You are on page 1of 19

Kvalem, I. L., Tren, B., Lewin, B., & tulhofer, A. (2014).

Self-perceived effects of Internet


pornography use, genital appearance satisfaction, and sexual self-esteem among young Scandinavian
adults. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 8(4), article 4. doi:
10.5817/CP2014-4-4

Self-perceived effects of Internet pornography use, genital


appearance satisfaction, and sexual self-esteem among young
Scandinavian adults
Ingela Lundin Kvalem1, Bente Tren2, Bo Lewin3, Aleksandar tulhofer4
1,2
3
4

University of Oslo, Norway


Uppsala University, Sweden
University of Zagreb, Croatia

Abstract
This study examined the associations among the frequency of viewing Internet pornography,
beliefs about how realistically pornography portrays sex, self-perceived effects of ones
pornography use, genital appearance satisfaction, and sexual self-esteem in young adults. Online
data were collected from four convenience samples of university students from Norway and
Sweden, members of a queer youth organization, and readers of an erotic magazine. Because of
cultural similarities and the comparable magnitude and patterns of the bivariate correlations
among the samples on the study variables, they were pooled into a single sample (N = 1,274). The
majority of men (81.1%) and a minority of women (18.1%) reported at least weekly use of
Internet pornography on their personal computers, whereas using a mobile phone or tablet was
less common. Most of the participants had sought mainstream pornographic content. Those with a
stronger belief in pornographic realism were more likely to perceive the effect of pornography use
positively. A hypothesized relationship between self-perceived positive effects of pornography use
and a higher level of sexual self-esteem was found for men but not for women. This result was
partially due to higher satisfaction with genital appearance among the men who mainly watched
mainstream pornography. Genital appearance satisfaction was linked to higher sexual self-esteem
for women, but it was not related to the self-perceived effect of pornography use. The results
indicate that pornography may expand personal sexual scripts for both men and women, and may
have a positive, although modest, influence on the sexual self-esteem of young male adults.
Keywords: Internet pornography; genital appearance; sexual self-esteem

Introduction
During the past two decades, there seems to have been a mainstreaming of pornography in the mass
culture, for instance, in music videos and womens literature (Comella, 2013). Continuous advancements
in technology have made Internet pornography increasingly more accessible to young adults worldwide.
In the past decade, Swedish and Norwegian researchers have concluded that the majority of young adults
have been exposed to pornography and report positive effects of using it (Daneback, Cooper, & Mnsson,
2005; Knudsen, Lfgren-Mrtenson, & Mnsson, 2007; Tren, Nilsen, & Stigum, 2006; Tren,
Spitznogle, & Beverfjord, 2004). Nevertheless, research on the impact of Internet pornography use
continues to be deficient in several areas. tulhofer et al. (2007) suggested that a new research agenda
should have a greater focus on individuals well-being and avoid ideological framing as much as possible.
In addition, the role of the genre (type of pornographic content) should be included in the design of
research on pornography. This recommendation involves a shift from the traditional focus on the
relationship between watching pornography and sexual risk behavior to the potential for positive

outcomes of using mainstream and non-mainstream pornography. Wright, Bae, and Funk (2013) also
recommended that future research on pornography broaden its scope to include women in general, and
more specifically, to include individuals perceptions of pornographys realism and the role of pornography
in sexual learning.

Figure 1. The conceptual model to be tested.

This study proposed a theoretical model (Figure 1) that addresses the relationships between Internet
pornography use, beliefs about the realism portrayed in pornography, the self-perceived effects of ones
own pornography use, genital appearance satisfaction, and positive sexual self-esteem in young adults in
Scandinavia.

Scripted Lusts
Gagnon and Simon (2005) claimed that people's sexuality develops through a social process of continuous
interaction with significant and generalized others. Through social processes, sexuality is conferred
through the expressions and meanings that society imposes. Sexuality is scripted on three distinct
levels: cultural scenarios, interpersonal scripts, and intrapsychic scripts (Simon & Gagnon, 1986). Cultural
scenarios are historical, culturally shared guidelines for sexuality that specify appropriate objects, aims,
and desirable qualities of self-other relations,.. sequencing of events, and what the actors are assumed
to be feeling (Simon & Gagnon, 1986, p. 105). In addition to learning what is viewed as appropriate
sexual conduct, the individual also learns what is considered as deviant patterns of behavior (Laumann &
Gagnon, 1995). The interpersonal script deals with the organization of context-specific behavior with
shared social conventions and expectations that allow the involved actors to negotiate and participate in a
complex dyadic process. Thus, the interpersonal script enables individuals to find mutuality in sexuality.
The intrapsychic script comprises factors that motivate the individual to become sexually aroused, and to
commit to, and engage in, sexual activities. The individual adaption and transformation of local cultural
scenarios can serve as private sexual cultures within the shared public sexual culture, and thus can be
the basis for sexual diversity in a society (Simon & Gagnon, 1986, p. 106). The prevailing sexual script in
the Nordic context is one that connects sexual activity to love. The Love Ideology forms an important
theme that legitimizes sex in a normative fashion, particularly when it concerns legitimizing partner
sexuality among young people (Lewin, Fugl-Meyer, Helmius, Lalos, & Mnsson, 2000; Tren, 1993).
However, this does not rule out at least a partial acceptance of recreational sex among consenting adults.
Sexual conduct seldom is learned by observing parents or other persons having sex in real life, and adults
do not teach the young how to have sex. In this context, sexually explicit media (SEM) represent an
exception, as they show the young individual possible sexual acts and arousal stimuli. Much of the
existing SEM, particularly the media targeting men, deviate to some extent from the prevailing sexual
script and show a hedonistic sexuality detached from the love script (Kutchinsky, 1988). In SEM, sexual
acts are the center of attention. Few studies on Internet pornography have operationalized the form,
content, or genre of pornography (Short, Black, Smith, Wetterneck, & Wells, 2012). When
operationalized, the definition of Internet pornography has a range of broader descriptions, such as
softcore vs. hardcore, mainstream vs. paraphilia/deviant (Seigfried-Spellar & Rogers, 2013; tuhlhofer,
Buko, & Landripet, 2008), types of sexual behavior (e.g., intercourse, anal, group sex), sexual

orientation preferences, and media format (e.g., amateur, animation) (Hald, 2006; Short et al., 2012).
What is considered common or mainstream will be highly subjective on the individual level, but also
changes relative to the social acceptability and accessibility of pornography (Hald, 2006; Paul, 2009).
However, there is support for making a distinction between a mainstream and non-mainstream genre.
Previous research has found that groups with a preference for paraphilia or deviant pornography had
higher sexual activity and an earlier age of onset for viewing pornography (Seigfried-Spellar & Rogers,
2013; tuhlhofer et al., 2007). Although the SEM industry has an offering for nearly every sexual
preference, people tend to watch mainstream pornography (Paul, 2009). Thus, it appears that most
people prefer watching a pornography genre that mirrors their prevailing intrapsychic script.
Using sexual script theory (Gagnon & Simon, 2005) as a point of reference, four hypotheses were tested,
described in the following sections.

Consumption and Self-Perceived Positive Effects of Pornography


Changes in accessibility and privatization of Internet pornography use may change the sexual scripts that
guide sexual behavior, both interpersonally and privately (Daneback, Mnsson, & Ross, 2012; Daneback,
Tren, & Mnsson, 2009). Cultural and sub-cultural differences in the consumption of Internet
pornography in the younger generation may reflect variations in their accessibility to technology, and in
the degree of normalization of pornography and thus the sexual scripts.
In 2013, young men reported using SEM more frequently; and as compared to women, they regarded the
effects of their SEM use as more positive and uncomplicated (Tren & tulhofer, 2013). Most of them
reported that SEM had motivated them to try out new sexual positions and acts, increased their
understanding of their sexual orientation, and made them more aware of what they like to do sexually. A
recent study of men who have sex with men (MSM) in Norway, also concluded that the frequent
consumption of gay SEM seemed to play a positive role in MSMs sexuality in a similar way (Hald,
Smolenski, & Rosser, 2013). In a Swedish study of womens pornography use (Rogala & Tyden, 2003),
65% of the women reported generally positive effects and increased lust, and felt that pornography
influenced their sexual behavior by giving them tips on new positions. However, it is possible that
frequent users of SEM also assess the effects more positively to avoid cognitive dissonance between their
attitudes and behaviors (Festinger, Carlsmith, & Bem, 2007). This tendency may be more common among
women because of a need to justify their use of SEM in a society where the majority of young women
have ambivalent or negative attitudes towards pornography (Johansson & Hammare, 2007).
H1: Higher consumption of Internet pornography is associated with self-perceived positive
effects of ones own pornography use.

Viewing Pornography as Realistic and Self-Perceived Positive Effects of Pornography


The sexual communication model (Goldstein, 1984, p. 32) advocates that, on a psychological level, the
average pornography user is cognitively and perceptually capable of recognizing that a large portion of
pornography content is composed of fictional or exaggerated representations of sexuality (Gagnon, 1977;
Hald, 2006). This claim may be true if the consumer has similar sexual interaction experiences in real life
as those seen on film. In this context, the recognition of exaggerated or fictional representations may be
more difficult if the viewed pornography has non-mainstream rather than mainstream content. Young
adults with non-mainstream preferences most likely will have fewer real life arenas in which to experiment
with their desires. Therefore, they will not have as many opportunities to adjust their beliefs based on
their own experiences, and SEM may become the central arena within which they live out their desires.
Those who prefer mainstream pornography may have adjusted their opinions about the realism of SEM
because they have more opportunities to experiment with their desires in real life. Therefore, it is
reasonable to assume that a higher frequency of pornography use, type of pornographic content, and the
degree to which one thinks pornography realistically portrays sex may influence an individuals selfevaluation of the effects of pornography.
H2: The extent to which one thinks of pornography as a realistic and educational medium is
associated with self-perceived positive effects of ones own pornography use.

Self-Perceived Positive Effects of Pornography and Sexual Self-Esteem


tulhofer et al. (2007) found that the exposure to pornography affected sexual scripts as well as sexual
experiences, which had a positive effect on sexual satisfaction, particularly in young men. This finding

reflects the positive outcomes of pornography use, as described by young Norwegian adults (Tren &
tulhofer, 2013), and generates a new hypothesis involving a link between the use of pornography and
sexual self-esteem. Sexual self-esteem is associated with sexual satisfaction, and represents ones
affective reactions to ones sexual thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (Zeanah & Schwarz, 1996, p. 3).
Positive sexual self-esteem includes feelings of pride, satisfaction, and security associated with sexual
conduct. If people believe that the use of pornography makes them try out new sexual positions and acts
and helps them to find out what they desire sexually, the self-perceived effects of pornography use could
transfer to a positive evaluation of oneself as a good lover. However, some individuals perceive their
pornography use negatively, which may be related to negative sexual self-esteem, including
disappointment, dissatisfaction, or a sense of vulnerability or insecurity.
H3: Self-perceived positive effects of ones own pornography use are associated with higher
sexual self-esteem.

The Role of Genital Appearance Satisfaction


The portrayal of idealized bodies by the media is often regarded as one of the most important sources of
an individuals body image (Blond, 2008; Grabe, Ward, & Hyde, 2008). Body dissatisfaction, specifically,
genital appearance dissatisfaction among women, has been associated with lower sexual self-esteem. This
association is often due to increased body self-consciousness (Schick, Calabrese, Rima, & Zucker, 2010).
Negative beliefs about penis size for men are associated with shame, lack of masculinity, and lack of
sexual prowess (Veale et al., 2014). A common belief in the popular media is that the SEM industry has
influenced opinion regarding the ideal body in general and the ideal genital appearance in particular. The
increase in the removal of pubic hair among women has been linked to the media effects of pornography
(Braun, Tricklebank, & Clarke, 2013; Labre, 2002). The low variability in the appearance of genitals in
traditional pornography has been reported to be a source of many mens opinions that their penis is too
small (Veale et al., 2014). However, the belief that the frequent use of SEM leads to genital appearance
dissatisfaction has not been confirmed in the research literature, which is sparse. In fact, a study of MSM
found no association between frequent pornography use and dissatisfaction with their own genital
appearance (Morrison, Morrison, & Bradley, 2007). One can also argue that the diversity of SEM on the
Internet can provide users with the chance to see a larger variety of genitals, which may lead to an
increased sense of normality and acceptance of ones own genitals. This feature could be important for
women in particular, because they cannot see their own genitals directly without using a mirror. Female
sexuality is subject to social restraint, and in general, women are not socialized to perceive their own
genitals as acceptable (Braun et al., 2013). Therefore, SEM may contribute to womens perceptions of the
appearance of their genitals as more acceptable.
From the perspective of social comparison theory (Festinger, 1954), the context in which the idealized
media image is presented can influence the body image of the perceiver (Hfner et al., 2008). The selfperceived effects of ones own pornography use may be related to the degree of satisfaction one
experiences with the appearance of ones own genitals. A person watching SEM with a focus on the
similarities between themselves and the actor, or on the positive aspects of the situation depicted, may
develop a more positive self-image and alleviate the pressure exerted by idealized standards of
appearance in SEM (Hfner et al., 2008). In addition to contextual differences, there are individual
differences in the tendency to compare oneself with others (Gibbons & Buunk, 1999). Doubts about
oneself, a higher level of self-consciousness, and an orientation towards others are related to a greater
tendency to engage in social comparisons; for some MSM, these comparisons result in body dissatisfaction
and lowered self-esteem (Schwartz, 2010).
H4: An association between self-perceived positive effects of ones own pornography use and
higher sexual self-esteem can be explained partially by genital appearance satisfaction.
In previous research differences have been identified between men and women in relation to consumption
patterns as well as in relation to social norms concerning the use of pornography. In the present article
additional differences between the sexes, such as use of mainstream or non-mainstream is discussed as
being expressions not only of gender differences but also related to differences in social context and
individual differences in sexual experience. Use of sexually explicit material available over the Internet
among young adults in Scandinavia will also be discussed in relation to perceived realism of pornography,
self-perceived effects of own use of sexually explicit material as well as satisfaction with the appearance
of one's own genitals and general sexual self-esteem. It is, based on the findings reported here,
suggested that future research should take into account how gender differences as well as variations in
preferences for different types of pornography interact in order to understand the mechanisms related to
use of sexually explicit material.

Methods
Participants
Convenience samples of psychology students from the University of Oslo in Norway (n = 387), sociology
students from Uppsala University in Sweden (n = 217), members of a Norwegian queer youth
organization (n = 183), and readers of the erotic magazine Cupido in Norway (n = 487) completed an
online survey in the spring of 2013. The four subsamples in the study were treated as one convenience
sample (N = 1,274) for the following reasons. First, the number of participants in each group was too
small to conduct statistical analyses on the separate groups of men and women with differing
pornographic genre experiences. Second, studies have found substantial cultural similarities in sexual
behavior, sexual norms, and pornography use in the Scandinavian countries (Daneback et al., 2005;
Lewin et al., 2000). Finally, the purpose of this study was to examine the strength of the relationships
between the selected variables and not to focus on the prevalence of the different variables. Due to the
socio-demographic differences between the four groups, the bivariate correlations between all of the
variables that were to be included in the structural model were analyzed beforehand, for each of the
groups. The magnitude and pattern of the bivariate associations were similar among the groups, justifying
pooling their data to form one sample. Thus, the aim was to detect meaningful relationships that could be
tested in future studies rather than generalize percentages and means to a specific population.
The socio-demographic characteristics of the total sample and each of the four sub-samples are presented
in Table 1.
Table 1. Socio-Demographic Characteristics of the Samples (percent).

Gender
Men
Women
Other
Age
1824 years
25 years
Native born
Yes
No
Relationship status
Single, never in a committed relationship
Single, have been in a committed
relationship
In a committed relationship, not
cohabiting
Living with a partner (cohabiting/married)
Main occupation
Studying
Working
Job seeker
Sick leave
Maternity leave
Other
*** p < .001

Total
sample

Oslo
students

Uppsala
students

Queer
youth

Cupido
readers

36.0
62.5
1.5
(n = 1,139)

14.3
85.4
0.3
(n = 371)

24.9
73.6
1.5
(n = 201)

39.5
53.1
7.4
(n = 162)

60.0
39.8
0.2
(n = 405)

52.6
47.4
(n = 1,151)

54.0
46.0
(n = 372)

57.8
42.2
(n = 204)

65.1
34.9
(n = 166)

43.5
56.5
(n = 406)

***

92.2
7.8
(n = 1,140)

92.5
7.5
(n = 371)

92.1
7.9
(n = 202)

89.5
10.5
(n = 162)

93.1
6.9
(n = 405)

ns

15.2
30.0

13.2
24.7

10.6
31.3

26.5
34.6

14.7
32.4

21.4

25.8

25.3

16.0

17.5

33.5
(n = 1,114)

36.4
(n = 365)

32.8
(n = 198)

22.8
(n = 162)

35.5
(n = 389)

68.6
24.6
2.4
1.4
0.3
2.8
(n = 1,136)

91.4
7.6
0.5
0.0
0.0
0.5
(n = 370)

97.5
1.5
0.0
0.0
0.0
1.0
(n = 201)

57.1
30.1
6.1
2.4
0.0
4.3
(n = 163)

37.8
49.5
3.7
3.0
0.7
5.2
(n = 402)

***

***

***

The majority of the participants in the total sample (62.5%) were women, 36.0% were men, and 1.5%
reported having an other gender identity. There were slightly more respondents in the group consisting
of those who were 1824 years old (52.6%) than in the 25 years or older (47.4%) group. The mean age
of the sample was 24.7 years (SD = 3.5, range = 1831 years). The mean age of the female respondents
was slightly lower than the mean age of the male respondents (24.3 years and 25.3 years, respectively).
Ninety-two percent of the sample was born in Norway or Sweden. The majority of respondents identified
their main occupation as either student (68.6%) or worker (24.6%). Respondents who claimed that they
were in a committed relationship comprised 54.9% of the sample; 30.0% were single but had been in a
relationship previously; and 15.2% were single and had never been in a committed relationship.

Procedure
Invitations to participate in the survey, with a direct link to the questionnaire on www.surveymonkey.no,
were sent to e-mail lists of students and members of a queer youth organization. All of the participants
received one reminder to complete the survey. Cupido readers were invited to participate on a banner on
the magazines web site www.cupido.no.
The survey consisted of 166 questions related to socio-demographic background, attitudes towards
different expressions of sexuality, sexual activity and experiences, sexual self-esteem, body image, beliefs
about pornography use, and actual pornography use.

Measures
Sexual self-esteem. The scale, developed by Snell, Fisher, and Walters (1993), consists of ten items
with the following instructions: Please indicate how much the following items describe how you have felt
about yourself in the three last months. Examples of the items are, I am better at sex than most other
people and My sexual relationship(s) is very good compared to most. Each item is rated from 1 = not
at all like me to 5 = exactly like me. A mean score for the ten items was calculated (M = 3.4, SD = 0.9),
with higher scores indicating positive sexual self-esteem. The Cronbachs in this sample was .93.
Pornography use. The participants were asked, Have you ever seen pornographic material? and all of
the remaining questions on pornography use were asked only of those individuals who responded Yes to
this question. The frequency of pornography use in general was assessed by asking, How often have you
used pornography in the last 12 months? with response options ranging from 1 = never to 8 = daily (M =
4.5, SD = 2.1).
Internet pornography use. Exposure to Internet pornography was measured using two questions under
the heading, Please state how often you have used pornography during the past 3 months for 1)
Internet porn on a computer and 2) Internet porn on a mobile phone or tablet. The response
categories ranged from 1 = have not used to 6 = more than once per day. Internet pornography use was
computed as the summed score of the two questions (M = 4.2, SD = 2.2, range 212).
Pornography genre. Respondents experiences were measured using the question Have you sought any
of the following types of pornography in the past 12 months? The response alternatives were 1 = BDSM,
2 = fetishism, 3 = bestiality, 4 = sexual activities that include violence or coercion, 5 = common or
mainstream pornography, and 6 = something else, which was followed by a request for respondents to
indicate a specific genre = What? A new dichotomous variable called pornography genre experience was
created where 1 = mainstream pornography, and 2 = non-mainstream pornography. Some of the
respondents who chose Something else wrote gay porn or lesbian porn. This response was
interpreted as mainstream pornography among the gay/lesbian respondents, and non-mainstream among
the heterosexuals. The categorization of sexual orientation was based on a single question where the
respondents indicated whether they were gay/lesbian, heterosexual, bisexual, or none of these three
alternatives.
Realistic depiction of sex in pornography. Four items from a scale developed by Buselle (2001) and
adapted for SEM by Peter and Valkenburg (2006) were used to measure respondents opinions: I think
pornographic depictions of sex are realistic, One can learn a lot about sex when watching pornography,
Pornography helps me learn how to act when I have sex with someone, and Pornographic sex is like
sex in real life. Each item was rated on a scale ranging from 1 = completely disagree to 5 = completely
agree. A mean score variable was created based on the response to the items (M = 2.1, SD = 0.8). The
Cronbachs was .78.

Self-perceived effects of own pornography use. The self-assessment scale, by Rosser (Hald, Tren,
Noor, Iantaffi, & Rosser, in press), consisted of nine items, rated on a response scale ranging from 1 =
very bad to 5 = very good, under the heading, To what degree has pornography influenced the following
in a good or bad way? The items were as follows: Your understanding of what you like to do sexually,
Your attitude about sex, Your knowledge of sexual acts, Your pleasure when you have sex with your
partner (s), Your interest in trying new sexual acts or positions, Your masturbation frequency, Your
understanding of your sexual orientation, Your relationship with your own body, and How frequently
you look for sex partners. The mean score of the nine items was calculated with a higher score indicating
self-perceived positive effects of pornography use (M = 3.5, SD = 0.7). The Cronbachs was .90.
Genital appearance satisfaction. This variable was measured using a single question: I like the looks
of my genitals, with response categories ranging from 1 = completely disagree to 5 = completely agree
(M = 3.5, SD = 1.1).

Statistical Analysis
Cases with missing values on fewer than 10% of the items on the measures of interest were replaced with
the mean value of the variable. To assess the bivariate-level associations between the selected variables,
we conducted a cross-tabulation analysis. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to test
gender differences on the combination of the dependent variables. Because of the gender-specific sexual
socialization and related body image issues (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Moradi, 2010), path analysis
using the maximum likelihood estimation method was performed separately for female and male
participants with AMOS 18.0 statistical software package (Byrne, 2001). A multi-group model approach
was used to compare mainstream and non-mainstream pornography users by gender. We assessed

Table 2. Frequency of Pornography Use in Young Adults (percent).


% All
Lifetime exposure to pornographic material
Yes
No

91.4
8.6
(n = 926)
Frequency of using pornography in past 12 months
Never
13.3
Once
5.1
A couple of times
17.6
Several times per year
10.6
About once a month
13.9
About once a week
15.1
Several times per week
19.6
Daily
4.9
(n = 822)
Use of Internet pornography on PC in past 3
months
Never
26.1
Once a month
33.4
Once a week
12.2
Few times per week
17.8
Once a day
8.3
Several times per day
2.2
(n = 809)
Use of Internet pornography on mobile phone
or tablet in past 3 months
Never
70.5
Once a month
13.3
Once a week
5.4
Few times per week
7.2
Once a day
2.0
Several times per day
1.6
(n = 792)

% Men

% Women

97.4
2.6
(n = 310)

88.1
11.9
(n = 599)

22.42, p < .001

1.0
1.0
5.5
6.2
8.0
21.8
44.6
11.8
(n =289)

20.3
7.5
24.6
12.6
16.8
11.2
5.8
1.2
(n = 517)

4.2
15.7
19.6
37.4
17.8
5.2
(n = 286)

39.1
42.8
7.9
6.7
3.0
0.6
(n = 507)

50.4
18.9
11.1
12.9
3.2
3.6
(n = 280)

81.3
10.1
2.4
4.2
1.4
0.6
(n = 497)

319.74, p < .001

308.10, p < .001

89.76, p < .001

whether the two groups were different with respect to the relationships between the variables of interest
by comparing a path model in which the structural parameters were left to vary freely (unconstrained
model) to a model in which the structural parameters were invariant across groups (constrained model). A
significant statistical difference between the two models indicated a substantial between-group difference.
To test the hypothesized mediation within the model, bootstrapping procedure with 2,000 resamples was
applied to provide a 95% confidence interval around the estimated indirect (or mediating) effects
(Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Only participants without missing information on the variables of interest were
included in the path analyses.

Results
The habits of pornography users are presented in Table 2. A total of 91.4% of the sample reported having
ever seen pornographic material: 97.4% of the men and 88.1% of the women (p < 0.001). Pornography
was watched most often on the Internet.
Of those having an experience with pornographic material, 5% reported daily use of pornography, while
13.3% had not used pornography for the past 12 months. There were large gender differences in the
frequency of pornography use. Over half of the men reported pornography use several times per week or
daily for the past 12 months, while only 7.0% of the women had the same pattern. Only 1.0% of the men
and 20.3% of the women had not watched pornography in the past year. The majority of the men
(80.0%) reported weekly or more frequent use of Internet pornography on their personal computers
(PCs) in the past 3 months, while the majority of women (81.9%) watched it less than once a month or
not at all. Use of a mobile phone or tablet to watch Internet pornography at least once during the
previous 3 months was less common for both men (49.6%) and women (18.7%). The total mean score
for watching Internet pornography was 5.8 for men (SD = 2.1) and 3.3 for women (SD = 1.7, df = 471, t
= 16.9, p < .001).

Table 3. Experience with Different Pornography Genres During the Past 12 Months
in Young Adults (percent).

BDSM
Fetishism
Bestiality
Sexual activities that include violence or coercion
Common or mainstream
Something else total
All or different combinations of the above
Gay/Lesbian (mainstream pornography)
Gay/Lesbian
Ageplay/MILF/teen
Group sex or anal (gender not specified)/cuckold
Soft/erotic/feminist/alternative (not specified)
Amateur or anime (not specified)
Hentai/BDSM/fetish/violence
Futanari, transpersons
No genre, something else

% All
19.6
3.9
0.6
1.2
59.4
15.3
2.3
2.5
1.1
1.1
1.9
1.4
1.4
1.6
1.1
0.9
(n = 643)

% Men
17.0
6.7
1.1
0.4
60.4
14.4
3.0
1.8
0.0
1.8
1.5
0.4
1.5
1.8
2.2
0.4
(n = 270)

% Women
21.4
1.9
0.3
1.9
58.7
15.8
1.9
2.9
1.9
0.5
2.2
2.2
1.3
1.3
0.3
1.3
(n = 373)

Note: Gay/Lesbian porn was categorized as mainstream pornography when the respondents reported being
gay/lesbian themselves. = 15.71, p < .01.

Which particular genre of pornography the respondents had sought during the previous 12 months is
presented in Table 3. The majority of participants reported that they had watched mainstream sex. More
women (21.4%) than men (17.0%) reported looking up BDSM, and this difference was significant.
Watching fetishism was more common among men, while bestiality and sexual activities that included

violence or coercion had been watched by a very small minority. On the other hand, 15.3% of all
respondents reported watching something else, and the specified categories under this alternative are
listed in Table 3. Approximately 2% indicated seeking more than one of the given categories besides
mainstream pornography. Within the something else category, experiencing lesbian/gay porn also was
reported, mostly by the respondents that identified themselves as being gay, lesbian, or bisexual (this
was later categorized as mainstream). Women sought out soft, erotic, or feminist porn more than men
did, while more men watched "futanari" (female anime characters that show both primary sexual
characteristics) or transperson porn than women did. A total of 62.2% of the men and 61.6% of the
women had experienced watching common or mainstream pornography, while 37.8% of the men and
38.4% of the women chose alternatives that they considered to be non-mainstream pornography.
The MANOVA revealed a statistically significant difference between men and women across the dependent
variables, F (4, 714) = 16.9, p < .001). Although there were no gender differences in the degree of
sexual self-esteem, men scored higher than women did on the other three study variables when the
dependent variables were analyzed separately: Realistic depiction of sex in pornography (men: M = 2.4,
SD = 0.8; women: M = 2.0, SD = 0.7, F (1, 717) = 46.7, p < .001), self-perceived effects of pornography
use (men: M = 3.7, SD = 0.7; women: M = 3.4, SD = 0.7, F (1, 717) = 14.9, p < .001), and genital
appearance satisfaction (men: M = 3.8, SD = 1.0; women: M = 3.4, SD = 1.1, F (1, 717),= 16.7 p <
.001).
Finally, path analysis was used to test the validity of the proposed structural model (cf. Figure 1) and
assess the hypothesized mediating role of genital appearance satisfaction on self-perceived effects of
pornography use and sexual self-esteem, separately for women and men. Multi-group analyses were
performed to explore possible moderating effects of the experienced pornography genre (mainstream vs.
non-mainstream). Figure 2 shows the structure of the relationships between beliefs about the realistic
depiction of sex in pornography, frequency of Internet pornography use, self-perceived effects of own
pornography use, genital appearance satisfaction, and sexual self-esteem among women. The model (2
(8) = 6.80, p > .62; GFI = .99, TLI = 1.0, RMSEA (95% CI) =.0.06) did not confirm the hypothesized
association between self-perceived effects of pornography use and sexual self-esteem in either group,
nor, consequently, the mediating role of self-perceived effects of pornography use. The differences
between the multi-group model in which structural elements were allowed to vary between the two
groups and the model in which they were made invariant were not statistically significant, indicating that
the observed findings were equally valid for women who watched mainstream pornography and those who
reported seeking non-mainstream material (2 = 11.70, df = 11, p > .08).

Figure 2. The structure of the relationships between use of Internet pornography, beliefs about the
realism portrayed in pornography, self-perceived effects of own pornography use, genital appearance
satisfaction, and sexual self-esteem among women who sought mainstream pornography (Main: n = 201)
and those who had experience with non-mainstream pornography (Non-Main: n = 127). *p < .05, **p <
.01, ***p < .001.

The findings of the multi-group path analysis for men are presented in Figure 3. The model explained 28%
of the variance in sexual self-esteem in men with mainstream pornography experiences, and 12% in men
with non-mainstream experiences. The path analysis model had a good fit with the data ( 2 (8) = 7.85, p
> .41; GFI = .99, TLI = 1.0, RMSEA (95% CI) = .0 .08). Not all of the path coefficients were significant
in the non-mainstream pornography users, in contrast to the mainstream pornography user group. This
finding probably was due to the small size of the group. Among the non-mainstream pornography users,
the paths between the self-perceived effects of pornography use and satisfaction with ones genital
appearance, as well as between genital appearance satisfaction and sexual self-esteem, did not reach
statistical significance.
The structure of the constructs of interest was similar in the mainstream and non-mainstream male
pornography users. The model in which structural parameters were allowed to vary between the groups
(the unconstrained parameters model) did not differ significantly from the model in which the structural
weights, covariance, and residuals were made invariant across the groups (2 = 18.49, df = 8, p >
.07). The hypothesized mediating role of genital appearance satisfaction in the association between selfperceived effects of pornography use and sexual self-esteem was significant only among the mainstream
pornography users (95% CI for the indirect effect = .02.18 p < .05).

Figure 3. The structure of the relationships between the use of Internet pornography, beliefs about the
realism portrayed in pornography, self-perceived effects of own pornography use, genital appearance
satisfaction, and sexual self-esteem among men who sought mainstream pornography (Main: n = 146),
and those who had experience with non-mainstream pornography (Non-Main: n = 92). *p < .05, **p <
.01, ***p < .001.

Discussion
The purpose of this study was to test a model of the relationships between the use of pornography and
sexual self-esteem in men and women watching different genres of Internet pornography. The model did
not fit for women, as there was no link between perceived effects of pornography and sexual self-esteem,
either directly or indirectly. The theoretical model had a good fit with the data for men watching
mainstream pornography, although some of the pathways were not significant for the non-mainstream
group. There was no difference in the structure of the constructs according to the type of pornography
experienced among the women or men.

The first hypotheses was confirmed in men and women who sought mainstream pornography in that
individuals with a higher consumption of Internet pornography also indicated that it provided them with a
more positive view of their own sexuality. The second hypothesis was supported in both men and women
in that there was a relationship between the extent to which one thinks that pornographic depictions of
sex are realistic and the educational and self-perceived positive effects of pornography use. The third
hypothesis was only confirmed in men. Men who thought that Internet pornography use had a positive
impact on their sexuality also tended to report a higher level of sexual self-esteem. This was, to some
degree, due to being satisfied with their genital appearance, as stated in the fourth hypothesis, but only
among the men who sought mainstream pornography. There was no link between the self-perceived
effects of pornography use and sexual self-esteem among women. For both mainstream and nonmainstream groups, women who were more satisfied with their genital appearance also had higher sexual
self-esteem, but their genital appearance satisfaction was not associated with having a positive view of
watching Internet pornography.
The majority of the male participants in this study were frequent pornography users. Although most of the
women had watched pornography during the past year, their viewing pattern was more infrequent than
the mens pattern, which corresponds to previous research findings (Tren & tulhofer, 2013). In
Norway, 96% of the adult population below 45 years of age used the Internet on an average day in 2013
("Norwegian media barometer, 2013," 2014, March). In this study, 80.1% of the men and 10.3% of the
women had watched Internet pornography at least weekly during the past three months. The high
percentage for Nordic men is in agreement with other findings from self-selected online samples (Hald,
2006). In a recent study (tulhofer, Tren, & Carvalheira, 2013) with 5,255 men aged 18 years and older
from Norway, Croatia, and Portugal, the following proportions of men aged 1829 years reported weekly
or more frequent use of pornography: Norway = 83%, Croatia = 80.2%, and Portugal = 63.7%. Most
Norwegians have access to a PC at home (96%), while fewer have access to smart phones (73%) and
tablets (61%) ("Norwegian media barometer, 2013," 2014, March). This difference might be one of the
reasons why fewer people (50% of men and 19% of women) watched Internet pornography on smart
phones/tablets than on PCs in the previous three months. Another possible reason for this finding is that
smart phones and tablets more often are used in public places. On the other hand, the mobility of this
technology makes it easier to access online SEM in new contexts, such as vacations, bedrooms, and
wherever cable/wireless access is unavailable.

Self-Perceived Positive Effects of Pornography


In the present study, the self-perceived positive effects of using pornography were examined in relation to
its influence on the discovery of sexual preferences, pleasure, sexual orientation, knowledge of and
interest in different sexual acts, attitudes towards sex and the body, and the frequency of masturbation
and seeking sexual partners. All of these facets may be regarded as representations of the ways in which
pornography is expanding peoples sexual repertoire or, to put it another way, their interpersonal and
intrapsychic sexual scripts. It is logical that watching pornography more often will broaden an individuals
knowledge of sex, not only in general, but also for specific types of sexual behavior. There are few other
social arenas where this is a possibility, and the Internet has increased accessibility to both mainstream
and non-mainstream pornography. Although men watch pornography much more frequently than women
do, the relationship of its frequency of use with the degree of self- perceived effects of pornography for
men seeking mainstream pornography was not that different from women with the same type of
experience. This finding is consistent with the results of a study on arousal-oriented online sexual
activities conducted by Shaughnessy, Byers, Clowater, and Kalinowski (2013). The men in that study
reported engaging in online sexual activity more frequently, including watching pornography. However,
the overall impact was small, and there were no gender or sexual orientation-based differences in the
mainly positive outcomes of solitary arousal. There may be a threshold effect for how much change in
sexual scripts can be made outside of real life experiences. It is possible that the learning and inspiration
curves from watching pornography have the greatest increases in the beginning, and then level off as the
content becomes repetitive. In the present study, the lack of association found in the female nonmainstream group may be due to the seemingly larger diversity in the listed experienced genres
(spanning from soft erotica to paraphilia) than in the male non-mainstream group. The degree of
internalization may also vary. The similar pattern of findings for each gender in the mainstream groups
does not imply the absence of qualitative differences in the pornographic subject matter, which may be
influential, or that certain aspects of the sexual scripts may have affected men and women differently.
Hardy (2004, p. 16) argues that pornography can be a common currency of images and meanings for
both men and women, but that the source of sexual scripts also can be transformed and elaborated,
depending on the subjective fantasy and reality of the viewer. The somewhat stronger association
between the perceived realism of pornography and a more elaborate intrapsychic script for women may
be an indication of such qualitative differences. As most women do not watch pornography very often, it is

possible that pornography with sex acts and situations that are easier to relate to will increase social
comparison and further the internalization process. Parallel to the success of Fifty Shades of Grey, light
BDSM has become closer to mainstream pornography for women (Deller & Smith, 2013). The books
attracted readers with and without previous knowledge of erotica and/or BDSM (Deller & Smith, 2013)
and gave women permission to explore new things sexually, besides the fantasy of giving up control
(Comella, 2013; Kutchinsky, 1988). In the same way that rape fantasies were common in the erotic
literature of the 1980s, light BDSM has become a source of both romantic and sexual fantasies in the
female audience during the last decade (Morley, 2012). This explanation is consistent with the findings of
Carvalho et al. (2013), which indicate that women experienced more subjective sexual arousal in
response to explicit material than men did, when they were asked to imagine the viewed sex acts in a
romantic/relational context.

Sexual Self-Esteem
Sexual self-esteem is about satisfaction with one's own sexual competence, performance as a sexual
partner, and the positive effects that sexual relationships have on satisfaction. We found no gender
differences in sexual self-esteem, in contrast to the original findings, in which men scored higher on
sexual self-esteem when this subscale was tested (Snell et al., 1993). In line with extant research
(Johansson & Hammare, 2007; Tren & tulhofer, 2013), compared to the women, the men in the
current study reported that pornography provided more realistic and educational depictions of sex; they
viewed the effects of pornography use to be more positive; and they were more satisfied with their genital
appearance. Watching pornography seemed to add to the participants interpersonal and intrapsychic
sexual scripts, but this association was moderately related to the mens satisfaction with their sexual
competence and sexual relationships, and it was not related to womens satisfaction. In all probability,
real life experiences, rather than watching pornography, more readily influence sexual self-esteem;
however, a positive interpretation of what a person sees, seems to play a small part for men in viewing
oneself in a more positive light as a sexual partner. Internet pornography production is characterized by
the fragmentation of images, compilations, short video clips, and teasers side by side, with a focus on
decontextualized sexual activity and less reliance on the narrative conventions of traditional pornographic
films (including hardcore films) or erotic literature (Garlick, 2010). The Internet also invites a viewing
pattern of jumping quickly between sites and watching snippets of stories (Jacobs, 2004). Whether it is
easier to identify with and be influenced by non-fictional characters in depictions of the real world or
fictional characters in a narrative story line is debatable (Keen, 2013). The arousal component of Internet
pornography is strong due to close-ups and the focus on the sex act. It provides little or no narrative or
fantasies, which are factors that appeal to women, but maintains its educational potential for instruction
regarding various sex acts (and bodies). If womens sexual self-esteem is more strongly influenced by
relational aspects (also in relation to sexual acts), it makes sense that an expanded, but instrumental,
sexual repertoire does not necessarily have a significant effect on women.
Body image more often is linked to performance and function of the body in men compared to a more
static objectified image in women (Grogan, 2008). Therefore, it is reasonable to presume that this also
applies to genital appearance. This difference in body image may explain why genital appearance was a
partial link for the male viewers of mainstream pornography, but not for the women. For men, there is a
more logical path from the selfperceived effects of watching pornography on their sexual repertoire by
viewing their genital appearance with a focus on sexual function or penis size (length/girth/form) to
sexual self-esteem as a competent lover. For example, in a qualitative study of Swedish adolescents, boys
expressed their opinions of how Internet pornography use led to feelings of insecurity about their sexual
performance, while frequent users among the girls described feeling insecure about their bodies (LfgrenMrtenson & Mnsson, 2009). For women, genital appearance satisfaction was associated only with
sexual-self-esteem, and not the self-perceived effect of pornography. This finding is partly supported by
Peter and Valkenburg (2014), who in a recent study found no relationship between Internet SEM use and
body image (including genital appearance) among women. Previous research has revealed that womens
dissatisfaction with body parts leads to shame about their bodies, which is related to a lower frequency of
sexual behavior, low sexual self-esteem, and less satisfaction with sexual relationships (Pujols, Meston, &
Seal, 2010). According to self-objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997), self-consciousness and
shame about their bodies is a result of womens socialization and internalization of being sexually
objectified. Therefore, it is possible that the evaluation of genital appearance among women is more likely
to be a result of a general objectified body image, and less likely to be influenced by pornographic images
or the self-perceived positive effects of watching pornography.

Limitations
There are several limitations to this study. First, although most people in Scandinavia use the Internet on
a daily basis, there is still a selection problem in using convenience samples when studying the self
perceived positive effects of pornography. The need to justify watching pornography by over-estimating
the positive aspects, especially among women, was mentioned earlier. However, a positive attitude about
pornography may also be a basis for becoming involved with pornography in the first place. The second
limitation is the combination of four separate convenience samples with somewhat different pornographyuse profiles. Although the recruitment of participants from separate arenas may have influenced the
relationships among the variables, we found only minor variations in the bivariate associations between
the study variables when the samples were compared. Small variations between types of samples also
have been found in other studies; for example, Byers and Shaughnessy (2014) found similar attitudes
toward online sexual activity between a student sample and a community sample with sexual minority
individuals.
Another limitation is how mainstream and non-mainstream pornography was operationalized. The largest
group within the non-mainstream category is the "something else" group, and those who chose this
alternative did not view their choices as mainstream. On the other hand, some of the answers given under
this alternative (listed in Table 3) would be interpreted by many as mainstream (e.g., group sex, erotic or
amateur porn). What is considered mainstream is, to some degree, relative to the viewer, and even if the
researcher provided explicit definitions of the types of porn, there would still be a large overlap between
the categories and their interpretations.
Finally, the length of the questionnaire may have affected the response rate and quality (McColl, Jacoby,
Thomas, Soutter, & Bamford, 2001). The pornography questions were located in the last section and it is
possible that both motivation and response accuracy declined towards the end. The impact is difficult to
assess since the number of questions varied for each respondent, depending on the answer they gave on
the many branching questions.

Conclusion
The purpose of this study was to examine the association between Internet pornography use and sexual
self-esteem by exploring the mediating role of young adults self-perceived effects of pornography use.
Being positively, albeit weakly, associated with sexual self-esteem, pornography may serve as an arena
for expanding personal (and possibly interpersonal) sexual scripts, particularly among young men.
Although definitive conclusions should be avoided due to the sample characteristics of the study, our
findings suggest that the association between pornography use and sexual self-esteem is complex,
partially gender-specific, and most likely dynamic. It is important that future studies attempt to analyze
the association in more detail and use a prospective design. In addition, this study raised other questions
that deserve further exploration. For example, while new technology may not substantially change the
frequency of pornography use, will the ability to watch pornography at any time, using smart phones or
tablets, change current patterns of use and their correlates? It remains an open question to what extent
increasingly popular online amateur pornography will affect the distinction between reality (real life) and
fiction (staged action), and the current "pornification" of the body and gendered sexual roles.

References
Blond, A. (2008). Impacts of exposure to images of ideal bodies on male body dissatisfaction: A review.
Body Image, 5, 244-250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.02.003
Braun, V., Tricklebank, G., & Clarke, V. (2013). It shouldnt stick out from your bikini at the beach:
Meaning, gender, and the hairy/hairless body. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37, 478-493.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684313492950
Busselle, R. W. (2001). Television exposure, perceived realism, and exemplar accessibility in the social
judgment process. Media Psychology, 3, 43-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0301_03
Byers, E. S., & Shaughnessy, K. (2014). Attitudes toward online sexual activities. Cyberpsychology:
Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 8(1), article 10. http://dx.doi.org/10.5817/CP2014-1-10

Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and
programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Carvalho, J., Gomes, A. Q., Laja, P., Oliveira, C., Vilarinho, S., Janssen, E., & Nobre, P. (2013). Gender
differences in sexual arousal and affective responses to erotica: The effects of type of film and fantasy
instructions. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1011-1019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0076-2
Comella, L. (2013). Fifty shades of erotic stimulus. Feminist Media Studies, 13, 563-566.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14680777.2013.786269
Daneback, K., Cooper, A., & Mnsson, S.-A. (2005). An Internet study of cybersex participants. Archives
of Sexual Behavior, 34, 321-328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-005-3120-z
Daneback, K., Mnsson, S.-A., Ross, M. W. (2012). Technological advancements and Internet sexuality:
Does private access to the Internet influence online sexual behavior? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Social Networking, 15,, 386-390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0188
Daneback, K., Tren, B., & Mnsson, S.-A. (2009). Use of pornography in a random sample of Norwegian
heterosexual couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 746-773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-0089314-4
Deller, R. A., & Smith, C. (2013). Reading the BDSM romance: Reader responses to Fifty Shades.
Sexualities, 16, 932-950. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1363460713508882
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-139.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202
Festinger, L., Carlsmith, J. M., & Bem, D. J. (2007). Issue 4: Does cognitive dissonance explain why
behavior can change attitudes? In J. A. Nier (Ed.), Taking sides: Clashing views in social psychology (2nd
ed., pp. 74-91). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory - Toward understanding women's lived
experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173-206.
Gagnon, J. H. (1977). Human sexualities. Glenview: Scott Foresman.
Gagnon, J. H., & Simon, W. (2005). Sexual conduct. The social sources of human sexuality (2nd ed.).
London: Aldine Transaction.
Garlick, S. (2010). Taking control of sex?: Hegemonic masculinity, technology, and Internet pornography.
Men and Masculinities, 12, 597-614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1097184x09341360
Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: The development of a
scale of social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 76, 129-142.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.129
Goldstein, A. (1984). The place of pornography: Packaging Eros for a violent age. New York: Harper's.
Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in body image concerns among
women: A meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 460-476.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460
Grogan, S. (2008). Body image. Understanding body dissatisfaction in men, women and children (2nd
ed.). London: Routledge.
Hald, G. M. (2006). Gender differences in pornography consumption among young heterosexual Danish
adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 577-585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9064-0
Hald, G. M., Smolenski, D., & Rosser, B. (2013). Perceived effects of sexually explicit media among men
who have sex with men and psychometric properties of the Pornography Consumption Effects Scale
(PCES). Journal of Sexual Medicine, 10, 757-767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02988.x

Hald, G. M., Tren, B., Noor, S. W., Iantaffi, A., & Rosser, B. R. S. (in press). Does sexually explicit
media (SEM) affect me? Assessing first person effects of SEM consumption among Norwegian men who
have sex with men. Psychology & Sexuality.
Hardy, S. (2004). Reading pornography. Sex Education, 4, 3-18.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1468181042000176506
Hfner, M., Jagsch, O., Kund, A., Mager, S., Pereira, P. T., & Zimmermann, A. (2008). The female may
feel male: Defending against the adverse consequences of exposure to idealized media images. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27, 778-808. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2008.27.8.778
Jacobs, K. (2004). Pornography in small places and other spaces. Cultural Studies, 18, 67-83.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0950238042000181610
Johansson, T., & Hammare, N. (2007). Hegemonic masculinity and pornography: Young people's attitudes
toward and relations to pornography. The Journal of Men's Studies, 15(1), 57-70.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3149/jms.1501.57
Keen, S. (2013). Narrative empathy. In P. E. A. Hhn (Ed.), The living handbook of narratology.
Hamburg: Hamburg University Press. Retrieved from: http://www.lhn.uni-hamburg.de/article/narrativeempathy
Knudsen, S. V., Lfgren-Mrtenson, L., & Mnsson, S.-A. (Eds.). (2007). Generation P? Youth, gender and
pornography. Copenhagen: Danish School of Education Press.
Kutchinsky, B. (1988). Erotik, erotika and pornography. Et essay om kn, kultur, krlighed og
kiosklitteratur [Eroticism, erotica and pornography. An essay about sex, culture, love and kiosk literature,
Danish text]. Nordisk Sexologi, 6, 108-128.
Labre, M. P. (2002). The Brazilian wax: New hairlessness norm for women? Journal of Communication
Inquiry, 26, 113-132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0196859902026002001
Laumann, E. O., & Gagnon, J. H. (1995). A sociological perspective on sexual action. In R. G. Parker & J.
H. Gagnon (Eds.), Conceiving sexuality: Approaches to sex research in a postmodern world (pp. 183213). New York: Routledge.
Lewin, B., Fugl-Meyer, K., Helmius, G., Lalos, A., & Mnsson, S.-A. (2000). Sex in Sweden - On the
Swedish sexual life. Uppsala: The National Institute of Public Health.
Lfgren-Mrtenson, L., & Mnsson, S.-A. (2009). Lust, love, and life: A qualitative study of Swedish
adolescents' perceptions and experiences with pornography. The Journal of Sex Research, 47, 568-579.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490903151374
McColl, E., Jacoby, A., Thomas, L., Soutter, J., & Bamford, C. (2002). Design and use of questionnaires: A
review of best practice applicable to surveys of health service staff and patients. Health Technology
Assessment, 5(31), 1-256. http://dx.doi.org/10.3310/hta5310
Moradi, B. (2010). Addressing gender and cultural diversity in body image: Objectification theory as a
framework for integrating theories and grounding research. Sex Roles, 63(1-1), 138-148.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9824-0
Morley, A. (2012). Famine for food, expectation for content: Jane Eyre as intertext for the Twilight
saga. In A. Morley (Ed.), Genre, reception, and adaptation in the "Twilight" series (pp. 15-28). Farnham:
Ashgate Publishers.
Morrison, T. G., Morrison, M. A., & Bradley, B. A. (2007). Correlates of gay men's self-reported exposure
to pornography. International Journal of Sexual Health, 19(2), 33-43.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J514v19n02_03
Norwegian media barometer, 2013. (2014, March). Retrieved from: https://www.ssb.no/en/kultur-ogfritid/statistikker/medie/aar/2014-03-25

Paul, B. (2009). Predicting Internet pornography use and arousal: The role of individual difference
variables. The Journal of Sex Research, 46, 344-357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490902754152
Peter, J., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2006). Adolescents exposure to sexually explicit online material and
recreational attitudes toward sex. Journal of Communication, 56, 639-660.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00313.x
Peter, J., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2014). Does exposure to sexually explicit Internet material increase body
dissatisfaction? A longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 297307.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.071
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple
mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717-731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553
Pujols, Y., Meston, C. M., & Seal, B. N. (2010). The association between sexual satisfaction and body
image in women. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 7(2pt2), 905-916. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.17436109.2009.01604.x
Rogala, C., & Tyden, T. (2003). Does pornography influence young women's sexual behavior? Womens
Health Issues, 13, 39-43.
Schick, V. R., Calabrese, S. K., Rima, B. N., & Zucker, A. N. (2010). Genital appearance dissatisfaction:
Implications for women's genital image self-consciousness, sexual esteem, sexual satisfaction, and sexual
risk. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 34, 394-404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2010.01584.x
Schwartz, J. P. (2010). Gay men and body image: Social comparisons, blame, and stigma. Dissertation
Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 70(10-A), 3687.
Seigfried-Spellar, K. C., & Rogers, M. K. (2013). Does deviant pornography use follow a Guttman-like
progression? Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 1997-2003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.04.018
Shaughnessy, K., Byers, E. S., Clowater, S., & Kalinowski, A. (2014). Self-appraisals of arousal-oriented
online sexual activities in university and community samples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 11871197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0115-z
Short, M. B., Black, L., Smith, A. H., Wetterneck, C. T., & Wells, D. E. (2012). A review of Internet
pornography use research: Methodology and content from the past 10 years. Cyberpsychology, Behavior,
and Social Networking, 15, 13-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0477
Simon, W., & Gagnon, J. H. (1986). Sexual scripts: Permanence and change. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
15, 97-120.
Snell, W. E., Jr., Fisher, T. D., & Walters, A. S. (1993). The multidimensional sexuality questionnaire: An
objective self-report measure of psychological tendencies associated with human sexuality. Annals of Sex
Research, 6(1), 27-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00849744
tulhofer, A., Buko, V., & Landripet, I. (2010). Pornography, sexual socialization, and satisfaction among
young men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 168-178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9387-0
tulhofer, A., Landripet, I., Momcilovic, A., Matko, V., Kladaric, P. G., & Busko, V. (2007). Pornography
and sexual satisfaction Any relationship? In S. V. Knudsen, L. Lfgren-Mrtenson, & S.-A. Mnsson
(Eds.), Generation P? Youth, gender and pornography (pp. 66-86). Copenhagen: Danish School of
Education Press.
tulhofer, A., Tren, B., & Carvalheira, A. (2013). Job-related strain and sexual health difficulties among
heterosexual men from three European countries: The role of culture and emotional support. The Journal
of Sexual Medicine, 10, 747-756. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02967.x

Tren, B. (1993). Norwegian adolescents' sexuality in the era of AIDS: Empirical studies on heterosexual
behaviour. Oslo: National Institute for Alcohol and Drug Research.
Tren, B., Nilsen, T. S., & Stigum, H. (2006). Use of pornography in traditional media and on the Internet
in Norway. The Journal of Sex Research, 43, 245-254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490609552323
Tren, B., Spitznogle, K., & Beverfjord, A. (2004). Attitudes and use of pornography in the Norwegian
population 2002. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 193-200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490409552227
Tren, B., & tulhofer, A. (2013). The consumption of sexually explicit media (SEM) among young adults
in Norway. Paper presented at the International Academy of Sex Research, Chicago.
Veale, D., Eshkevari, E., Read, J., Miles, S., Troglia, A., Phillips, R., & Muir, G. (2014). Beliefs about penis
size: Validation of a scale for men ashamed about their penis size. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 11,
84-92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jsm.12294
Wright, P. J, Bae, S., & Funk, M. (2013). United States women and pornography through four decades:
Exposure, attitudes, behaviors, individual differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 1131-1144.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0116-y
Zeanah, P. D., & Schwarz, J. C. (1996). Reliability and validity of the sexual self-esteem inventory for
women. Assessment, 3, 1-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/107319119600300101

Correspondence to:
Ingela Lundin Kvalem, PhD
University of Oslo, Department of Psychology,
PB 1094, Blindern
N-0317 Oslo
Norway
E-mail: i.l.kvalem(at)psykologi.uio.no
Phone (office): +47 22 82 51 79
Fax: +47 22 84 50 51

About author(s)
Ingela Lundin Kvalem, Ph.D. is an associate professor of health psychology at the
Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway. Her research is focused on
two key lines of inquiry: 1) body image and ideals in adolescence and adulthood in
relation to sexuality and obesity and 2) evidence-based health interventions.

Bente Tren, Ph.D. is a professor in health psychology at the Department of


Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway. Her research is focused on three key lines of
inquiry: 1) sexual risk behavior in adolescence and adulthood, 2) use of pornography,
and 3) sexual function and well-being.

Bo Lewin, Ph.D. is professor emeritus in sociology at Uppsala University and has


been active in the study of human sexual behavior and sexual beliefs for forty years.
He has worked with sexual health issues for the World Health Organization and has
been a visiting scholar at the Kinsey Institute.

Aleksandar tulhofer, Ph.D. is professor of sociology and head of the sexology unit
at the Department of Sociology, University of Zagreb, Croatia, and affiliated faculty of
the Kinsey Institute for Research in Sex, Gender, and Reproduction. His research
interests are adolescent sexual risk taking, epidemiology of sexual health problems,
hypersexuality and pornography use, and the role of intimacy in male sexuality.

2008 Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace | ISSN: 1802-7962 | Faculty


of Social Studies, Masaryk University | Contact | Editor: David Smahel

Copyright of Cyberpsychology is the property of Masarykova Univerzita, Fakulta Socialnich


Studii and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like