You are on page 1of 8

SRAC Publication No.

431

Southern
Regional
Aquaculture
.
Center

November 1991

Testing Flavor Quality of Preharvest


Channel Catfish
Martine van der Ploeg*

Farm-raised channel catfish is pounds are occasionally the result ceptability of fish to the processor.
known for its mild flavor and lack of inadvertent pollution, but in Methodology and criteria for fla-
of the “fishy” odor that is typical general are produced by biological vor quality evaluation vary from
for marine and wild freshwater processes that take place in the plant to plant. When off-flavors
fish. This mild flavor is savored by pond environment. Regardless of are not pronounced, fish may be
American consumers and, to- the source, the compounds are ab- rejected for purchase by one plant
gether with constant product qual- sorbed by fish through the gills but accepted at another plant. Fla-
ity, is an important attribute for and accumulate in the flesh. vor quality control does not need
successful marketing of farm- to be subjective. A standardized
raised catfish. Sensory analysis method for flavor testing can en-
Unfortunately, catfish may acquire To avoid marketing fish with envi- sure maintenance of one flavor
flavors perceived as unacceptable ronment-related off-flavors, fish quality standard in the industry
by the consumer. Such objection- must be screened for flavor quality and an objective evaluation of fla-
able flavors are known as “off-fla- before harvesting. Quantitative vor acceptability.
vors.” Marketing of off-flavor fish chemical analysis is inadequate for Most fish farmers rely exclusively
is likely to jeopardize consumer flavor quality control because of on processing plant personnel for
satisfaction and future market de- time constraints, costs involved, flavor analysis. Nevertheless, con-
mand. Therefore, adequate quality and the limited number of odor- ducting sensory analysis of fish fla-
control to guarantee that off-flavor ous compounds that can be de- vors at the catfish farm maybe
fish are not marketed is essential tected with available methods. quite useful. Information on the
to the catfish industry. Sensory analysis (taste-testing), type and intensity of fish flavor
which treats trained “tasters” as may aid the farmer in managing
Reasons for “off-flavors” analytical instruments, is the only around off-flavor episodes or in
Undesirable flavors may develop applicable method for routine decision-making when pond treat-
during grow-out or after process- evaluation of fish flavor quality. ments for off-flavor are being con-
ing of fish. Prolonged or improper Sensory analysis can detect odor- sidered. If fish are routinely
storage causes processed fish pro- ous compounds at very low levels, checked for flavor quality, pond
ducts to spoil. This problem can be and discriminates between types harvest schedules can be adjusted
avoided by adherence to accepted of off-flavors as well as flavor in- to account for potential flavor
processing and storage proce- tensity. Variability and bias in sen- problems, and the success of treat-
dures. Odorous compounds in sory analysis can be minimized by ments to remove off-flavor can be
pond water are responsible for proper selection and training of monitored. On-farm taste-testing
off-flavors that develop in the fish judges. also releases some of the sample
prior to harvesting. These com- Preharvest flavor testing is prac- burden from processing plant qual-
ticed at all channel catfish process- ity control personnel. If fish are
* Delta Research and Extension Center, ing plants, and flavor quality is the prescreened at the farm, only sam-
Mississippi State University most important criterion for ac- ples from ponds deemed accept-
able need to be submitted to plant through the pharyngeal passage volves a qualitative and a quantita-
personnel for further evaluation. when tasting (Figure 1). tive aspect: off-flavors are describ-
ed with standardized terminology
This report offers a method appro- Depending on the volatility of the
and the flavor intensity is rated.
priate for testing fish flavor. The odorous compounds, a flavor may
method can be adapted for be experienced directly after a sam- Description of off-flavors is often
processing plant quality control or ple is taken into the mouth or after difficult because of these factors:
for use on catfish farms. Descrip- chewing. Chemical feeling factors ■ there are many chemical com-
tion of catfish flavors commonly (astringency, spice heat, metallic
pounds responsible for off-fla-
found in preharvest fish, grada- flavor) are experienced in the
vors;
tions in flavor intensity, sampling mouth and nasal cavities. To taste
schedules, selection and training a fish sample properly, the sample the same chemical compound
of judges, preparation of fish for must be dispersed over all the sur- can elicit different flavor de-
testing and testing procedures faces of the tongue to reach the scriptions from different people;
will be explained. various taste buds. A sample must ■ certain flavors may be pro-
be chewed well to allow all aro- duced by more than one chemi-
Flavor perception mas to reach the olfactory area. cal compound;
Flavor is a food attribute and is ex- The description of fish flavor takes
into account the sensory experi- ■ variation in the concentration of
perienced both in the mouth
(taste) and in the nose (smell). ences of taste, smell and mouth an odorant may cause changes
feel, but not texture. in the flavor characteristics
Specific flavors result from a bou-
rather than in flavor intensity.
quet of tastes, odors and chemical
feeling factors. The four basic Flavor analysis However, the subjective nature of
tastes (salty, sweet, sour and bit- The fish taste-testing method de- flavor descriptions need not inter-
ter) are perceived by taste buds on scribed in this report is modeled fere with the objectivity of flavor
the tongue. Volatile odorous com- after the Flavor Profile Analysis analysis, provided that a stand-
pounds of food are sensed by olfac- procedure used by the food and ardized terminology is used. It is
tory receptors at the top of the beverage industry and municipal possible to train judges to give the
nasal cavities. The odorous com- water utilities to describe taste, same descriptor to a particular fla-
pounds reach the olfactory area odor and flavor characteristics of vor, even when the sensory experi-
through the nose when sniffing or their products. Flavor-testing in- ence among judges differs. When a
new flavor is encountered,
judges must reach consensus
on an appropriate descriptor
that thereafter will be used by
all judges for that particular
flavor.

Olfactory
Catfish flavor
descriptors
Researchers and plant process-
ing personnel use various de-
scriptors to describe flavors in
channel catfish. Some attempts
have been made to stand-
ardize these flavor descriptors,
but inconsistency and lack of
distinction between different
flavors still exists. Sources of
odorous compounds in fish
ponds include algae, microor-
ganisms that decompose vege-
tation, fish waste products and
pollutants such as diesel fuel
or pesticides. Many of the
specific compounds that cause
objectionable flavors in fish
have not been identified.
Therefore, most flavor descrip-
Figure 1. Section through the human head showing the olfactory area.
tors are standardized by and

2
referenced to names of commonly neol” or “musty/MIB.” Two other wheel contains those 25 descrip-
known materials and scents with characteristic off-flavors, “diesel” tors that are commonly used by
odor characteristics similar to the the fish taste panels at Auburn
off-flavor. Examples of such de-
scriptors are “chicken,” referring to and “pesticide,” are described by University in Alabama, Delta Re-
the flavor of broiled chicken meat, the general name for the cause of search and Extension Center at
and “sewage” that refers to the the flavors. Stoneville, Mississippi, and Delta
odor of sewage lagoons. Pride processing plant, Indianola,
Recognizing the limitations of any
The specific source of two com- flavor classification scheme, the Mississippi, Similar flavors are
mon off-flavors is known, and common flavors encountered in placed together within the outer
these flavors have been named for catfish from commercial catfish wheel. Flavor descriptors have
both the odor descriptors and the ponds in the southeastern United been clustered in six categories
causative agents: “earthy/geos- States are depicted in a flavor which are called Acceptable, Blue-
min” and “musty/methylisobor- wheel (Figure 2). The proposed green Algae, Chemical, Decay,

Flavor Wheel

Figure 2. Flavor descriptors commonly used by fish taste panels in testing preharvest pond-raised channel catfish.

3
Vegetable, and Fishy. The four vor is often referred to as “blue- vors are easily recognized by the
tastes (sweet, sour, bitter, and green algae” but it is not the only characteristic odors associated
salty) have not been included in flavor that can be attributed to with diesel fuel, kerosene, lighter
blue-green algae and is better fluid and motor oils. Pesticide off-
the flavor wheel as they are not referred to by the name of the flavors vary in character depend-
known to be dominant charac- causative compound. The ing on the source of pollution but
teristics of disagreeable flavors in musty/MIB flavor is often con- can usually be assigned to this
channel catfish. fused with the earthy/geosmin fla- group based on their unique odor.
vor but when smelled side by side Metallic flavors are actually more
General acceptability of fish fla-
the distinction is obvious. Intense
vors is indicated with a ribbon in of a taste sensation or mouth feel.
musty/MIB flavors are reminis- Some indication of this type of
the center of the wheel: fish flavor
cent of camphor. Geosmin and
is most likely to be objectionable at characteristic can be experienced
MIB off-flavors are most common
the maximum width of the ribbon by placing a penny on the tongue
in pond-raised fish during the for a few seconds. The source of
(e.g., musty/MIB, and diesel fla-
warmer parts of the year. metallic flavor is not known.
vors) and, depending on flavor in-
tensity, may be acceptable to a The precise chemical causes of
Decay category
processor where the ribbon attenu- pine and woody off-flavors are not
ates or is absent (e.g., celery or known with certainty, but there is Off-flavors in this category are dif-
corn flavors). some indication that metabolizes ficult to describe because we often
of methylisobomeol or other simi- associate them with more than one
Acceptable category odor characteristic of decaying
lar compounds are responsible.
Farm-raised catfish should have a The pine flavor is reminiscent of matter. Off-flavors of the decay
mild flavor relative to marine or the odor of pine needles and pine- category are frequently found dur-
wild freshwater fish. The preferred scented air fresheners. The flavor ing the cooler months. Causes of
flavor is a combination of nut-like is often strong and always objec- these flavors are not known but
or pecan-like character and “sweet tionable in farm-raised catfish. odorous sulfur or organic nitrogen
protein” that is reminiscent of Frequently musty/MIB and pine compounds resulting from decom-
broiled chicken breast meat, corn, flavor occur simultaneously, with position processes in the pond are
or butter. Note that although these pine as the dominant flavor. Some suspected. Consumption of rotting
descriptors are considered positive individuals tasting fish with pine fish may contribute to these off-fla-
flavor attributes, if chicken, corn, flavor have associated the flavor vors as well. Descriptors in this
or buttery flavors dominate the with oil-based paint or turpentine. group include: egg/ sulfury, sew-
mild catfish flavor, fish may not be age, decaying vegetation, rotten
The woody off-flavor is one of the
acceptable to a processor. and moldy. The intensity of these
more difficult flavors to recognize
off-flavors may differ considerably
Blue-green algae category and describe. The flavor is similar
among fish as they maybe caused
Flavors in this category are the to the odor associated with wood
by odorous compounds in the
most common objectionable fla- chips or tree stumps. It is more dis-
water as well as by what the fish
vors in farm-raised catfish. The tinct when fish are cooked without
scavenge.
category is called “blue-green skinning. An astringent after-taste
is often noted after tasting fish The egg/sulfury flavor is the fla-
algae” because these flavors are
with woody off-flavor; this effect vor of (old) hard-boiled eggs. Sew-
often associated with blooms of
has not been experienced with age flavors are reminiscent of the
blue-green algae in aquiculture
odor of sewage lagoons. Decaying
ponds. Descriptors in this group other off-flavors. Woody off-fla-
include geosmin, MIB, pine, and vors in pond-raised catfish appear vegetation flavors are typified by
to be most common in the cooler the odor of decomposing aquatic
woody.
seasons. Some evidence exists that weeds or wet hay. Sewage and de-
The geosmin flavor is the earthy- they are caused by decomposition caying vegetation flavors both are
muddy flavor of geosmin, an odor- products of 2-methylisoborneol. considered objectionable. The fla-
ous compound produced by vor descriptor rotten is used when
certain species of the blue-green Chemical categoy fish taste like spoiled (red) meat;
algae and actinomycetes bacteria. Flavors in this category can be this flavor is most offensive and
The flavor often is associated with quite repulsive and are so differ- can be rather strong. The moldy
the odor of freshly turned garden ent from other flavors that they flavor does not have a sulfury
soil. The musty/MIB flavor is the can be detected and described character but rather suggests the
must y odor of methylisoborneol, with little difficulty. Two flavors odor of moldy cork.
another odorous compound pro- undoubtedly result from inadver-
duced by certain species of the
Vegetable category
tent pollution of the culture sys-
blue-green algae and actinomy- tem and, fortunately, are not Several off-flavors are redolent of
cetes bacteria. The musty/MIB fla- found frequently. Diesel off-fla- raw vegetables. The celery flavor

4
is the flavor of celery stalks. The Flavor intensity Threshold concentrations vary
mushroom flavor is the flavor of among individuals because people
Flavor intensity is the quantitative are not equally sensitive to odors.
raw, white mushrooms, and has
aspect of fish flavor quality and
an earthy note but differs from the
estimates the concentration of the Therefore, a threshold concentra-
earthy-muddy flavor typical of
flavor compound(s) of interest. tion of a consumer population is
geosmin. The descriptor greens/
Flavor intensity may be quantified defined as the lowest level that 50
grassy is used for flavors that can
on a scale as presented in Table 1. percent of the people can detect.
be associated with the odors or fla-
The threshold concentration is the Typically, people who judge flavor
vors of leafy green vegetables and
lowest level at which an odorous quality are expected to be sensitive
fresh cut grass. This flavor some-
compound can be perceived. enough to detect the compounds
times has an earthy note similar to
the earthy note in cooked spinach. of interest at a concentration lower
Onion flavor refers to the flavor of Table 1. Fish flavors intensity than the threshold level for the
wild onion and has a sulfury note. scale. consumer population. The thres-
In one particular incident, the oc- Verbal Intensity hold levels for geosmin and meth-
currence of this flavor could be at- Description Scale ylisobomeol have been established
tributed to large amounts of wild and can be used to select judges
No off-flavors 0 for fish flavor analysis (see section
onion leaves which were blown
into the water when pond banks Threshold T on selecting judges).
were mowed. Subtle celery, mush- Very slight 0.5 The facial scale in Figure 3 is a
room and onion flavors may be ac- Slight 1 graphic depiction of the degree of
ceptable to a processor. Slight to distinct 1.5 off-flavor in fish. Fish without off-
Fishy category Distinct 2 flavors receive the score 0 because
undesirable flavors are perceived
Unlike marine fish, a distinctly Distinct to strong 2.5
to be absent. In off-flavor fish the
fishy flavor is undesirable in chan- Strong 3 intensity of a flavor is scored as 1
nel catfish. The fishy taste is often
for a slight but recognizable flavor;
stronger in the dark muscles along
2 for moderately strong or distinct
the lateral line. Large fish have
more dark muscles than small fish.
In catfish with a large amount of
fat on the surface of these dark
muscles, it is difficult to judge if
fish flavor is disagreeable because
of a fishy off-flavor or because of
high fat content. The fishy cate-
gory includes rancid, stale and
cardboard-like flavors that suggest
prolonged storage although fish
were fresh when prepared, and
the crawfish and fish-oil flavors
that are typical for other seafood
products, Causes of these flavors
are not known. .
The cardboard flavor is similar to
the flavor of wet cardboard or a
wet brown bag; the rancid flavor
is the odor of rancid butter or fat;
and the stale flavor is best de-
scribed as the flavor of freezer-
burnt fish. These flavors are
objectionable because they contra-
dict the freshness of the product.
The crawfish flavor is reminiscent
of boiled crawfish tails and, al-
though undesirable, it usually is
not offensive in channel catfish.
The fish oil flavor is the pungent Figure 3. Flavor intensity scale. Adapted from: M. Meilgaard, G.V. Civille, and
flavor of codliver oil. B.T. Carr. Sensory evaluation techniques. CRC Press, Boca Raton,
Florida.

5
flavors; and 3 for strong flavors. A entiate between flavor intensities smokers are not less sensitive than
value of “0.5” maybe assigned to can be determined by asking po- non-smokers provided that they
fish with very slight off-flavors. tential judges to rank odorous solu- do not smoke within 1 hour of test-
tions of different concentrations. ing. Allergy symptoms, medica-
Extremely strong off-flavors may After selection, judges must be fa- tion and a “common cold” may
be indicated with the number 4. temporarily interfere with the abil-
miliarized with the flavor intensity
When off-flavor is strong enough scale. This can be done by tasting ity to taste. People with ‘a good
to warrant a score of 4, it can be nose’ are generally capable of test-
solutions of sugar or salt of differ-
smelled and it is not necessary to ent concentrations (Table 2) to “an- ing fish flavor quality after appro-
taste the fish sample. In all other chor” the intensity scale, and priate training. It is important that
cases fish must be tasted as the fish flavor always be checked by
subsequent taste-testing of various
odor of a fish sample can be rather food or beverages to evaluate the the same judges to maintain a cer-
different from the flavor that is tain degree of consistency in the re-
intensity of sweet and salt in those
perceived in the mouth. sults. If fish normally are checked
products. Through training and
by two judges it may be expected
practice, judges can develop a con-
Selection and training of sistent response to varying odor
that at least one judge will be avail-
judges concentrations and learn to assign
able at all times.
Individuals differ in their ability to
similar intensity scores to different
detect odors and flavors. Some
Fish preparation
flavors of equal strength. Bias to-
people may complain about a dis- wards flavors that are perceived to Flavors may vary from fish to fish
agreeable flavor in fish when oth- be more offensive than others within a pond, but the nature of
ers cannot detect any problem. should be minimized through that variation is unknown. At least
Variation in sensitivity to specific training; a strong dislike of a par- two fish should be analyzed from
flavors among flavor quality con- ticular flavor should not influence a pond at each sampling. Fish may
trol personnel can be reduced by the flavor intensity rating. Judges be snagged or caught with hook
proper selection and training of must be trained in recognizing the and bait. Fish size should be repre-
judges. Judges must be able to dif- different off-flavors and in the use sentative for the fish population of
ferentiate between geosmin and of flavor descriptors according to interest. Store fish on ice until
methylisoborneol, and they must the catfish flavor wheel. The best preparation because fish flavor
be able to detect these compounds training is tasting fish with an ex- can change as a result of decompo-
in water at 0.02 ppb (parts per bil- perienced person. sition. Fish should be dressed and
lion), the established threshold tasted as soon as possible after
concentration. The ability to differ- In general, males and females are they have been collected. If fish
equally sensitive to odors, and cannot be checked immediately
storage of gutted fish in a freezer
Table 2. Flavor intensities for different concentrations of sugar and is recommended. Limit storage
salt. time to a week, otherwise fish
Chemical Concentration Intensity Food or may acquire extra off-flavors.
beverage that Many different materials may im-
corresponds part an extra flavor to fish during
Sweet: sugar 5% 1- Slight Peanut butter, cleaning and preparation. Exam-
(1.5 oz/quart water) unsweetened juice pies are the odor of waterproof
markers, flavor of a wooden cut-
1 % 2- Distinct Soft drinks, vanilla ting board, odor of detergent that
(3 oz/quart water) ice cream has not been properly removed
from the work area, utensils, or
15% 3- Strong Jellies, preserves hands, and the flavor of the paper
(4.5 oz/quart water) bags in which fish are prepared.
Such flavors imparted after har-
Salty: salt 0.4% 1- Slight White bread, canned vest may mask the flavors that
(1/2 tsp/quart water) peas fish acquired in the pond. Extra
flavors that are always present at
2- Distinct Canned soup, the same intensity will be experi-
0.8%
(1 tsp/quart water) sardines enced as “background noise” and
do not necessarily interfere with
1.0% 3- Strong Soy sauce, anchovies flavor analysis. The brown paper
(1 1/4 tsp/quart water) bags that are used to wrap fish
when using a microwave do not
change flavor perception if the
same-brand of-bags is always used.
Fish may be skinned before prepa- especially when off-flavors are sub- ture of off-flavor episodes. Fish
ration but it is not required. Fish tle or belong to the Fishy or Decay may acquire and purge off-flavors
flavor is often strongest in the categories. Greater sample-to-sam- at different rates depending on the
layer of fat just under the skin. Off- ple consistency in describing fla- source, type and intensity of the
flavors are therefore easier to de- vors and intensity can be obtained flavor, water temperature, fish
tect when fish are not skinned. if more than one judge is involved size, and possibly other factors.
Usually it is sufficient to sample in the process. Each judge tastes Off-flavors may develop within a
the flesh in the portion between and grades the fish of a pond, then matter of hours if the level of odor-
vent and tail. the findings are discussed. When a ous compounds in the water rises
consensus is reached between suddenly, as could be the case dur-
Fish should be cooked either by
judges, flavor description and fla- ing a chemical spill or a sudden
microwaving or by steaming
vor intensity should be recorded. die-off of odor-producing algae.
above boiling water. Never add
Average the flavor descriptions The rate of off-flavor removal is
seasonings or breading. Wrap fish
and flavor intensity scores for each much slower than uptake, and fish
samples separately so that odors
pair of fish to decide on the overall are not expected to purge geosmin
released during cooking do not
fish flavor of a pond. and methylisoborneol flavors in
contaminate the other samples.
less than about five days.
When a microwave is used, wrap The actual taste-test consists of
fish in paper or plastic bags. smelling the odor with little sniffs, Processing plants routinely test
When fish are steamed, wrap filets and tasting the flavors while fish flavor to ensure that off-flavor
separately in aluminum foil and slowly chewing the fish sample. It fish are not harvested nor proc-
cover the pot during steaming. may be necessary to taste a sample essed. Their main concern is
Tightly folded aluminum foil al- twice to better judge the presence timely detection of off-flavors, and
lows air to expand but prevents of subtle flavors. Olfactory recep- fish sampling is scheduled accord-
odors from escaping the package. tors fatigue easily during sensory ingly. When a producer wants to
Cooking time depends on the evaluation, especially when fish sell fish, a sample usually is sub-
method used, and size and num- have strong off-flavors. Fish sam- mitted for flavor evaluation a
ber of fish samples that are pre- ples should not be swallowed as week or so before a planned har-
pared simultaneously. Avoid that may cause the senses to fa- vest to obtain a tentative approval
overcooking which may create tigue faster. Strong off-flavors for purchase. Fish flavor must
other flavors. For example, brown- may be judged by smelling alone then be checked one day before or
ing of fish produces a caramelized or should be tasted at the end of at the day of the harvest to make
flavor. Some trial and error may the session. Cleanse the palate be- certain that no off-flavors have de-
be necessary to determine the tween samples with odor-free veloped since fish were scheduled
proper cooking time. water, apple juice or water that for harvesting. A final sample is
has been slightly acidified with cit- taken from the transport truck be-
Testing fish flavor quality ric acid (0.03 percent w/v). Some fore fish are unloaded at the plant.
Fish must be tested for off-flavors people prefer to eat white bread or Fish must meet flavor standards at
in an environment free of odors unsalted crackers between sam- all times, or harvesting will be can-
that can interfere with sensory ples. celled and fish returned to the
evaluation. Perfumes, the smell of Judges may be provided with a pond from the transport truck.
food, cigarette smoke and other small snack, like cookies, at the Biweekly on-farm fish sampling
strong odors must be absent be- end of each session to assure that for flavor evaluation allows pro-
fore and during a taste-test. Before bad flavors do not linger in the ducers to monitor the incidence
a taste-test, judges should wash mouth for a long time after the and length of off-flavor episodes.
their hands with a non-odorous session. Also, it is important that During the cooler months sam-
soap (e.g., Ivory). Judges are ad- the last impression of a taste ses- pling frequency may be reduced to
vised to abstain from eating at sion is a good one. If possible, no once per month. To determine the
least 15 to 30 minutes prior to more than ten fish should be sam- effect of pond treatments to get rid
testing. pled in one session. Fish flavor is of off-flavor, fish should be sam-
Start a fish tasting session with an judged more objectively in several pled immediately before a treat-
on-flavor fish sample as a “warm- short sessions than during one ment and approximately five and
up” to set the base line for flavor long session. ten days after the treatment.
evaluation. Fish filets must be
smelled and tasted while they are Pond sampling schedules
still warm, as flavor perception for fish flavor control
changes with temperature. Several Sampling schedules for fish flavor
parts of the filet should be sam- evaluation must take into account
pled because an off-flavor may not the dynamic and unpredictable na-
be distributed evenly in the filet,
Acknowledgments Additional reading Lovell, R.T. and D. Broce. 1985.
materials Cause of musty flauor in pond-cul-
The author wishes to thank Drs. tured penaeid shrimp. Aquacul-
C.S. Tucker, R.T. Lovell and S.W. Johnsen, P.B. 1989. Factors influenc- ture, 50:169-174.
Krasner for their helpful com- ing the flavor quality of farm-
ments on the manuscript. The con- raised catfish. Food Technology, Tucker, C.S. and J.F. Martin. 1991.
tributions of the members of the November, pp. 94-97. Environment-related off-flavors in
Stoneville taste-panel, L.S. Jack- fish, pp. 133-179. In: D.E. Brune
Johnsen, P.B., G.V. Civille, and J.R. and J.R. Tomasso (eds.). Aquac-
son, T.D. Santucci, K.L. Smith,
Vercelotti. 1987. A lexicon of
M.B. Pickens and M.E. Dennis, ulture and water quality. Ad-
pond-raised catfish flavor descrip- vances in World Aquaculture,
and S. Marshall of Delta Pride,
tors. Journal of Sensory Studies, Vol. 3, The World Aquaculture
Inc., are gratefully acknowledged.
2:85-91. Society. Baton Rouge, LA.
A four-color flavor wheel poster is
available for display purposes. Krasner, S.W., M.J. McGuire, and
The flavor wheel was developed V.B. Ferguson. Tastes and Odors:
by the Delta Research and Exten- The flavor profile method. Journal
sion Center, Mississippi Agricul- of the American Water Works
tural and Forestry Experiment Association, 77:3439.
Station, Mississippi State Univer- Lovell, R.T. 1983. New off-flavors in
sity. pond-cultured channel catfish.
Aquaculture, 30:329-334.

The work reported in this publication was supported in part by the Southern Regional Aquaculture Center through Grant No. 89-38500-4516 from the United
States Department of Agriculture.

You might also like