-efcacy. As shown in the upper triangle of Table 1, the correlation (i.e., total effect, b
yx
)
between the latent learning goal orientation variable and performance was not signicant (r 0.03,
ns), as anticipated. Using the no directs model as a base, we next t a learning goal direct model by
adding a path from learning goal orientation to performance. Although this model exhibited excellent
t indices, [x
2
(22) 48.79, p <0.01; GFI 0.95; CFI 0.98; SRMSR0.047], it was not a
signicant improvement over the no direct model [Dx
2
(1) 2.53, n.s.] and it differed signicantly from
the saturated model [Dx
2
(2) 29.75, p <0.001]. This implies that the direct effect of learning goal
orientation to performance was not signicant, and indeed it was not (b
ym.x
0.12, n.s.). In contrast,
the indirect effect of learning goal orientation to performance via self-efcacy was signicant in this
model (b
mx
b
ym.x
0.09, Sobel 5.18, SE1.83, p <0.05). In summary, the indirect effect
(b
mx
b
ym
) was signicant in this model, whereas the direct X!Y relationship (b
yx.m
) was not. Given
that the total (b
yx
) was also not signicant, these results are consistent with the hypothesis of an indirect
effect.
Fully mediated effect
We hypothesized that the inuence of the normative information manipulation on performance would
be fully mediated by self-efcacy. As illustrated in the upper triangle of Table 1, the correlation
between the normative information manipulation and performance was signicant (r 0.16, p <0.05),
as anticipated. Therefore, the total (b
yx
) condition was fullled. Next, again using the no directs model
as a base, next t a normative information direct model by adding a path from normative information
to performance. This model also exhibited excellent t indices, [x
2
(22) 49.20, p <0.01; GFI 0.95;
CFI 0.98; SRMSR0.050], but was also not a signicant improvement over the no direct model
[Dx
2
(1) 2.12, n.s.], and differed signicantly from the saturated model [Dx
2
(2) 30.16, p <0.001].
Thus, the direct effect of normative information to performance was not signicant (b
yx.m
0.10, n.s.)
although the indirect effect was (b
mx
b
ym.x
0.05, Sobel 2.04, SE0.95, p <0.05). In summary,
the results of this model indicate that normative information has a direct effect on the self-efcacy
mediator (b
mx
), self-efcacy has a signicant relationship with performance (b
ym
), and that the direct
effect of normative information to performance (b
yx.m
) is no longer signicant. Given the earlier
signicant total X!Y effect (b
yx
), these results are consistent with the hypothesis of a full mediation.
Partially mediated effect
Last, we hypothesized that the inuence of the baseline performance on the experimental trial
performance would be partially mediated by self-efcacy. Accordingly, using the no directs model
again as a base, we t a baseline performance direct model by adding a path from baseline
performance to performance. This model exhibited excellent t indices [x
2
(22) 21.90, n.s.;
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1048 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
GFI 0.98; CFI 1.00; SRMSR0.024], was a signicant improvement over the no directs model
[Dx
2
(1) 29.42, p <0.01], and did not differ signicantly from the saturated model [Dx
2
(2) 2.86,
n.s.]. This implies that the direct effect of baseline performance to performance was signicant, and in
fact it was (b
yx.m
0.40, p <0.01), as was the indirect effect via self-efcacy (b
mx
b
ym.x
0.10,
Sobel 0.10, SE0.04, p <0.01). Moreover, baseline performance evidenced a signicant direct
effect with the self-efcacy mediator (b
mx
), and self-efcacy had a signicant relationship with
performance (b
ym.x
). These ndings are consistent with the hypothesis of a partially mediated effect.
The results of this model, which in effect constitutes the hypothesized model when all three intervening
hypotheses are considered together, are presented in Figure 4.
Summary
The series of model tests illustrated the chain of evidence required for different types of intervening
effects. It is noteworthy that the overall t indices were excellent for all but the directs only, no
directs, and the null latent models. The series of tests made clear that the lack of t stemmed from
signicant relationships between: (1) all three antecedents and the self-efcacy mediator; (2) the
efcacy mediator and the performance outcome; and (3) baseline performance and the performance
outcome, directly. Inclusion of these relations fully accounted for the covariance among the latent
variables. It is also worth noting that the indirect effects tests were signicant for all three antecedents
in both the no directs model, as well as when considered individually in the direct effects models.
Discussion
The purpose of this paper was to revisit the popular question of how do you test mediated
relationships? We submitted that researchers should consider issues related to the theory that they are
testing, the research design that they are employing, and the construct validity of the measures that they
collect. We argued that mediational inferences hinge on the ability of researchers to: (1) justify the
causal order of variables; (2) reasonably exclude the inuence of outside factors; (3) demonstrate
acceptable construct validity of their measures; (4) articulate, a priori, the nature of the intervening
effects that they anticipate; and (5) obtain a pattern of effects that are consistent with their anticipated
relationships while also disconrming alternative hypotheses.
Figure 4. Model results of different intervening effects
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1049
Research design factors are paramount for reasonable mediational inferences to be drawn. If the
causal order of variables is compromised, then it matters little how well the measures perform or the
covariances are partitioned. Because no analytic technique can discern the true causal order of
variables, establishing the internal validity of a study is critical. Adequately ruling out the inuence of
alternative explanations is also vital for drawing mediational inferences. Randomized eld experiments
afford the greatest control over such concerns, yet they may not be feasible for a number of reasons.
Nevertheless, they remain the gold standard and should be pursued whenever possible. Quasi-
experimental designs offer reasonable fall back options, but as Campbell and Stanley (1966) long ago
warned, are fraught with threats to internal validity. Lacking the ability to perform any type of
experiment, temporal precedence, and strong theory offer some bases for specifying causal order, but
they are certainly not the strongest positions to defend. In the end, journal editors, reviewers, and
consumers of research will no doubt have greater condence in studies that leverage strong theory and
experimental design features, reasonable exclusion of alternative explanations for effects, measures
that have good construct validity and were gathered in the proper temporal precedence, and results that
were consistent with the hypothesized relationships.
In our empirical illustration, we justied the causal order of variables using a combination of
techniques. First, an individual difference variable (learning goal orientation) was collected before the
experiment was even introduced. Second, participants completed a practice exercise to familiarize
themselves with the task and to establish a baseline. Third, we then randomly assigned participants to
normative information experimental conditions, after which we assessed their self-efcacy before they
completed the performance trial. We reported conrmatory factor analysis results that supported the
measurement properties of the scales we employed, and then described a series of competing structural
models that homed in on the parameters of interest for different intervening relationships. Given the
strong theoretical foundation concerning self-efcacy, the combination of experimental design
features, temporal precedence, measurement quality, and focused analyses represents a fairly strong
position from which to draw mediational inferences.
We also sought to differentiate indirect effects, partial mediation, and full mediation. Clearly they are
similar in the sense that they all describe an intervening process linking antecedents with an outcome.
However, we submitted that there are important, albeit subtle, differences between the nature of the
relationship that they each advance. Moreover, we argued that different types of conrmatory and
disconrming evidence are warranted for each type of relationships. Most importantly, we argued that
researchers should articulate a priori hypotheses concerning the nature of the relationship(s) that they
anticipate. This underscores the importance of adopting a conrmatory approach toward tests of
intervening effects. As illustrated in the panels of Figure 2, the base model(s) that one chooses presents
important guidelines for the evidential basis of different types of inferences. Moreover, when
considered collectively in a larger structural model, which parameters are included has implications
for tests of indirect and mediated relations. For example, a close examination of the results we reported
will reveal that the magnitude of any given direct and indirect effect varied as a function of what other
parameters were being modeled. In practice, it could well be that the signicance of a given parameter
will change depending on the nature of the entire network of model relations. Therefore, we encourage
researchers to articulate an a priori model (including any potential co-variates of interest), and to report
the parameter estimates for that model. Naturally, a revised model may be suggested by the data; in
which case it is informative to report the parameter estimates from that context as well. Of course,
revised models need to be validated on a new sample.
We provided an empirical illustration of the three types of intervening relationships. In so doing, we
outlined how a series of structural equation models could be employed to test the relevant parameters
for each relationship. This could have just as easily been done using standard multiple regression
techniques. However, SEM techniques offer three critical advantages over multiple regression
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1050 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
approaches. First, using a two-stage SEM approach, researchers explicitly address the measurement
properties of the variables that they have collected before the consideration of the more substantive
relations. Whereas SEM does not absolve researchers from the importance of using valid and reliable
measures, it does explicitly account for howmeasurement properties inuence substantive conclusions.
Second, SEM techniques explicitly consider the potential inuence of constrained parameters. In other
words, SEM model t indices hinge on the veracity relationships thought not to exist. For example, a
hypothesis of full mediation rests not only on the signicance of X!M and M!Y parameters, but
also on whether X fails to relate signicantly to Yonce M has been considered. The third strength of
SEM analyses derives from the nested model comparisons. Whereas sample size and related factors
determine the power of tests of whether a parameter of interest differs signicantly from zero, these are
held relatively constant in the context of nested model tests. In other words, the SEM nested model
comparisons allow one to home in on the specic parameters of interest and to contrast a given pattern
of effects against viable alternatives. Clearly substantive considerations should guide the selection of
alternative models (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988), yet we believe the three types of intervening effects
we described will likely represent fairly viable alternatives for any hypothesis of interest.
While we are clearly echoing previous calls for greater use of SEM techniques in mediational
analyses (Baron & Kenny, 1986; James & Brett, 1984), they are not panaceas. Researchers must still
attend to the preconditions for tests of mediation that we reviewed. Furthermore, the various
comparison models that we advanced are not all directly comparable. Model contrasts are only valuable
if competing models are nested. In other words, models are nested if one represents a more restrictive
version of the other. Whereas both the saturated model and null latent models provide valuable
universal benchmarks, the directs only and no directs models are only useful for limited comparisons.
Nevertheless, the series of model comparisons enable researchers to test all the relevant parameters
related to intervening effects. We should add that simpler approaches such as regression may well be
applied in circumstances where the assumptions of SEMtechniques have not been met (e.g., reasonable
sample sizes).
Extensions
Moderated relationships
Thus far we have been concerned with strictly main effect or linear relationships associated with
various intervening effects. However, interactions or moderator relationships can also be incorporated
into this framework. Both James and Brett (1984) and Baron and Kenny (1986) discussed procedures
for testing both mediators and moderators simultaneously. James and Brett (1984), and more recently,
Muller, Judd, and Yzerbyt (2005), further differentiated different forms that combinations of mediators
and moderators may take. First, they described mediated moderation as the situation where an
interaction between two antecedents, as related to a criterion variable, passes through a mediator. In
effect, this implies that the moderator inuences the X!M link of a mediated relationship. For
example, the inuence of normative information on individuals self-efcacy might be contingent on
the extent to which participants identify with the normative group. In other words, normative
performance information about people just like me is likely to inuence a persons self-efcacy far
more than is information about people much different than me. Tests of this moderation, whether they
be conducted using moderated regression or more sophisticated SEM techniques (Cortina, Chen, &
Dunlap, 2001), would follow the analytic approach that we outlined earlier, while also considering
interactions involving the antecedent variable(s) and the moderator.
The other combination of variables is referred to as moderated mediation (James & Brett, 1984). In
this case, the moderator exerts its inuence on the M!Y path in the X!M!Y sequence. For
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1051
example, individuals self-efcacy is likely to have a stronger relationship with their performance in
weak or unconstrained environments, whereas strong environments or ones with abundant situational
constraints would likely attenuate self-efcacy !performance relations. Here again, the standard
evidential basis for establishing mediated relations is followed; only the moderators inuence on tests
including the M!Y combination is also considered. In short, the difference between the two types of
combinations follows from whether the moderator exerts its inuence on the X!M link (mediated
moderation) or on the M!Y link (moderated mediation) in the X!M!Y sequence.
Multiple mediators
To this point we have been discussing relationships between antecedents and an outcome via a single
mediator. However, there are many circumstances where multiple mediators may be in operation.
There are two different varieties of multiple mediation. The rst instance of multiple mediation simply
involves a longer causal chain such as X!M1 !M2 !Y. For example, self-set goals have long been
considered as a mediating mechanism linking self-efcacy in performance (Bandura & Locke, 2003).
Consequently, the fully mediated relationship between normative information and performance that we
illustrated would be transmitted through a self-efcacy !self-set goals !performance chain.
Whether the relationship between self-efcacy and performance is partially or fully mediated by goals
must be hypothesized and analyzed accordingly, as does the normative information !self-
efcacy !self-set goals sequence. Analytic techniques to address the relative contribution of some
X variable on some distal Y variable as transmitted by two (or more) sequential mediators are still
evolving (see Shrout & Bolger, 2002). Nevertheless, the preconditions for testing mediational type
inferences that we outlined would apply.
The second form of multiple mediation concerns two or more stacked mediators. For example,
Kohler and Mathieu (1993) advanced a model whereby individual resource variables and work related
perceptions where associated with different forms of absenteeism as mediated by three work attitudes
(e.g., job satisfaction) and three forms of work stress (e.g., somatic tensions). These authors considered
the work attitudes and stresses as co-occurring in the sense that they advanced no causal sequence
among them. Kohler and Mathieu (1993) tested mediational relations using a block of mediators
considered together as a set. More recently, Preacher and Hayes (2005) have advanced techniques to
not only assess the extent to which blocks of such mediators convey indirect effects, but also enable
researchers to differentiate the extent to which the collective indirect effects are attributable to each of
the mediators considered.
Multi-level approaches
Throughout this paper we have assumed that all variables of interest were indexed at the same level of
analysis. However, mediational inferences can also be considered in the context of multi-level designs.
Generally, multi-level designs come in two varieties: (1) nested entities; and (2) longitudinal
approaches. In nested entity designs, some focal level-1 of analysis (e.g., individuals) is considered in
the context of higher level-2 units (e.g., teams). In these designs, antecedents and mediators may
emanate from different levels of analysis and combine to inuence a lower-level criterion (see Mathieu
& Taylor, in press). For example, team characteristics (X) may inuence members individual
performances as mediated by level-2 team processes (M) or perhaps by their level-1 identication with
the team.
A second type of multi-level design is commonly referred to as within-subject (Judd, Kenny, &
McClelland, 2001), growth-curve modeling (e.g., Bliese & Polyhart, 2002), or repeated measures (e.g.,
Moskowitz & Hershberger, 2002) designs. In these designs, the lower level-1 variables are represented
as repeated observations of the same unit of analysis (e.g., individual) over time. For example, one
might consider the inuence of level-2 individuals personality traits (X) on their individual level-1
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1052 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
performance overtime (Y), as mediated by their work attitudes. Mediators (M) in this context could be
relatively stable level-2 work attitudes (e.g., organizational commitment) assessed at a single point in
time, or temporally changing level-1 variables (e.g., moods) assessed using time sampling techniques.
Plenty of good work is currently being advanced along these lines (e.g., Judd et al., 2001; Kenny,
Korchmaros, & Bolger, 2003). In summary, multi-level designs expand the scope of mediational
inferences to incorporate relationships that reside within levels of analysis, traverse levels of analysis,
and unfold over time.
Conclusion
Our goal for this paper was to revisit issues related to the validity of mediational inferences in
organizational behavior. We sought to emphasize the inextricable ties between theory, design,
measurement, and analysis related to such inferences. We also argued that indirect effects, partial
mediation, and full mediation represent slightly different forms of intervening effects. We submitted
that researchers should specify which they anticipate a priori, as each relies on slightly different types
of statistical evidence. Our hope is that this paper provides a framework for future investigations. We
also believe that this approach should provide a foundation upon which to expand and incorporate
moderated relationships, more complex multiple mediation applications, and multi-level designs.
Acknowledgements
We thank Gilad Chen, Jodi Goodman, Kris Preacher, Jack Veiga, and Zeki Simsek for their helpful
comments on an earlier version of this paper.
Author biographies
John E. Mathieu (John.Mathieu@business.uconn.edu) is a Professor and Cizik Chair of Management
at the University of Connecticut. He received his PhD in Industrial/Organizational Psychology from
Old Dominion University. He is a member of the Academy of Management and a Fellow of the Society
of Industrial Organizational Psychology, and the American Psychological Association. His current
research interests include models of team and multi-team processes, and cross-level models of
organizational behavior.
Scott R. Taylor (Scott.Taylor@business.uconn.edu) is a PhD candidate in organizational behavior at
the University of Connecticut. He received his MBA from the University of Virginia. He is a student
member of the Academy of Management and Society of Industrial Organizational Psychology. His
current research interests include team leadership and inuence, multi-level models of organizational
behavior, and multi-team systems.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1053
References
Alwin, D. F., & Hauser, R. M. (1975). The decomposition of effects in path analysis, American Sociological
Review, 40, 3747.
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended
two step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411423.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative Self-efcacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 88, 8799.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological
research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychlology,
51, 11731182.
Beauducel, A., & Wittmann, W. (2005). Simulation study on t indices in conrmatory factor analysis based on
data with slightly distorted simple structure. Structural Equation Modeling, 12, 4175.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative t indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238246.
Bliese, P. D., &Ployhart, R. E. (2002). Growth modeling using randomcoefcient models: Model building, testing,
and illustrations. Organizational Research Methods, 5, 362387.
Bollen, K. A. (1987). Total direct and indirect effects in structural equation models. In C. C. Clogg (Ed.),
Sociological methodology (pp. 3769). Washington DC: American Sociological Association.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. Oxford, England: Wiley.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1989). Single sample cross-validation indices for covariance structures. Multi-
variate Behavioral Research, 24, 445455.
Button, S. B., Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1996). Goal orientation in organizational research: A conceptual and
empirical foundation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 2648.
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1966). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago:
Rand McNally.
Chen, G., Gully, S., Whiteman, J.-A., & Kilcullen, R. (2000). Examination of relationships among trait-like
individual differences, state-like individual differences, and learning performance. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 85, 835847.
Claessens, B. J. C., Eerde, W. V., Rutte, C. G., & Roe, R. A. (2004). Planning behavior and perceived control of
time at work. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 937950.
Collins, L. M., Graham, J. W., & Flaherty, B. P. (1998). An alternative framework for dening mediation.
Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 295312.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for eld settings.
Boston: Houghton Mifin.
Cortina, J. M., Chen, G., & Dunlap, W. P. (2001). Testing interaction effects in LISREL: Examination and
illustration of available procedures. Organizational Research Methods, 4, 324360.
Diefendorff, J. M. (2004). Examination of the roles of action-state orientation and goal orientation in the goal-
setting and performance process. Human Performance, 17, 375395.
Fan, X., & Sivo, S. A. (2005). Sensitivity of t indices to misspecied structural or measurement model
components: Rationale of two-index strategy revisited. Structural Equation Modeling, 12, 343367.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology
research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 115134.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldman, G. R. (1980). Work redesign. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Hagtvet, K. A., & Nasser, F. M. (2004). How well do item parcels represent conceptually dened latent constructs?
A two-facet approach. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 168193.
Hall, R. J., Snell, A. F., & Foust, M. S. (1999). Item parceling strategies in SEM: Investigating the subtle effects of
unmodeled secondary constructs. Organizational Research Methods, 2, 233256.
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and
moderators: Examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 65, 599610.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1054 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR
Hom, P. W., & Kinicki, A. J. (2001). Toward a greater understanding of how dissatisfaction drives employee
turnover. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 975987.
Hoyle, R. H., & Kenny, D. A. (1999). Statistical power and tests of mediation. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Statistical
strategies for small sample research. Newbury Park: Sage.
Hu, L., &Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for t indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria
versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155.
James, L. R. (1980). The unmeasured variable problem in path analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 415
421.
James, L. R., & Brett, J. M. (1984). Mediators, moderators, and tests for mediation. Journal of Applied
Psychlology, 69, 307321.
James, L. R., Mulaik, S. A., & Brett, J. M. (1982). Causal analysis: Assumptions, models and data. Newbury Park,
CA: Sage .
James, L. R., Mulaik, S. A., & Brett, J. M. (2006). A tale of two methods. Organizational Research Methods, 9(2),
233244.
Joreskog, K., Sorbom, D., du Toit, S., & du Toit, M. (2000). LISREL 8: New statistical features. Lincolnwood, IL:
Scientic Software International.
Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981). Process Analysis: Estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation
review, 5, 602619.
Judd, C. M., Kenny, D. A., & McClelland, G. H. (2001). Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in
within-subjects designs. Psychological Methods, 6, 115134.
Kenny, D. A. (1979). Correlation and causality. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. In S. F. D. Gilbert, &
G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4 ed., Vol. 1). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Kenny, D. A., Korchmaros, J. D., & Bolger, N. (2003). Lower-level mediation in multilevel models. Psychological
Methods, 8, 115128.
Kohler, S. S., & Mathieu, J. E. (1993). Individual characteristics, work perceptions, and affective reactions
inuences on differentiated absence criteria. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14, 515530.
Landis, R. S., Beal, D. J., & Tesluk, P. E. (2000). A comparison of approaches to forming composites measures in
structural equations models. Organizational Research Methods, 3, 186207.
Lewin, K. (1943). Dening the eld at a given time. Psychological Review, 50, 292310.
MacCallum, R. C., Wegener, D. T., Uchino, B. N., & Fabrigar, L. R. (1993). The problem of equivalent models in
applications of covariance structure analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 185199.
MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding, and
suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173181.
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods
to test the signicance of the mediated effect. Psychological Methods, 7, 83104.
Marsh, H. W., Kit-Tai, H., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on Hypothesis-Testing
Approaches to setting cutoff values for t indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentlers (1999)
ndings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 320341.
Mathieu, J. E. (1991). A cross-level nonrecursive model of the antecedents of organizational commitment and
satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 617618.
Mathieu, J. E., & Button, S. B. (1992). An examination of the relative impact of normative information and self-
efcacy on personal goals and performance over time. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 17581775.
Mayer, R. C., & Gavin, M. B. (2005). Trust in management and performance: Who minds the shop while the
employees watch the boss? Academy of Management Journal, 48, 874888.
Mitchell, T. R. (1985). An evaluation of the validity of correlational research conducted in organizations. Academy
of Management Review, 2, 192205.
Mitchell, T. R., & James, L. R. (2001). Building better theory: Time and the specication of when things happen.
Academy of Management Review, 26, 530547.
Moskowitz, D. S., Hershberger, S. L. (2002). Modeling intraindividual variability with repeated measures data:
Methods and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.
Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When moderation is mediated and mediation is moderated.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 852863.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Pearl, J. (2000). Causality: Models, reasoning and inference. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Phillips, J. M., & Gully, S. (1997). Role of goal orientation, ability, need for achievement, and locus of control in
the self efcacy and goal setting process. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 792802.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
MEDIATIONAL INFERENCES 1055
Potosky, D. (2002). A eld study of computer efcacy beliefs as an outcome of training: The role of computer
playfulness, computer knowledge, and performance during training. Computers in Human Behavior, 18, 241
255.
Potosky, D., & Ramakrishna, H. V. (2002). The moderating role of updating climate perceptions in the
relationship between goal orientation, self-efcacy, and job performance. Human Performance, 15(3), 275
297.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple
mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717731.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2005). SPSS and SAS macros for estimating and comparing indirect effects in
multiple mediator models, 717731 (Under review).
Saks, A. M. (1995). Longitudinal-eld investigation of the moderating and mediating effects of self-efcacy on the
relationship between training and newcomer adjustment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 211225.
Scandura, T. A., & Williams, E. A. (2000). Research methodology in management: Current Practices, trends, and
implications for future research. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 12481264.
Schwab, D. P. (1980). Construct validity in organizational behavior. Research in Organizational Behavior, 2, 343.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for
generalized causal inference. Houghton-Miin, Boston: Houghton-Miin.
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and
recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422445.
Smith, E. R. (1982). Beliefs, attributions, and evaluations: Nonhierarchical models of mediation in social
cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 248259.
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic condence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In
S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why Experiments are often more
effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 89, 845851.
Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efcacy and work related performance: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 124, 240261.
Stezl, I. (1986). Changing the causal hypothesis without changing the t: Some rules for generating equivalent path
models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 21, 309331.
Stone-Romero, E. F., & Rosopa, P. (2004). Inference problems with hierarchical multiple regression-based tests of
mediating effects In J. Martocchio (Ed.), Research in personnel and human resources management (Vol. 23,
pp. 249290). Greenwich, CT: Elsevier.
Sutton, R. I., & Staw, B. M. (1995). What theory is not. Administration Science Quarterly, 40, 371384.
Tett, R. P., & Meyer, J. P. (1993). Job satisfaction, organizational commitment, turnover intention and turnover:
Path analysis based on meta-analytic ndings. Personnel Psychology, 46, 259293.
Weiss, H. M., Suckow, K., & Rakestraw, T. L. (1999). Inuence of modeling on self-set goals: Direct and mediated
effects. Human Performance 12, 89114.
West, S. G., Biesanz, J. C., &Pitts, S. C. (2000). Causal inference and generalization in eld settings: Experimental
and quasi-experimental designs. In H. T. Reis, &C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and
personality psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Whetten, D. A. (1989). What constitutes a theoretical contribution? Academy of Management Review, 14, 490495.
Wood, R. E., Goodman, J. S., Cook, A., & Beckman, N. (in press). Mediation testing in management research: A
review and proposals. Organizational Research Methods.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 27, 10311056 (2006)
1056 J. E. MATHIEU AND S. R. TAYLOR

You might also like