You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Venturing

The role of entrepreneurs in rm-level innovation: Joint effects of positive


affect, creativity, and environmental dynamism
Robert A. Baron a,, Jintong Tang b,1
a
b

Spears School of Business, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74078, United States
Department of Management, John Cook School of Business, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, MO, 63108, United States

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history:
Received 18 February 2009
Received revised form 18 June 2009
Accepted 19 June 2009
Keywords:
Innovation
Affect
Entrepreneurs

a b s t r a c t
Innovation is often a crucial ingredient in new venture success but at present, we know
relatively little about the role of individual entrepreneurs in encouraging its occurrence. The
present research addressed this issue by investigating the joint effects, on rm-level
innovation, of two variables pertaining to entrepreneurs (their creativity and positive affect)
and a key environmental variable (environmental dynamism). Although it has been predicted
that affect plays an important role in entrepreneurship [Baron, R. A., 2008. The role of affect in
the entrepreneurial process. Academy of Management Review 33(2), 328340.], relatively little
empirical evidence on its effects has been obtained to date (e.g., [Foo, M.D. In press. Emotions
and entrepreneurial opportunity evaluation. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice.]). Further,
although the link between creativity and innovation has been described in past literature
[Amabile, T.M., 1988. A model or creativity and innovation in organizations. In B.M. Staw and L.L.
Cummings (Ed.)., Research in organizational behavior 10, 123167.], direct evidence for its
existence is currently lacking. Findings of the present research indicate that positive affect
among founding entrepreneurs is signicantly related to their creativity and that creativity, in
turn, is positively related to rm-level innovation. Both of these relationships are moderated by
environmental dynamism, being stronger in highly dynamic than stable environments.
2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Executive summary
Innovation has often been identied as an essential ingredient in new venture success (e.g., Ireland and Webb, 2007). At present,
however, relatively little direct evidence exists concerning mechanisms through which individual entrepreneurs encourage innovation
in their companies. The present research sought to add to current knowledge of this process by investigating the joint effects, on rmlevel innovation, of two variables pertaining to entrepreneurs (their positive affect and creativity) and a key environmental variable
(environmental dynamism). These variables were selected for inclusion on the basis of a thorough review of prior research and theory,
which strongly suggests their relevance to new venture innovation. Creativity has often been viewed as an antecedent of rm-level
innovation (e.g., Amabile,1996), while positive affect has been found to inuence many aspects of cognition and behavior (e.g., Forgas,
2000; Lyubomirsky et al., 2005), including ones directly relevant to activities entrepreneurs perform in launching new ventures.
Recent theorizing and empirical ndings (e.g., Foo, in press) indicate that these effects may extend to the domain of entrepreneurship
(Baron, 2008). Environmental dynamism, too, has been found to be an important factor in new venture creation (Aldrich, 2000).
The present research tested a model proposing that entrepreneurs' positive affect is related to their creativity and that
creativity, in turn, is related to rm-level innovation. Further, both of these relationships are moderated by environmental
dynamism, being stronger in dynamic than stable environments. Results offered support for all these predictions.

Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 405 744 3325.


E-mail addresses: Robert.baron@OSU.edu (R.A. Baron), jtang3@slu.edu (J. Tang).
1
Tel.: +1 314 977 3811.
0883-9026/$ see front matter 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jbusvent.2009.06.002

50

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

The present ndings offer several important contributions. First, they provide new evidence for a link between entrepreneurs'
creativity and rm-level innovation. In this respect, they help to bridge the gap between individual-level variables such as affect
and creativity and the organizational-level variables (e.g., innovation) which are of major interest to the eld of entrepreneurship.
Second, they provide direct empirical evidence for the role of positive affect in important aspects of entrepreneurship. Such a
relationship has been proposed (Baron, 2008), but to date, only a few studies have investigated it directly (e.g., Foo, in press; Foo
et al., 2009). Third, by providing evidence that environmental dynamism moderates the relationships between positive affect and
creativity, and between creativity and rm-level innovation, the present research reects recommendations by Hitt et al. (2007) to
apply a multi-level approach to efforts to understand complex organizational phenomena. This approach has already gained
growing use in several branches of management (e.g., Korsgaard et al., 2008) but has only recently been adopted in
entrepreneurship research (e.g., Schindehutte and Morris, 2009).
2. Introduction
More than 40 years ago, Baumol (1968, p. 67) noted that: Trying to understand entrepreneurship without the entrepreneur is
like trying to understand Shakespeare without Hamlet. Similarly, Shane et al. (2003, p.259) suggested: The entrepreneurial
process occurs because people act to pursue opportunities . . . Comments such as these indicate that the eld of entrepreneurship
recognizes the important role of entrepreneurs in new venture creation. At the same time, however, the eld focuses major
attention on rm-level outcomesthe survival, growth, and protability of new ventures. This dual focus raises an important
question that has been noted by many researchers (e.g., Baron, 2007; Locke and Baum 2006; Shane et al., 2003): How do the
characteristics and actions of individual entrepreneurs ultimately inuence rm-level outcomes? This is a complex question, and
addressing it fully will require the sophisticated multi-level research described by Hitt et al. (2007)an approach that is
increasingly apparent in current entrepreneurship research (e.g., Hmieleski and Baron, 2009; Schindehutte and Morris, 2009).
We attempt to contribute to this ongoing endeavor by investigating the joint effects on rm-level innovation, of two variables
pertaining to entrepreneurs (creativity and positive affect) and a key environmental variable (environmental dynamism).
Specically, we investigate a model suggesting that entrepreneurs' positive affect is related to their level of creativity and that
creativity, in turn, is related to rm-level innovation. The model further proposes that both of these relationships are moderated by
environmental dynamism, being stronger in highly dynamic than in relatively stable environments. Consistent with literature in
several different elds (e.g., Forgas, 2000; Lyubomirsky et al., 2005), we dene affect as referring to a broad range of subjective
feeling states. Thus, affect encompasses both moods, which are often relatively long-lasting in nature but not focused on specic
events or objects (e.g., cheerfulness, depression), and emotions (e.g., anger, sorrow, joy) which are often shorter in duration and
are more specically directed toward a particular object (e.g., a person, object, event; Frijda, 1993).
Affect refers both to relatively stable, cross-situational tendencies to experience specic subjective feelings (i.e., dispositional
affect) and to temporary shifts in such feelings generated by specic events (i.e., state or event-generated affect). We suggest that
since innovation is a continuing process, it is more likely to reect stable affective dispositions on the part of entrepreneurs than
eeting or temporary affective reactions to specic events. Thus, we focus on stable, dispositional affect in the present research. It
is important to note, however, that although dispositional and state affect derive from different sources (biological processes and
perhaps genetic inuences on the one hand versus discrete external events on the other), a substantial body of research indicates
that they produce parallel effects in many situations (see Lyubomirsky et al., 2005, for a detailed review). Thus, although the
distinction between dispositional and state affect is important, one key goal of the present research (investigating the role of
affect in creativity and rm-level innovation), we suggest that focusing on relatively stable (i.e., dispositional) affect
is appropriate.
The present study offers several contributions. First, and most generally, it seeks to contribute to current efforts to relate the
characteristics of individual entrepreneurs to important rm-level outcomes. Clarifying the mechanisms through which individual
entrepreneurs inuence rm-level outcomes such as innovation is an essential task if the eld of entrepreneurship is to attain full
understanding of the new venture creation process. Second, this research provides empirical evidence on the role of positive affect
in entrepreneurship. Such a role has been predicted on the basis of existing theory and research (Baron, 2008; Lyubomirsky et al.,
2005), but has only recently become a subject of ongoing research in the eld of entrepreneurship (e.g., Foo, in press; Foo et al.,
2009). In the present research, potential relationships between positive affect and important aspects of new venture creation are
empirically examined. Affect has been found to exert strong and consistent effects on many important organizational processes,
ranging from performance appraisals, job interviews, and organizational citizenship, to actual task performance (e.g., Brief and
Weiss, 2002; Isen and Labroo, 2003). Further, it has been suggested (Baron, 2008; Mitchell et al., 2007,) that affect may have
especially strong effects in the domain of entrepreneurship for two important reasons. First, the environments in which
entrepreneurs operate are often highly unpredictable and replete with rapid change. Research on the inuence of affect indicates
that it is precisely in such environments that it exerts its strongest effects on cognition and behavior (e.g., Forgas, 2000; Forgas and
Smith, 2007). Second, affect has been shown to strongly inuence several tasks entrepreneurs perform in launching and operating
new venturesdecision making and judgment, forming productive working relationships with others, persuasion, and eliciting
high levels of enthusiasm in others (e.g., Diener and Seligman, 2002; Forgas, 2000). These considerations, plus growing empirical
evidence for the important role of affect in the entrepreneurial process (e.g., Foo, in press), strongly suggest the usefulness of
further investigations designed to clarify the potential role of affect in key aspects of entrepreneurship.
The present research also offers new insights into the potential link between entrepreneurs' creativity and innovation by new
ventures. Such a relationship has been suggested by many researchers (e.g., Shane, 2003), but here this relationship is investigated

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

51

directly. Finally, by including environmental dynamism as a moderating variable, the present research reects recommendations
by Hitt et al. (2007) to apply a multi-level approach to efforts to understand complex organizational phenomena. This approach
has already gained growing use in several branches of management (e.g., Korsgaard et al., 2008) but has only recently been
adopted in entrepreneurship research (e.g., Schindehutte and Morris, 2009). We believe that the moderated mediation model
proposed here represents a useful application of this approach to acquiring insights into key aspects of entrepreneurship. Next, we
describe the theoretical and empirical foundations of this model and derive specic, testable propositions from it.
3. Theory and hypothesis development
3.1. Positive affect and creativity
Feeling and longing are the motive forces behind all human creations (Albert Einstein, 1930.)
These words, spoken by one of the world's most prominent scientists, suggest that he was well aware of the role of affect (i.e.,
feelings and emotions) in creativity. The suggestion of a link between these variables has been systematically examined in several
elds (e.g., social psychology, organizational behavior; Barsade and Gibson, 2007; Brief and Weiss, 2002; Isen and Baron, 1991) and
in general, ndings indicate that positive moods or feelings (i.e., positive affect) do indeed promote creativity (e.g., Ashby et al.,
1999; Lyubomirsky et al., 2005). However, evidence inconsistent with this general pattern has also been reported (e.g., George
and Zhou, 2002; Kaufmann and Vosburg, 2002), so until recently, a degree of uncertainty remained concerning the generality of
the positive affectcreativity link.
The results of a recent meta-analysis by Baas et al. (2008) have signicantly claried this issue. The ndings of this thorough
review indicate that positive affect enhances creativity overall, but is most likely to produce such effects when it is combined with
high levels of activation (e.g., enthusiasm, elation) and a promotion regulatory focus (a focus on aspirations or accomplishments;
Higgins, 1997, 2006). In contrast, positive affect that is not accompanied by high levels of activation (e.g., feelings of calmness or
serenity) or that is accompanied by a prevention regulatory focus (a focus on safety and avoiding negative outcomes), is much less
likely to promote creativity. Additional evidence indicates that, with a few notable exceptions (e.g., instances in which individuals
interpret negative affect as a sign that additional effort is necessary with respect to creative tasks; George and Zhou, 2002),
negative affect is less clearly related to creativity. Further, negative affect accompanied by high activation (e.g., fear, anxiety) has
been found to be associated with actual reductions in creativity (e.g., Baas et al., 2008).
Overall, then, it appears that the combination of high levels of positive affect and a strong focus on achievement shown by many
entrepreneurs (Baron, 2002, 2008) may contribute to their creativity (e.g., Ward, 2004). Previous research on the relationship between
positive affect and creativity has revealed several mechanisms through which positive affect exerts such effects. First, positive affect
generates increased levels of dopamine in the brain, a neurotransmitter that stimulates brain regions involved in complex cognitive
activity. Dopamine facilitates the ease of switching among alternative cognitive sets, a capacity often identied as one key component of
creativity (e.g., Baas et al., 2008; Dreisbach and Gotschke, 2004). Second, positive affect has been found to broaden cognitive categories
(Fredrickson and Branigan, 2005; Isen and Daubman, 1984), thus increasing the range of items included in such categories. Finally,
positive affect enhances unusual or remote associations, another component of creativity (Isen et al., 1985; Gotschke, 2006). Together,
these effects combine to enhance creativity, which often involves making cognitive connections between previously unrelated ideas or
concepts (e.g., Amabile, 1983, 1988). On the basis of this extensive body of evidence, we propose the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 1. The higher entrepreneurs' stable (i.e., dispositional) positive affect, the greater their level of creativity.
3.2. Creativity and innovation
While a relationship between positive affect and creativity is potentially important, another linkone between creativity and
new venture innovationis, perhaps, even more crucial, i.e., does enhanced creativity among entrepreneurs increase rm-level
innovationactual application of the outcomes generated by creativity? Although little or no direct evidence on this relationship is
currently available, there are empirical and theoretical grounds for its existence.
First, it has often been assumed that creativity is one of the factors that contribute to innovation. For instance, Amabile (1996,
p. 143) notes: All innovation begins with creative ideascreativity by individuals and teams is a starting point for innovation.
(Italics added) In other words, the products of creativitynew ideas, principles, or conceptsserve as the raw materials for
innovation. More generally, creativity is often a necessary condition for subsequent innovations, although not a sufcient one, since
many ideas generated by creativity are not commercially feasible or cannot be developed by the persons who generate them (e.g.,
McMullen and Shepherd, 2006; Ward, 2004).
Second, existing evidence indicates that founding entrepreneurs exert a powerful impact on the cultures of their developing
organizations (e.g., Gartner et al., 1994). Especially in the early period following launch, the founders' values and motives are central to
dening the nature of work environments and to the development of norms governing how the new ventures will actually operate
(e.g., Baum and Locke, 2004). A high level of creativity on the part of founding entrepreneurs may produce an organizational culture
that values both creativity and innovation. Such a culture can be established through the founding entrepreneurs, who demonstrate
creativity in their own behavior, and through their support for creativity and innovation on the part of employees.

52

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

Finally, extant models of innovation have often emphasized the importance of creativity in the development of new and
commercially valuable products or services. For instance, Snow (2007: 101) denes innovation as a new product, service, or
idea, a new process technology, a new business model, thus assigning a central role to, originality), a key aspect of creativity.
Similarly, Leiblein (2007: 142) notes that in order to generate potentially valuable opportunities, organizationsemploy creative
individuals and use ongoing iterative analyses thus generating continuous change in tactics and goals. The role of creativity in
innovation is also emphasized by Shane (2003), who notes that many founding teams engage in various forms of brainstorming
in order to increase the number of new ideas available, and so enhance creativity, which then provides an important foundation for
innovation. Overall, then, there appear to be theoretical grounds for suggesting that positive affect enhances entrepreneurs'
creativity, and that enhanced creativity, in turn, can encourage rm-level innovation. In past research, innovation has often been
measured both in terms of the number of innovations adopted and by radicalness of these innovations (e.g., Smith et al., 2005;
Subramaniam and Youndt, 2005). The reasoning above relates to innovation generally, and so applies to both of these measures.
Thus, we propose the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 2a. The higher entrepreneurs' level of creativity, the greater the number of innovations adopted by their new ventures.
Hypothesis 2b. The higher entrepreneurs' level of creativity, the greater the extent to which these innovations are radical rather
than incremental in nature.
3.3. Potential moderating effects of environmental dynamism
As noted above, there are both empirical and theoretical grounds for suggesting that positive affect among entrepreneurs
enhances creativity and that creativity, in turn, contributes to innovation (e.g., Leiblein, 2007). However, it is crucial to note that
these effects, when they occur, do so against a backdrop of powerful environmental variables. Indeed, as suggested by the multilevel perspective described by Hitt et al. (2007), in order to fully understand complex organizational processes (including new
venture development and growth), it is usually essential to consider variables operating at different levels of analysis (e.g.,
individual, group, subunits, organizations, interorganizational, and environmental). The current study incorporates this
perspective by examining the potential moderating effects of environmental dynamism with respect to the relationship between
positive affect and creativity and the relationship between creativity and innovation.
Dynamism refers to the extent to which environments in which new ventures operate are subject to unpredictable and rapid
changeand thus, to high levels of uncertainty (Dess and Beard, 1984; Miller, 2007). As explained below, there are grounds for
predicting that dynamism moderates both the link between positive affect and creativity and the link between creativity and
innovation, although perhaps through somewhat different underlying mechanisms.
Turning rst to the relationship between positive affect and creativity, the meta-analysis by Baas et al. (2008) indicates that
positive affect enhances creativity primarily when it is accompanied by high levels of activation. This suggests that the link between
positive affect and creativity will be stronger in environments that generate high levels of activation than ones that generate lower
levels of activation. For several reasons, highly dynamic environments are more likely to induce high levels of activation among
entrepreneurs than less dynamic environments. Highly dynamic environments are unpredictable and lled with rapid and dramatic
change; as such, they often involve high levels of uncertainty and risk, plus the necessity for making key decisions on the basis of
incomplete information (e.g., Aldrich, 2000; Miller, 2007). Such conditions have been found to engender high levels of stress (e.g.,
Beehr et al., 2000). and stress, in turn, usually involves high levels of activation (e.g., Stranks, 2005). Thus, for these reasons, levels of
activation among entrepreneurs are likely to be higher in dynamic than stable environments, and consistent with ndings reported
by Baas et al. (2008), these higher levels of activation will lead to a stronger relationship between positive affect and creativity in
dynamic than stable environments. On the basis of this reasoning, we propose the following hypothesis.
Hypothesis 3. Environmental dynamism moderates the relationship between positive affect and creativity, such that this
relationship is stronger in highly dynamic than in more stable environments.
Turning to the link between creativity and innovation, this relationship, too, may be moderated by environmental dynamism.
As noted earlier, existing evidence suggests that positive affect enhances creativity (e.g., Baas et al., 2008) and that creativity, in
turn, plays a key role innovation. Creativity, however, merely provides the raw materials for innovation. In order for the new
ideas generated by creative thinking to be implemented in actual innovations, it is necessary that entrepreneurs be motivated to
consider these ideas carefully and implement the ones that appear most benecial. We suggest that such motivation will often be
higher in dynamic than stable environments, because dynamic environments, by their very nature, virtually require rapid and
effective innovations in products and services (e.g., Eisenhardt, 1989). As Davila et al. (2006) note, in such environments,
innovation is often the key element in achieving rapid growth. Overall then, the link between creativity and innovation will be
stronger in highly dynamic than stable environments, because the environmentally-generated pressures to use the ideas
generated by creative thought will be stronger in dynamic environments. On the basis of this reasoning, we offer the following
hypotheses, which, again, reect two widely used measures of innovation (number of innovations and radicalness of these
innovations).
Hypothesis 4a. Environmental dynamism moderates the relationship between creativity and number of innovations adopted by
new ventures, such that this relationship is stronger in highly dynamic than in more stable environments.

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

53

Hypothesis 4b. Environmental dynamism moderates the relationship between creativity and radicalness of innovations adopted
by new ventures, such that this relationship is stronger in highly dynamic than in more stable environments.
In sum, in this research we propose, and test, a moderated-mediation model of the role of positive affect in entrepreneurship
(e.g., Preacher et al., 2007). This model (represented in Fig. 1) proposes that among founding entrepreneurs, positive affect
promotes creativity, and creativity, in turn, promotes rm-level innovation. Further, the model also suggests that both of these
links are moderated by environmental dynamism, being stronger in highly dynamic than stable environments. Because previous
research suggests that negative affect does not exert consistent effects on creativity, the model focuses only on the potential
inuence of positive affect (e.g., Baas et al., 2008). However, this no way implies that negative affect is unrelated to creativity and
innovation; rather, it merely reects the fact that existing evidence concerning the effects of this variable does not provide a strong
and consistent empirical foundation on which to base theoretical predictions.
4. Research method
4.1. Sample and procedures
Participants were entrepreneurs (i.e., founders or owners who have participated in the startup process of their businesses) in
several Southeastern states of the United States: Alabama, Georgia, Louisiana, Mississippi, and Tennessee. Initially, 154
entrepreneurs were identied by students who interviewed entrepreneurs for a case study assignment in an entrepreneurship
class. An additional 354 entrepreneurs' names and addresses were randomly selected from the Reference USA database. Reference
USA is the equivalent of Dun & Bradstreet database in providing credit information on businesses and corporations. The invitation
letter explained that we sought to survey entrepreneurs on their creativity and rm innovation for a research project. In designing
the survey, we conducted face-to-face interviews (each interview lasted from 40 to 60 minutes) with eight entrepreneurs (located
in the Southeast) to ensure that items included were correctly interpreted by participants.
A total of 500 surveys were mailed, accompanied by self-addressed and stamped return envelopes Of the 500 entrepreneurs, 67
returned the surveys, among which 6 were eliminated because the subjects responded that they were not active in the founding
process. Another 5 were eliminated because the cover letters were mistakenly returned instead of the surveys. Therefore, 56 usable
surveys were generated from the rst round. Two rounds of reminders were sent in January and February of 2007 respectively,
generating another 43 complete responses. Thus, our nal sample size was 99, reecting a 19.8% response rate.
To check for non-response bias, we compared available rm characteristics from the Reference USA database, including rm
size, sales, industry, and year established. The results did not reveal any bias in the sample. The entrepreneurs in our sample were
operating in a wide array of industries from agriculture and manufacturing, to health care and social assistance. Among the 99
entrepreneurs, 80% were male, 3.8% were less than 25 years old, 17.3% were between 25 and 34 years old, 22.1% were 3544 years old,
33.7% were 4554 years old, and 23.1% were over 55 years old. The average business tenure was 11 years, and the average previous
business experience was 14 years. The average rm size was 60 employees and mean sales revenues for 2006 was 12 million dollars.
4.2. Measures
Our data were derived from two sources: replies to a survey and archival industry data. Five-point Likert scales ranging from 1
(stronglydisagree)to5(stronglyagree)wereusedforallsurveyitems.Scoresforindividualitemsoneachscalewereaveragedtoproduce
a single score for each respondent. Established scales, with well-documented reliability and validity, were utilized whenever possible.
4.2.1. Positive affect
Positive affect was measured using the 10-item scale from the PANAS developed and validated by Watson et al. (1988). The
PANAS scale has been used to assess respondents general feelings and emotions (i.e., how they feel on the average) in prior
research (e.g., Barsky et al., 2004; Watson et al., 1988). Although the full PANAS scale gauges both positive and negative affectivity,

Fig. 1. A moderated mediation model of the inuence of positive affect on innovations.

54

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

it has also been adapted to measure each of these dimensions separately. For example, Fritz and Sonnentag (2009) employed the
10 positive affect items in their study of proactive behavior and positive affect. Similarly, Barsky et al. (2004) utilized the 10
negative affect items to investigate the relationship between negative affect and job stress. Because our concern was primarily with
dispositional positive affect, we used the 10-item positive affect scale from the PANAS and asked respondents to indicate the extent
to which they generally (across situations and time) experienced various feelings and emotions (e.g., active, enthusiastic, excited).
The coefcient alpha for positive affectivity scale was .82, which was consistent with the reliability of the scale reported in recent
research (George and Zhou, 2002).
4.2.2. Creativity
A measure of creativity used extensively in creativity research (e.g., Shalley and Perry-Smith, 2001) and modied by PerrySmith (2006), was employed. Respondents were asked to rate the extent to which their work in their companies involved (1) new
ideas and approaches to customer problems; (2) new applications for existing technology; (3) risk-taking; (4) radical new ideas;
and (5) novel long-term vision or applications. The scale demonstrated acceptable reliability (alpha = .74).
4.2.3. Environmental dynamism
Following Boyd (1990), we assessed industry-level environmental dynamism by regressing time against industry sales for the
ve year period of 2002-2006. This time frame is consistent with our survey data, which were collected at the end of 2006, and
asked respondents to report the number of innovations developed in the past 5 years. Data for total industry sales were obtained
from the Fundamental Annual section of the Standard & Poor's Compustat database which provides U.S. and Canadian
fundamental and market information on more than 30,000 active and inactive publicly held companies. The following criteria
were incorporated in the search query. First, the search was limited to only U.S. based companies which are active in the database.
Second, the search range included All companies which resulted in the search of the entire database. Third, the search variables
requested included the company's North American Industry Classication Code (NAICS), the Data Year, the Company Name, and
the Total Revenue/Sales for each company. Annual data for the ve-year (2002 to 2006) period were obtained following the above
procedures. Next, the total sales were summed for each industry category across the 5 years by groupings based on the rst two
digits (which denote the top level industry classications) of the NAICS codes. As a result, cumulative sales gures for each top
level industry classication across the ve year period were acquired. The relevant industry code for each rm in our sample was
obtained from the Reference USA database.
To conduct the regressions, time (20022006) was entered as independent variables and annual sales as dependent variables for
each industry category according to the NAICS code. Then, the standard errors of the regression coefcients were divided by the mean
sales values of the 5 years. The result was used as the measure of industry-level environmental dynamism in our study, and reects the
extent to which sales were dynamic (i.e., changing) in each industry. This approach has been used as an archival measure of dynamism
in several previous studies (e.g., Hmieleski and Baron, 2009). The mean of the environmental dynamism variable was 1.24, indicating
moderate levels of change in average sales volumes. Because the raw dynamism data were right-skewed (skewness statistic = 12.08),
natural logs were employed. As a result, the skewness statistic was reduced to 1.61; the resulting mean of the natural logs was .01.
Innovation. Innovation has been measured through new product introductions (Katila, 2002). Thus, in our study, the construct of
innovation was measured with two indicators of such introductions. First, the entrepreneurs were asked how many new products or
services has your rm introduced in the past 5 years? This item was used to obtain data on the number of innovations developed by
new ventures. This measure of innovation was found to be robust over a wide array of research settings (Damanpour,1991; Smith et al.,
2005). To measure the extent to which these innovations were radical (i.e., innovations that make the organization's current product or
service lines obsolete) rather than incremental in nature, a three-item scale developed by Subramaniam and Youndt (2005) was used.
This measure of innovations was based on previous research by Tushman and Anderson (1986) and Henderson and Clark (1990).
Respondents were asked to rate how radical each innovation was in terms of (1) making present products obsolete, (2) fundamentally
changing current products or services, and (3) making existing expertise in prevailing products/service obsolete (alpha = .87). These
three items were averaged to obtain the score for radical innovations. 2
4.2.4. Control variables
A total of 6 control variables were included. At the individual level, entrepreneurs' age, gender (female = 0; male = 1), previous
experience, and business tenure were controlled because they have been found to exert confounding effects on rm performance
(Reynolds, 2000). Entrepreneurs' age was measured with 5 categories: (1) b 25; (2) 2534; (3) 3544; (4) 4554; and (5) N
55 years old. Previous experience was measured with a 3-item scale that asked respondents to report the number of years of
previous managerial, entrepreneurial, and industrial experiences (alpha = .90). Business tenure was measured by means of the
number of years the respondent has been working in the current business. At the rm level, previous research has shown that rm
size and sales revenues inuence innovation and other aspects of entrepreneurial development (Baron and Tang, 2009; Ucbasaran
et al., 2009). Firm size was measured by the number of current employees in the rm. Entrepreneurs were also asked to report the
2
As noted by a reviewer, one of the items used to measure creativity referred to the extent to which the entrepreneurs' work involved radical new ideas, and
one measure of innovation referred to the extent to which innovations were radical in nature. To assess the conceptual clarity and distinctiveness of these
constructs (creativity, innovation), we performed additional analyses in which the creativity item was allowed to cross-load on the radicalness of innovation
construct. Results indicated that the creativity item loaded only.06 on innovation, but .89 on creativity. Thus, it appears that the two constructs are indeed
distinct, as suggested by the results of a conrmatory factor analysis (reported below).

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

55

sales revenues of year 2006. For previous experience, business tenure, rm size, and sales revenues, natural logs were used to
account for skewed distribution within the data.
4.3. Discriminant validity
We examined the discriminant validity of our variables prior to hypothesis testing. Although creativity and innovation are
theoretically distinguishable constructs, we conducted conrmatory factor analyses (CFA) and calculated the square root of the
average variance extracted (AVE) for each variable to empirically evaluate discriminant validity.
To conduct CFA, we rst formed a one-factor and a two-factor model. Then we examined the model t of each and tested chisquare differences to determine which model provided better t to the data. Results showed that the 2-factor model (reecting
creativity and innovation) t the data very well (CFI = .90, NFI = .90, GFI = .90, AGFI = .80, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .09, 2/df = 3.30)
while the one-factor model did not (CFI = .72, NFI = .69, GFI = .72, AGFI = .49, RMSEA = .24, SRMR = .19, 2/df = 8.23) (Hu and
Bentler, 1999). The chi-square difference test between the two models (2difference = 101.32, df = 1, p b .001) was signicant,
favoring the 2-factor model. In addition, in the 2-factor model, all items signicantly loaded on their respective latent variables.
Next, we calculated the square root of the average variance extracted for each of the 2 variables (Fornell and Larcker, 1981).
According to the AVE method, constructs are different if the average variance extracted (AVE) for one construct is greater than their
shared variance, i.e., the square root of the average variance extracted (AVE) for a given construct should be greater than the
absolute values of the standardized correlations of the given construct with any other construct in the analysis. Results indicated
that the square root of the AVE values (.64 for creativity, and .90 for radicalness of innovations) were greater than all corresponding
zero-order correlations. Together, the CFA and AVE results indicated that creativity and (radical) innovation were indeed distinct
variables, thus suggesting satisfactory discriminant validity.
4.4. Tests for common method variance
In order to check the presence of common method variance, we adopted Harman's one-factor test (Podsakoff et al., 2003). We
entered all the self-reported variables in an exploratory factor analysis with principal axis factoring and varimax rotation. Five
factors emerged with eigenvalues greater than 1 and 52.67% of the variance explained. No single factor was dominant with the rst
factor accounting for 17.35% of the variance. Thus, no common method variance was detected in this study.
5. Results
Table 1 summarizes the means, standard deviations, and correlations for all variables. As indicated in Table 1, entrepreneurs'
positive affect is positively associated with creativity, and creativity is positively related to both number of innovations and
radicalness of these innovations.
Tables 2 and 3 present the results of the moderated mediation analyses. Hypotheses were tested using hierarchical regression
analysis. After the mean-centering technique had been used, the variance ination factor (VIF) estimates (1.23.4) for all variables
in the full models suggested that multicollinearity did not pose a serious problem (Barringer and Bluedorn, 1999).
5.1. Tests of hypotheses
Hypothesis 1 predicts that entrepreneurs' dispositional (i.e., stable) positive affect is positively related to their creativity.
Results relevant to this hypothesis are presented in Model 2 of Table 2 and indicate that, as predicted, positive affect is signicantly
related to creativity (B = .79, p b .001). Hypothesis 2a predicts that creativity is positively related to the number of innovations by
entrepreneurial ventures, while Hypothesis 2b predicts that creativity is positively related to the radicalness of these innovations.
Results (Model 11 of Table 3) provide support for H2b: creativity is signicantly related to the radicalness of innovations (B = .45,

Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and correlations.

1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.

Variables

Mean

S.D.

Entrepreneurs' Age
Gender
Tenurea
Experiencea
Firm sizea
Firm salesa
PAb
Creativity
Dynamism
# of innovations
Radicalness of innovations

3.53
.80
.84
1.10
1.01
5.81
4.17
3.98
.01
1.36
2.22

1.19
.43
.51
.34
.70
1.60
.49
.82
.01
1.94
.98

N = 99 Logarithm

1
.07
.61
.70
.41
.10
.04
.17
.14
.10
.20

PA = Positive Affect p b .05 p b .01.

.11
.09
.05
.22
.15
.16
.02
.12
.22

.69
.51
.15
.05
.23
.16
.19
.20

.54
.21
.03
.19
.00
.26

.46
.00
.10
.10
.32

.14

.13

10

.37
.15
.05
.24
.24

.38
.07
.20
.21

.16
.22
.27

.19
.23

.47

56

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

Table 2
Results of the moderation effect of positive affect and environmental dynamism on creativity.
Variables

Creativity

Age
Gender
Tenure a
Experience a
Firm size a
Firm sales a
PA b
Dynamism
PA dynamism
R2
Adjusted R2
R2
F

Model 1

Model 2

Model 3

Model 4

.19 (.17)
.09 (.11)
.88(.39)
.12 (.71)
.15 (.20)
.06 (.08)

.06 (.16)
.10 (.10)
.92 (.34)
.14 (.63)
.28 (.18)
.06 (.07)
.79 (.20)

.12 (.15)
.06 (.09)
.53 (.37)
.25 (.62)
.32 (.17)
.09 (.07)
.86 (.20)
7.10 (2.97)

.09 (.15)
.08 (.09)
.53 (.35)
.13 (.60)
.38 (.17)
.13 (.07)
.73 (.20)
5.94 (2.92)
12.09 (5.98)
.51
.41
.05
4.09

.19
.09
.19
1.84

.39
.30
.20
14.62

.46
.36
.07
5.74

Note: Unstandardized coefcients are displayed in the table with standard errors in parentheses. p b .05 p b .01 p b .001.
a
Logarithm.
b
PA = Positive Affect.

p b .05). However, results do not provide support for H2a (Model 5 of Table 3): creativity is not signicantly related to number of
innovations reported by entrepreneurs (B = .50, n.s.).
Hypothesis 3 proposes a moderating effect of environmental dynamism on the relationship between positive affect and
creativity. As shown in Model 4 of Table 2, results offer support for Hypothesis 3 (B of the interaction between positive affect and
dynamism = 12.09, p b .05). Consistent with predictions, the link between positive affect and creativity is indeed stronger in the
presence of high than low dynamism.
Hypothesis 4a predicts a moderating role of dynamism with respect to the relationship between creativity and number of
innovations, while Hypothesis 4b predicts a moderating role of dynamism with respect to the relationship between creativity and

Table 3
Results of the moderated mediation analysis.
Variables

Age
Gender
Tenure a
Experience a
Firm size a
Firm sales a

Number of innovations

Radicalness of innovations

Model 1

Model 2

Model 3

Model 4

Model 5

Model 6

Model 7

Model 8

Model 9

Model 10

Model 11

Model 12

.31
(.40)
.21
(.26)
1.67
(.90)
3.03
(1.63)
1.60

.55
(.38)
.21
(.24)
1.73
(.84)
3.51
(1.54)
.1.82

.47
(.39)
.25
(.24)
1.23
(.94)
2.99
(1.59)
1.87

.46
(.39)
.25
(.24)
1.22
(.95)
2.98
(1.61)
1.86

.51
(.39)
.29
(.24)
.96
(.96)
3.04
(1.60)
1.67

.52
(.40)
.29
(.25)
.97
(.97)
3.08
(1.62)
1.72

(.47)
.02
(.17)

(.44)
..23
(.18)
1.38
(.50)

(.44)
.27
(.18)
1.49
(.50)
9.21
(7.58)

(.46)
.26
(.19)
1.51
(.54)
9.36
(7.82)
1.63
(16.00)

(.48)
.20
(.20)
1.14
(.61)
6.37
(8.13)
7.72
(16.64)
.50
(.40)

.08
(.23)
.12
(.15)
.77
(.52)
1.17
(.94)
.24
(.27)
.05
(.11)
.53
(.28)

.19
(.22)
.06
(.14)
.09
(.54)
.48
(.91)
.31
(.25)
.01
(.10)
.56
(.29)
12.40
(4.34)

.15
(.22)
.08
(.14)
.10
(.53)
.63
(.89)
.39
(.25)
.06
(.11)
.41
(.30)
10.95
(4.34)
15.03

.11
(.21)
.05
(.13)
.14
(.52)
.69
(.86)
.21
(.26)
.00
(.10)
.08
(.32)
8.27
(4.38)
9.57
(8.96)
.45
(.22)

.45
.34
.00
.01

.47
.34
.02
1.54

(.50)
.23
(.21)
1.24
(.66)
6.76
(8.26)
4.31
(18.37)
.45
(.42)
3.88
(8.45)
.47
.33
.00
.21

.15
(.23)
.12
(.15)
.76
(.52)
1.02
(.95)
.17
(.27)
.11
(.10)

.14
(.20)
.05
(.13)
.16
(.50)
.59
(.83)
.09
(.25)
.07
(.11)
.17
(.34)
7.38
(4.24)
1.67
(9.43)
.57
(.22)
9.00
(4.34)
.50
.37
.05
4.31

PA b
Dynamism
PA dynamism
Creativity
Creativity dynamism
R2
Adjusted R2
R2
F

.34
.25
.34
3.86

.43
.34
.09
7.72

.45
.35
.02
1.48

(8.88)

.20
.10
.20
1.96

Note: Unstandardized coefcients are displayed in the table with standard errors in parentheses.
a
Logarithm.
b
PA = Positive Affect.
p b .05.
p b .01.
p b .001.

.24
.12
.03
3.47

.36
.24
.20
8.15

.40
.27
.04
2.86

.45
.32
.06
4.28

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

57

radicalness of innovations. Results offer support for H4b (Model 12 of Table 3): dynamism positively moderates the relationship
between creativity and radicalness of innovations (B of the interaction between creativity and dynamism = 9.00, p b .05). However,
results do not offer support for H4a: dynamism did not moderate the relationship between creativity and number of innovations
(Model 6 of Table 3; B = 3.88, n.s.).
5.2. Mediating role of creativity
To test the proposal that dynamism-moderated creativity mediates the relationship between positive affect and radicalness of
innovations, we adopted the procedures developed by Baron and Kenny (1986). According to the logic of this procedure, mediation
is suggested if the following conditions are met: (a) the independent variable is a signicant predictor of both the dependent
variable and the mediator, (b) the mediator is a signicant predictor of the dependent variable, and (c) the effects of the
independent variable on the dependent variable are reduced when the mediating variable is added to the regression equation. Full
mediation is indicated if the effect of the independent variable is no longer signicant when the mediating variable is added,
whereas partial mediation is suggested if the effect of the independent variable is reduced but remains signicant.
We rst examined the relationship between the independent variable and dependent variable as well as the relationship between
the independent variable and the mediator. As shown in Model 10 of Table 3, dynamism-moderated positive affect was signicantly
related to the radicalness of innovations (B= 15.03, p b .05). As shown in Table 2, a signicant relationship exists between dynamismmoderated positive affect and creativity (B= 12.09, p b .05). Second, dynamism-moderated creativity is signicantly associated with
the radicalness of innovations (B= 9.00, p b .05) as indicated in Model 12 of Table 3. Third and nally, as Models 10 and 12 in Table 3
demonstrate, the coefcient for the dynamism-moderated effects of positive affect on radicalness of innovations became insignicant
when the dynamism-moderated effects of creativity were included in the regression equation. The coefcient decreased from 15.03
(pb .05 in Model 10) to 1.67 (n.s. in Model 12). Thus, dynamism-moderated creativity fully mediates the positive relationship
between dynamism-moderated positive affect and radicalness of innovations in the present data.
Further evidence of full mediation was obtained by the Sobel test. Sobel (1982) tests calculate the magnitude of the
unstandardized indirect effect and its associated standard error. The ratio of the indirect effect over its standard error is referred to
as the Sobel statistic, which is compared to a z distribution to determine the statistical signicance of the indirect effect. The Sobel
tests indicated that the indirect effect of dynamism-moderated positive affect on radicalness of innovations (Sobel statistic = 1.20,
p b .05) was in the anticipated direction and statistically signicant, providing further evidence for full mediation.
6. Discussion
6.1. Key ndings and implications
The present research investigated the relationships between two individual-level variables (entrepreneurs' positive affect and
creativity), and rm-level innovation. Both individual-level variables (i.e., positive affect; creativity) were chosen on the basis of
extensive evidence indicating that they are relevant to tasks performed by entrepreneursactivities that strongly shape new venture
performance (e.g., Baron, 2007). Further, creativity has often been identied as an essential antecedent of innovation (e.g., Amabile,
1996), while positive affect has been found to inuence many aspects of cognition and behavior (e.g., Cohen, 2005; Forgas, 2000; Isen
and Labroo, 2003). On the basis of such evidence, it has recently been suggested that affect may also play an important role in
entrepreneurship (Baron, 2008), inuencing entrepreneurs decisions, their capacity to recognize or create opportunities, the size and
quality of their networks, and their motivation and perseverance in the face of adversity (Barsade and Gibson, 2007).
The present research provides empirical data relating to these proposals. Consistent with a large body of previous evidence
(e.g., Baas et al., 2008), results indicate that entrepreneurs positive affect is signicantly related to their creativity. Further,
enhanced creativity was found to be signicantly related to one measure of new venture innovation (the radicalness of such
innovations). Overall, these ndings indicate that positive affect is related to important aspects of entrepreneurshipa nding
consistent with other results reported recently in the entrepreneurship literature (e.g., Foo, in press). However, such effects are not
direct in nature; rather, they are mediated by intervening variables. Specically, in the present research, creativity was found to
mediate the relationship between entrepreneurs' positive affect and rm-level innovation.
The results of the present research also indicate that both the relationship between positive affect and creativity, and the
relationship between creativity and innovation, are moderated by environmental dynamism. Both relationships were stronger in
highly dynamic environments than in more stable ones. Although the present research was not specically designed to explicate
the mechanisms underlying these moderating effects, both were predicted on the basis of previous research. With respect to the
link between positive affect and creativity, it was reasoned that highly dynamic environments are more likely to generate high
levels of activation among entrepreneurs, a condition found, in a recent meta-analysis (Baas et al., 2008) to be required for positive
affect to enhance creativity. Turning to the relationship between creativity and rm-level innovation, it was reasoned that
innovation is more important in highly dynamic environments than in stable environments (e.g., Ireland and Webb, 2007) and
that as a result, the ideas generated by creative thinking are more likely to be carefully considered and actually implemented
in dynamic than stable environments. Thus, the relationship between creativity and innovation will be stronger in such
environments. Further research is clearly required to clarify the precise mechanisms through which environmental dynamism
moderates the relationships between affect and creativity, and creativity and innovation, but evidence for such moderation is
informative in and of itself.

58

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

At this point, the fact that positive affect was signicantly related to radicalness of innovations but not to their overall number
(as reported by entrepreneurs), should be addressed. One possible explanation for these ndings is that in reporting the number of
innovations in their new ventures individual entrepreneurs applied highly diverse criteria. Some may have included only major
innovations, while others may have included incremental innovations in their tally. The usefulness of this measure may be readily
enhanced in future research through the inclusion of additional clarifying criteria concerning what should and should not be
included by entrepreneurs in their tallies of innovations.
6.2. Limitations and suggestions for future research
Although the results of the present research are consistent with the ndings of many previous studies (e.g., Baas et al., 2008),
they are subject to several limitations that should be carefully addressed in future research. First, several of the measures employed
were self-report in nature (e.g., creativity, innovation). Although these measures were based on ones used in previous research and
have been shown to possess acceptable reliability and validity, it is clear that the constructs of primary interest (creativity,
innovation) are complex. Thus, replication of the present research with additional measures of these variables is necessary before
the present results can be accepted with condence.
Second, although vigorous efforts were made to obtain a broad sample of entrepreneurs, all participants in the present research
operated new ventures in one region of the United States. Further, although these new businesses were located in a wide range of
industries, it is possible that the sample was not truly representative of entrepreneurs in other industries. Additional data, gathered
in other geographic locationsand perhaps outside North Americaare necessary to fully evaluate the extent to which the present
ndings can be generalized to other groups of entrepreneurs operating in environments and contexts different from the ones
included here.
Third, although the present data set was carefully designed to assess the key variables of interest, it did not examine other
potential mediators of the relationship between affect and innovation aside from creativity. Although results indicate that
creativity fully mediated the relationship between positive affect and innovation in the present research, positive affect may also
inuence innovation through other mechanisms not specically investigated here (e.g., the optimistic bias, which may be
facilitated by high levels of positive affect; e.g., Busenitz and Barney, 1997; Forgas, 2000). Future research should investigate the
role of other potential mediators of the relationships between positive affect and rm-level variables such as innovation.
An additional limitation involves use of the PANAS as the measure of affect. Although this measure has been used in many
previous studies and has been found to have acceptable reliability and validity (e.g., Watson et al., 1988), researchers who
investigate affect have called attention to the fact that the PANAS primarily measures affective states that are high in activation.
Specically, the negative affect (NA) dimension represents the experience of negative affect that is high in activation (e.g., feelings
of nervousness, guilt, anger), while the positive affect (PA) dimension represents the experience of positive affect high in activation
(e.g., feelings of excitement, enthusiasm; Watson and Clark, 1997). Low activation experiences (both negative and positive) are not
included (e.g., bored and sleepy are examples of low activation negative affect; relaxed and calm are examples of low activation
positive affect; e.g., Barrett, 2006). Future research should address this important issue through the use of measures that have been
designed to assess both the valence (positive, negative) and activation dimensions of affect (e.g., Van Katwyk et al., 2000).
Further studies should also examine the potential role of affect in other aspects of the entrepreneurial process. For instance,
does affect play a role in what Kirzner (1979) terms alertnessthe capacity to recognize emerging opportunities? Research on
affect (e.g., Fredrickson and Branigan, 2005; Isen and Daubman, 1984) suggests that positive affect might increase alertness by
enhancing entrepreneurs capacity to perceive opportunities in complex patterns of environmental change (e.g., changes in
demographics, technology, government policies, markets, etc.; Baron, 2006). Investigating the relationship between alertness and
affect might provide further insights into the basic nature of alertness (e.g., its foundations in basic perceptual and cognitive
processes; Mitchell et al., 2007).
Finally, future research should also examine the role of affect in other aspects of entrepreneurs' behavior and cognitionfor
instance the goals that they set for themselves and their new ventures (e.g., Locke and Latham, 2005 ) and their capacity for
tolerating the high levels of stress to which they are exposed (e.g., Beehr et al., 2000). Previous research suggests that affect is
strongly related to these and other reactions, so investigating its potential effects on these processes in the domain of
entrepreneurship would appear to be an important and potentially valuable task.
6.3. Contributions and conclusions
Despite these limitations, the present ndings offer several useful contributions. First, they help clarify the potential role of
affect in entrepreneurship. As noted earlier, such a role has been predicted, but to date, only a small number of studies have
examined such effects (e.g., Foo, in press; Foo et al., 2009). Second, the present results provide empirical evidence for a link
between creativity and innovation. Such a link has often been suggested in past literature (Amabile, 1988), but at present, relatively
little direct evidence for it exists.
Second, the present ndings add to our understanding of the complex processes through which individual-level variables such
as creativity and positive affect ultimately inuence organizational-level outcomes such as innovation. Attaining greater
understanding of these processes t has been identied as an important task by many researchers in the eld of entrepreneurship
(e.g., Baum and Locke, 2004). The present ndings contribute to progress on this task by suggesting mechanisms through which
two important individual-level variablespositive affect and creativitycan inuence important activities by new ventures.

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

59

From a more general perspective, results serve to provide a more balanced view of the potential inuence of positive affect than
extant literature in several different elds has sometimes suggested. In the past, positive affect has frequently been viewed
primarily as a source of error or biasa factor that interferes with basic cognitive processes such as judgment and decision making
and hence exerts detrimental effects on important organizational processes (e.g., Brief and Weiss, 2002; Isen and Baron, 1991).
More recent ndings, however, indicate that affectand especially positive affectmay confer important benets on the persons
who experience it. For instance, on the basis of an extensive review of existing evidence, Lyubomirsky et al. (2005: 804) conclude
that: People who experience a preponderance of positive emotions tend to be successful and accomplished across multiple life
domainsnot merely because success leads to happiness, but because positive affect engenders success (Italics added.) The present
ndings extend these conclusions to the realm of entrepreneurship, suggesting that positive affect among entrepreneurs may
encourage creativity and, indirectly, rm-level innovation. Since innovation, in turn, is often a key ingredient in new venture
success, the tendency to experience positive feelings or emotions frequently, and in a wide range of contexts, may indeed confer
important advantages on entrepreneurs and the new ventures they launch.

References
Aldrich, H., 2000. Organizations evolving. Sage, London.
Amabile, T.M., 1983. The social psychology of creativity: a componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43, 357376.
Amabile, T.M., 1988. In: Staw, B.M., Cummings, L.L. (Eds.), A model or creativity and innovation in organizations: Research in organizational behavior, vol. 10,
pp. 123167.
Amabile, T.M., 1996. Creativity in context. Westview Press, Boulder, Colorado.
Ashby, F.G., Isen, A.M., Turken, A.U., 1999. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its inuence on cognition. Psychological Review 106, 529550.
Baas, M., De Dreu, C.K.W., Nijstad, B.A., 2008. A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological
Bulletin 134, 779806.
Baron, R.A., 2002. OB and entrepreneurship: the reciprocal benets of closer conceptual links. In: Staw, B.M., Kramer, R. (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior.
JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, pp. 225269.
Baron, R.A., 2006. Opportunity recognition as pattern recognition: how entrepreneurs connect the dots to identify new business opportunities. Academy of
Management Perspectives 20 (1), 104119.
Baron, R.A., 2007. Cognition and affect: invaluable tools for answering why, how and what questions about entrepreneurs and the entrepreneurial process.
In: Minitti, M., Minniti, M., Zacharakis, A., Spinelli, S. (Eds.), Entrepreneurship. Praeger, Wesport, Ct, pp. 2140.
Baron, R.A., 2008. The role of affect in the entrepreneurial process. Academy of Management Review 33 (2), 328340.
Baron, R.M., Kenny, D.A., 1986. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51, 11731182.
Baron, R.A., Tang, J., 2009. Entrepreneurs social skills and new venture performance: mediating mechanisms and cultural generality. Journal of Management 35 (2),
282306.
Barrett, L.F., 2006. Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10, 2046.
Barringer, B., Bluedorn, A., 1999. The relationship between corporate entrepreneurship and strategic management. Strategic Management Journal 20 (5), 421444.
Barsade, S.G., Gibson, D.E., 2007. Why does affect matter in organizations? Academy of Management Perspectives 21, 3659.
Barsky, A., Thoresen, C.J., Warren, C.R., Kaplan, S.A., 2004. Modeling negative affectivity and job stress: a contingency-based approach. Journal of Organizational
Behavior 25 (8), 915936.
Baum, J.R., Locke, E.A., 2004. The relationship of entrepreneurial traits, skill, and motivation to subsequent venture growth. Journal of Applied Psychology 89 (4),
587598.
Baumol, W., 1968. Entrepreneurship in economic theory. American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings, pp. 6471.
Beehr, T.A., Jex, S.M., Stacy, B.A., Murray, M.A., 2000. Work stressors and coworker support as predictors of individual strain and job performance. Journal of
Organizational Behavior 21, 391405.
Boyd, B., 1990. Corporate linkages and organizational environments: a test of the resource dependence model. Strategic Management Journal 11, 419430.
Brief, A.P., Weiss, H.M., 2002. Organizational behavior: affect in the workplace. Annual Review of Psychology 53 (1), 279307.
Busenitz, L., Barney, J.B., 1997. Differences between entrepreneurs and managers in large organizations: biases and heuristics in strategic decision-making. Journal
of Business Venturing 12, 930.
Cohen, J.D., 2005. The vulcanization of the human brain: a neural perspective on interactions between cognition and emotion. Journal of Economic Perspectives 19,
324.
Damanpour, F., 1991. Organizational innovation: a meta-analysis of effects of determinants and moderators. Academy of Management Journal 34, 555590.
Davila, T., Epstein, J., Shelton, R., 2006. Making innovation work: how to manage it, measure it, and prot from it. Wharton School Publishing, Upper Saddle River.
Dess, G.G., Beard, D.W., 1984. Dimensions of organizational task environments. Administrative Science Quarterly 29, 5273.
Diener, E., Seligman, M.E.P., 2002. Very happy people. Psychological science 13 (1), 8184.
Dreisbach, G., Gotschke, T., 2004. How positive affect mediates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition 30, 343353.
Eisenhardt, K.M., 1989. Making fast strategic decisions in high-velocity environments. Academy of Management Journal 32 (3), 543576.
Foo, M.D., in press. Emotions and entrepreneurial opportunity evaluation. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1360905.
Foo, M.D., Uy, M.A., Baron, R.A., 2009. How do feelings inuence effort: an empirical study of entrepreneurs affect and venture effort. Journal of Applied Psychology 94,
10861094.
Forgas, J.P., 2000. Feeling and thinking: affective inuences on social cognition. Cambridge University Press, New York.
Forgas, J.P., Smith, C.A., 2007. Affect and emotion. In: Hogg, M., Cooper, J. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of social psychology: Concise student edition. Sage, Thousand
Oaks, pp. 146175.
Fornell, C., Larcker, D., 1981. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research 18 (1),
3950.
Fredrickson, B.L., Branigan, C., 2005. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion 19, 313332.
Frijda, N.H., 1993. Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In: Lewis, M., Haviland, J.M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 381403.
Fritz, C., Sonnentag, S., 2009. Antecedents of day-level proactive behavior: a look at job stressors and positive affect during the workday. Journal of Management 35
(1), 94111.
Gartner, W., Shaver, K., Gatewood, E., Katz, J., 1994. Finding the entrepreneur in entrepreneurship. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice 18, 59.
George, J.M., Zhou, J., 2002. Dual tuning in a supportive context: joint contributions of positive mood, negative mood, and supervisory behaviors to employee
creativity. Academy of Management Journal 50 (3), 605622.
Gotschke, T., 2006. Affect modulation of selective attention and background-monitoring. Invited talk presented at the Experimenteel Psychologische
Onderzoeckschool (EPOS) symposium: Affective modulation of cognitive performance. Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands.

60

R.A. Baron, J. Tang / Journal of Business Venturing 26 (2011) 4960

Henderson, R., Clark, K.B., 1990. Architectural innovation: the reconguration of existing product technologies and the failure of established rms. Administrative
Science Quarterly 35, 930.
Higgins, E.T., 1997. Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist 52, 12801300.
Higgins, E.T., 2006. Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychological Review 113, 439460.
Hitt, M.A., Beamish, P.W., Jackson, S.E., Mathieu, J.E., 2007. Building theoretical and empirical bridges across level: multilevel research in management. Academy of
Management Journal 50 (6), 13851399.
Hmieleski, K, M., Baron, R.A., 2009. Entrepreneurs optimism and new venture performance: a social cognitive perspective. Academy of Management Journal 52,
473488.
Hu, L., Bentler, P.M., 1999. Cutoff criteria for t indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternative. Structure Equation Modeling 6,
155.
Ireland, R.D., Webb, J.W., 2007. Strategic entrepreneurship: creating competitive advantage through streams of innovation. Business Horizons 50, 4959.
Isen, A.M., Baron, R.A., 1991. Positive affect as a factor in organizational behavior. In: Staw, B.M., Cummings, L.L. (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior. JAI Press,
Greenwich, CT, pp. 153.
Isen, A.M., Daubman, K.A., 1984. The inuence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47, 12061217.
Isen, A.M., Labroo, A.A., 2003. Some ways in which positive affect facilitates decision making and judgment. In: Schneider, S., Shanteau, J. (Eds.), Emerging
perspectives on judgment and decision research. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 365393.
Isen, A.M., Johnson, M.M., Mertz, E., Robinson, G.F., 1985. The inuence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 48, 14131426.
Katila, R., 2002. New products search over time: past ideas in their prime? Academy of Management Journal 45 (5), 9951010.
Kaufmann, G., Vosburg, S.K., 2002. The effects of mood on early and late idea production. Creativity Research Journal 14, 317330.
Kirzner, I.M., 1979. Perception, opportunity, and prot. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Korsgaard, M.A., Jeong, S.S., Mahony, D.M., Pitariu, A., 2008. A multilevel view of intragroup conict. Journal of Management 34, 12221252.
Leiblein, M.J., 2007. Environment, organization, and innovation: how entrepreneurial decisions affect innovative success. Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal 1, 141144.
Locke, E.A., Baum, J.R., 2006. Entrepreneurial motivation. In: Baum, J.R., Frese, M., Baron, R.A. (Eds.), The psychology of entrepreneurship. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp.
6792.
Locke, E.A., Latham, G.P., 2005. Goal setting theory: theory building by induction. In: Smith, K.L., Hitt, M. (Eds.), Great minds in management. Oxford University
Press, New York.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., Diener, E., 2005. The benets of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin 131, 803855.
McMullen, J.S., Shepherd, D.A., 2006. Entrepreneurial action and the role of uncertainty in the theory of the entrepreneur. Academy of Management Review 36 (1),
132152.
Miller, K.D., 2007. Risk and rationality in entrepreneurial processes. Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal 1, 5774.
Mitchell, R.K., Busenitz, L., Bird, B., Gaglio, C.M., McMullen, J.S., Morse, E.A., Smith, J.B., 2007. The central question in entrepreneurial cognition research.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice 31, 127.
Perry-Smith, J.E., 2006. Social yet creative: the role of social relationships in facilitating individual creativity. Academy of Management Journal 49 (1), 85101.
Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J., Podsakoff, N.P., 2003. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended
remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology 88, 879903.
Preacher, K.J., Rucker, D.D., Hayes, A.F., 2007. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research
42, 185227.
Reynolds, P., 2000. In: Katz, J. (Ed.), National panel study of U. S. business start-ups: background and methodology. In: Advances in entrepreneurship, rm
emergence, and growth, vol. 4. JAI Press, Stanford, CT, pp. 153227.
Schindehutte, M., Morris, M.H., 2009. Advancing strategic entrepreneurship research: the role of complexity science in shifting the paradigm. Entrepreneurship
Theory and Practice 33, 241276.
Shalley, C.E., Perry-Smith, J.E., 2001. Effects of social psychological factors on creative performance: the role of informational and controlling expected evaluation
and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 84, 122.
Shane, S., 2003. The individual-opportunity nexus approach to entrepreneurship. Eward Elgar, Aldershot, United Kingdom.
Shane, S., Locke, E.A., Collins, C.J., 2003. Entrepreneurial motivation. Human Resource Management Review 13 (2), 257279.
Smith, K.G., Collins, C.J., Clark, K.D., 2005. Existing knowledge, knowledge creation capability, and the rate of new product introduction in high-technology rms.
Academy of Management Journal 48 (2), 346357.
Snow, C.C., 2007. Moderator comments: innovation. Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal 1, 101102.
Sobel, M.E., 1982. Asymptotic condence intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In: Leinhart, S. (Ed.), Sociological methodology. Jossey-Bass,
San Francisco, pp. 290312.
Stranks, J., 2005. Stress at work. Elsevier Butterworth-Heinemann, Burlington, MA.
Subramaniam, M., Youndt, M., 2005. The inuence of intellectual capital on the nature of innovative capabilities. Academy of Management Journal 48 (3), 450464.
Tushman, M., Anderson, P., 1986. Technological discontinuities and organizational environments. Administrative Science Quarterly 31, 439465.
Ucbasaran, D., Westhead, P., Wright, M., 2009. The extent and nature of opportunity identication by experienced entrepreneurs. Journal of Business Venturing 14,
99115.
Van Katwyk, P.T., Fox, S., Spector, P.E., Kelloway, E.K., 2000. Using the Job-related Affective Well-being Scale (JAWS) to investigate affective responses to work
stressors. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 5, 219230.
Ward, T.B., 2004. Cognition, creativity, and entrepreneurship. Journal of Business Venturing 19, 173188.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., 1997. The measurement and mismeasurement of mood: recurrent and emergent issues. Journal of Personality Assessment 68, 267296.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A., 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 54, 10631070.

You might also like