You are on page 1of 17

Child Development, July/August 2005, Volume 76, Number 4, Pages 795 810

Duration and Developmental Timing of Poverty and Childrens Cognitive and


Social Development From Birth Through Third Grade
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network

Relations of duration and developmental timing of poverty to childrens development from birth to age 9 were
examined by comparing children from families who were never poor, poor only during the childs infancy (0 3
years of age), poor only after infancy (4 9 years of age), and chronically poor. Chronically poor families pro-
vided lower quality childrearing environments, and children in these families showed lower cognitive per-
formance and more behavior problems than did other children. Any experience of poverty was associated with
less favorable family situations and child outcomes than never being poor. Being poor later tended to be more
detrimental than early poverty. Mediational analyses indicated that poverty was linked to child outcomes in
part through less positive parenting.

The developmental risks associated with poverty cognitive and social development are examined us-
and economic disadvantage have been well docu- ing the longitudinal data from birth to Grade 3 (age 9
mented, but the processes that account for the rela- years) collected in the National Institute of Child
tions between poverty and childrens development Health and Human Development Study of Early
and the role of the timing and duration of poverty Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SEC-
have not been thoroughly explored (Bradley & Cor- CYD). The quality of the home environment, parent-
wyn, 2002; McLoyd, 1998). In this article, the rela- ing sensitivity, and child care experiences are
tions of duration and timing of poverty to childrens analyzed as potential mediators of poverty effects.
Previous studies have shown that children who
live in persistent or chronic poverty have less fa-
This study was directed by a steering committee and supported vorable cognitive and social development and
by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
poorer physical and mental health than do those
ment (NICHD) through a cooperative agreement (U10) that calls
for scientific collaboration between the grantees and the NICHD who live in transitory poverty (Bolger, Patterson,
staff. Participating investigators, in alphabetical order, are Virginia Thompson, & Kupersmidt, 1995; Costello, Compton,
Allhusen, University of California, Irvine; Jay Belsky, Birkbeck Keeler, & Angold, 2003; Duncan & Brooks-Gunn,
University of London; Cathryn Booth-LaForce, University of 1997; Korenman, Miller, & Sjaastad, 1995; McLeod &
Washington; Robert Bradley, University of Arkansas at Little Rock;
Nonnemaker, 2000; McLeod & Shanahan, 1993).
Celia A. Brownell, University of Pittsburgh; Margaret Burchinal,
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Susan B. Campbell, In one recent study, concurrent poverty, defined
University of Pittsburgh; K. Alison Clarke-Stewart, University of only from earned income, was found to be as detri-
California, Irvine; Martha Cox, University of North Carolina at mental to 11-year-olds as persistent poverty
Chapel Hill; Sarah L. Friedman, NICHD; Kathryn Hirsh-Pasek, throughout the school years (Ackerman, Brown, &
Temple University; Aletha Huston, University of Texas at Austin;
Izard, 2004a, 2004b).
Jean F. Kelly, University of Washington; Bonnie Knoke, Research
Triangle Institute; Nancy Marshall, Wellesley College; Kathleen McLoyd (1997, 1998) proposed that poverty, par-
McCartney, Harvard University; Fred Morrison, University of ticularly when it is chronic, affects childrens cogni-
Michigan; Phil Nader, University of California, San Diego; Marion tive development and socioemotional well-being
OBrien, University of North Carolina at Greensboro; Margaret because inadequate income increases parents expo-
Tresch Owen, University of Texas at Dallas; Ross Parke, University
sure to negative life events and stressors, which in
of California, Riverside; Chris Payne, University of North Carolina
at Greensboro; Robert Pianta, University of Virginia; Wendy turn produce psychological distress that diminishes
Robeson, Wellesley College; Susan Spieker, University of Wash- the capacity to provide sensitive, responsive
ington; Deborah Lowe Vandell, University of Wisconsin Madi- parenting and increases the likelihood of punitive,
son; and Marsha Weinraub, Temple University. We thank our coercive parenting styles (McLoyd, 1998). It has been
study coordinators at each site who supervised the data collection,
the research assistants who collected the data, and especially the
shown that families living in chronic poverty have
families, teachers, and child care providers who participated in less stimulating home environments and more pro-
the study. longed and extensive parental stress than do families
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, OEP, Office of the
Director, NICHD, 6100 Executive Boulevard, Room 4A01, Rock- r 2005 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
ville, MD 20852. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2005/7604-0003
796 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

living in transitory poverty (Linver, Brooks-Gunn, & 1995). Furthermore, child care for low-income in-
Kohen, 2002). The quality of the home environment fants and toddlers is especially likely to be of poor
improves when families move out of poverty and quality before they become eligible for most Head
declines when they move into poverty (Garrett, Start and other early intervention programs (NICHD
Ngandu, & Ferron, 1994; Votruba-Drzal, 2003). In a Early Child Care Research Network, 2001a; Phillips,
recent review of the literature, Bradley and Corwyn Voran, Kisker, Howes, & Whitbook, 1994). There are
(2002) concluded that access to stimulating learning few if any reports, however, following the same
materials and opportunities for exploration mediat- children from birth into later childhood with in-
ed the relation of socioeconomic status to both cog- depth measures of family economic resources and
nitive skills and social behavior. There are few childrens development; therefore, any conclusions
analyses testing the mediators of persistent versus about developmental timing remain tentative.
transitory poverty effects, however, and their results One issue in studying the timing of poverty in-
are inconsistent (Bolger et al., 1995; McLeod & Sha- volves the definition of early and late develop-
nahan, 1993). mental periods. In the present study, early is defined
Child care experiences may mediate some of the as the childs first 3 years and late is defined as age 4
relations of persistent poverty to childrens intellec- to third grade. These periods were chosen for two
tual and social development. Children in poverty reasons. First, infants and toddlers from low-income
spend less time in nonmaternal care but experience families are most likely to spend their time at home
lower quality care than do those in more affluent or in home-based settings that are similar to their
families. Because the quantity of care children re- homes; therefore, poverty predicts the quality of
ceive is associated with relatively high levels of be- both home and child care environments (e.g.,
havior problems at school entry (NICHD Early Child NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1997).
Care Research Network, 2003a), experiencing fewer By age 4, most children, including those from low-
hours of child care could counteract effects of pov- income families, enter preschool or organized early
erty on childrens problem behavior. Although child child care programs, and because such programs are
care quality is associated with cognitive develop- often subsidized for children from poverty-level
ment of children in poverty, there is mixed evidence homes, there is less family income discrepancy in the
about whether child care quality matters more for quality of care received (e.g., Phillips et al., 1994).
poor than for nonpoor children (Burchinal, Peisner- Second, earlier analyses of the NICHD sample
Feinberg, Bryant, & Clifford, 2000; NICHD Early showed relations of income to developmental out-
Child Care Research Network, 2001a, 2002). comes in the first 3 years (Dearing, McCartney, &
With regard to the timing of poverty effects, some Taylor, 2001; Mistry, Biesanz, Taylor, Burchinal, &
authors have shown that poverty in early childhood Cox, 2004).
is more deleterious to long-term behavioral and Income effects on childrens environments and
achievement outcomes than poverty in middle child- development are not linear; variations near the bot-
hood or adolescence (e.g., Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, tom of the income distribution have considerably
2000). Early poverty is also more strongly related stronger relations to family processes and to cogni-
than later poverty to psychiatric morbidity (Lipman tive and social development than do variations in the
& Offord, 1997; Rutter, 1989). If the effects of poverty upper ranges (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). Income
are conveyed to children through less sensitive and differences in the lower but not the upper ranges
stimulating interactions with adults and fewer re- predict years of completed schooling (Duncan, Ye-
sources in their home and child care environments, ung, Brooks-Gunn, & Smith, 1998) and the amount of
poverty might be most detrimental during the very cognitive stimulation in the home (Garrett et al.,
early years. The home environment is more closely 1994; Votruba-Drzal, 2003). Analyses of the sample
linked to the cognitive development of young pre- used in the present study found links between in-
school children than to that of school-age children, creases in income and higher cognitive and language
who have access to more alternative sources of in- performance at age 3 only for children whose fami-
tellectual stimulation and academic training (Brad- lies were poor at the time of the childs birth (Dea-
ley, Corwyn, Burchinal, Pipes-McAdoo, & Garcia ring et al., 2001). A follow-up study found that
Coll, 2001). Young children may also be especially income from birth to age 3, particularly in lower
vulnerable when parental sensitivity is low because parts of the income distribution, was related to both
of their dependence on caregivers and because of the family processes (maternal depression and sensitiv-
nature of early developmental tasksFachieving au- ity) and child cognitive and social competence
tonomy and competent self-regulation (Campbell, (Mistry et al., 2004). Thus, in the present analyses,
Duration and Timing of Poverty 797

income is categorized based on whether families quality) mediate relations of poverty to childrens
were poor or not because this is the distinction that development?
prior research suggests is important to family pro-
cesses and childrens outcomes. Categorizing fami-
Method
lies into poverty groups made it possible to test ex-
plicitly whether the experience of early poverty
Participants
differed from the experience of later poverty in terms
of patterns of change over time in the quality of the Families were recruited through hospital visits to
home environment and, in turn, in child outcomes. mothers shortly after the birth of a child in 1991 in 10
In this study, poverty is defined by a median in- locations in the United States (Little Rock, AR; Or-
come below 200% of the federal poverty threshold. ange County, CA; Lawrence and Topeka, KS; Well-
Although 100% of the federal poverty threshold is esley, MA; Pittsburgh, PA; Philadelphia, PA;
often used to designate poverty, many observers Charlottesville, VA; Seattle, WA; Hickory, NC; and
agree that this index does not represent a minimally Madison, WI). Recruitment and selection procedures
sufficient income (Citro & Michael, 1995). Moreover, are described in several publications (see NICHD
eligibility for some federal programs has been ex- Early Child Care Research Network, 2001b). Of the
panded to 200% of the poverty threshold; thus, this initial pool of eligible mothers contacted for partici-
threshold is particularly relevant to future policy pation, 1,364 completed the 1-month home visit and
decisions. The 200% threshold is also relevant to became study participants. These 1,364 families were
large numbers of U.S. families. In 1998, when the similar to the eligible hospital sample on major de-
children in this study were 7 years old, 18% of chil- mographic characteristics (years of maternal educa-
dren in the United States lived in families with in- tion, ethnicity, and presence of partner in home). The
comes below the federal poverty threshold, and 40% resulting sample was diverse, including 24% ethnic
lived in families with incomes at or below 200% of minority children, 11% mothers who had not com-
poverty (http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/00trends). pleted high school, and 14% single-parent mothers.
Earlier analyses of the NICHD SECC have tested Mothers had an average of 14.4 years of education,
poverty and related risks during the first 3 years of and 51.7% of the children were boys.
life. Children in poor families had lower scores than
did those in nonpoor families on cognitive and pre-
Income Groups
academic tests, lower social competence, and higher
levels of behavior problems at age 3 (Dearing et al., Family income was calculated from mothers re-
2001; Mistry et al., 2004; NICHD Early Child Care ports at each data collection point and included
Research Network, 2001a, 2002). This is the first re- earnings of the mother, earnings of her resident
port from the NICHD SECC to examine the relations husband or partner, and all other sources of house-
of poverty to childrens cognitive and social devel- hold income, including public assistance. An in-
opment beyond age 3, to analyze duration and tim- come-to-needs ratio was calculated for each family at
ing of poverty, and to examine potential home and each data collection time point by dividing the total
child care mediators of poverty effects. family income by the poverty threshold for that
The analyses presented here focus on four groups family size. For each family, the median income-to-
of families that differ on duration and timing of needs ratio was calculated for two age periods in the
poverty: (a) never poor, (b) poor during the childs childs life: The early childhood period was 6, 15, 24,
first 3 years but not later (early poor), (c) poor from and 36 months, and the preschool middle child-
ages 4 to 9 but not before that (late poor), and (d) hood period was 54 months, kindergarten, first
chronically poor. We address two major sets of grade, and third grade.
questions: (a) Are duration and timing of poverty Complete income data were available for 803
associated with variations in home enrichment or families. A family was defined as poor if the median
parenting quality over time? Are these relations ex- income-to-needs ratio was less than 2.0 during that
plained by family and maternal characteristics? and period. Four income groups resulted: never poor
(b) Do duration and developmental timing of pov- (687 families, 64% of the sample), poor early (100
erty predict variations in childrens cognitive/lan- families, 9% of sample), poor late (55 families, 5% of
guage and socioemotional development once family sample), and chronically poor (225 families, 21%
characteristics are controlled? Do experiences in the of the sample). These sample sizes are before
home (measured by home enrichment and maternal imputing data. After imputation, the four income
sensitivity) and child care settings (quantity and groups consisted of 857 (63%) families who were
798 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

never poor, 123 (9%) families who were poor early Quality of the Home Environment
but not late, 70 (5%) families who were poor late but
not early, and 314 (23%) families who were chroni- Home enrichment. The age-appropriate version of
cally poor. the Home Observation for the Measurement of the
Environment (HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 1984)
was administered at 6, 15, 36, and 54 months and at
Overview of Data Collection
third grade. In the infant-toddler version of the
Children at 10 geographic sites were followed HOME scale, used at 6 and 15 months, factor anal-
from birth to third grade. Mothers were interviewed ysis was used to identify an enrichment scale de-
at home when infants were 1 month old. When scribing the availability of stimulating toys and
children were 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months old and in parental efforts to provide stimulation for the child,
first and third grades, we assessed the home and which consisted of nine items at 6 months and eight
family environments. At each age before school en- items at 15 months (a at each age 5 .66). In the pre-
try, we also observed childrens child care environ- school version, used at 36 and 54 months, the enrich-
ments and obtained data on their hours in ment subscale is the mean of three of the standard
nonmaternal care. Childrens cognitive and language subscales: learning materials, academic stimulation,
development was assessed at 24, 36, and 54 months and variety (as 5 .41 to .88 at the two ages). At third
of age and during the first and third grades. Mothers grade, the measure included an eight-item enrich-
and teachers provided information about childrens ment subscale (a 5 .58). Correlations across adjacent
behavior problems at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months and time points ranged from .44 to .65. A total of 885
in kindergarten, first, and third grades. Complete families had data at all five time points and 191 had
descriptions of the data collection procedures used data at four time points. All data collectors were
can be found in the manuals of operation for the trained centrally, and reliability was assessed by
NICHD Study of Early Child Care, which are avail- having each observer code videotaped home visits
able at http://public.rti.org/secc. every 4 months during data collection. Coding of the
videotaped visits was compared with codes gener-
ated by experts. All observers maintained a criterion
Child and Family Characteristics
of 90% agreement with the expert codes.
Child gender and ethnicity were recorded at 1 Maternal sensitivity. Mother child interaction was
month. Partner status (resident husband or partner videotaped in semistructured 15-min observations at
vs. no resident partner) was obtained by maternal 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months and at first and third
interview during telephone calls or in-person inter- grades (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network,
views at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months and at first and 1999). At 6 months, the observation task included
third grades, and complete data were available for 7 min of free play with any toy or object available in
928 families. Maternal education in years was ob- the home or none at all, and 8 min using a standard
tained by interview at 1 month. Mothers were asked set of toys. At 15, 24, 36, and 54 months, a three-
about the number of children in the household boxes task (Vandell, 1979) was used in which
during each interview, and 919 families had data mothers were asked to open and play with three
from all eight ages. The average number of hours per containers of age-appropriate toys in a predeter-
week spent in all jobs was obtained at all eight as- mined order. Videotapes were coded at a single site
sessment points for 858 mothers and 579 fathers or by raters who were unaware of other information
partners. Reported hours employed were stable over about the families. Intercoder reliability was deter-
time, with correlations among adjacent periods mined by independent coding of 20% of the tapes at
ranging from .59 to .67 for the mother and .44 to .61 each assessment period. Intercoder reliability was
for the father or partner. Maternal depressive calculated as the intraclass correlation (Winer, 1971)
symptoms were measured using the Center for Ep- and ranged from .83 to .87 for the maternal sensi-
idemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES D; tivity composite ratings used in the present study.
Radloff, 1977) at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months and at A maternal sensitivity composite variable was
first and third grades. The CES D provides an index constructed at each age. At 6, 15, and 24 months,
of depressive symptoms and is appropriate for use in maternal sensitivity was calculated as the sum of
a nonclinical population (as 5 .88 to .91 in the pres- 4-point ratings of sensitivity to nondistress, positive
ent sample). Correlations across adjacent time points regard, and intrusiveness (reversed). At 36 and 54
ranged from .51 to .58. Data for all seven time points months and at first and third grades, 7-point ratings
were available for 837 mothers. of supportive presence, respect for autonomy, and
Duration and Timing of Poverty 799

hostility (reversed) were combined (a4.70 at every maternal care was created at each age to identify
age). Total scores were transformed into z scores for these children. To include them in all analyses, the
analysis. Correlations across adjacent time points mean quality of care score was assigned to these
ranged from .40 to .51. There were 800 families with children. This missing value technique allows the
complete data at all seven time points and 198 more inclusion of all childrens data in the analysis while
with data at six time points. ensuring that the coefficient for child care quality is
computed only from children who were in child care
(see NICHD Early Child Care Research Network &
Child Care Characteristics
Duncan, 2003, for complete details).
Quantity. Mothers reported at 3- or 4-month in-
tervals on the number of hours per week children
Child Developmental Outcomes
had spent in nonmaternal care. An average weekly
hours of care was computed for each child up to each Cognitive and language development. Childrens
age at which outcome assessments were obtained: development of cognitive and language skills was
24, 36, and 54 months. measured at 24, 36, and 54 months and in first and
Quality. Child care settings were observed at 6, 15, third grades. At 24 months, the Mental Development
24, 36, and 54 months for children who were in child Index from the Bayley Scales of Infant Development,
care for 10 or more hours per week at ages 6 to 36 2nd ed. (Bayley II; Bayley, 1991) was used to assess
months or for 8 or more hours per week at 54 overall developmental status. At 36 months, a com-
months. At each of two visits, observers completed posite score was derived from the Bracken Scale of
two 44-min cycles of the Observational Record of the Basic Concepts (Bracken, 1984) and the Reynell De-
Caregiving Environment (ORCE; NICHD Early velopmental Language Scales (Reynell, 1991). The
Child Care Research Network, 1996, 2000) during School Readiness subscale of the Bracken, which
which time they coded the frequency of specific assesses childrens knowledge of colors, letters,
caregiver behaviors and then rated the quality of the numbers, comparisons, and shapes, was used. The
caregiving on several scales. The ORCE was sys- childrens percentile rank was rescaled to have a
tematically adapted to be age appropriate. At 6, 15, mean of 100 (SD 5 15) and combined with the
and 24 months, composite quality scores were cal- standard scores on the Language Comprehension
culated as the mean of five 4-point subscales (sensi- and Expressive Language subscales of the Reynell to
tivity to childs nondistress signals, stimulation of obtain a general index of childrens cognitive and
childs development, positive regard toward child, language development (a 5 .77).
detachment [reflected], and flatness of affect [re- At 54 months, cognitive, language, and academic
flected]; as 5 .87 to .89). At 36 months, these five performance was assessed with subtests from the
scales plus two additional subscalesFfosters childs Woodcock Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery
exploration and intrusive (reflected)Fwere includ- Revised (Woodcock & Johnson, 1989, 1990) and the
ed in the composite (a 5 .83). At 54 months, the Preschool Language Scale (PLS 3; Zimmerman,
overall quality of caregiving composite was the Steiner, & Pond, 1992). A single score was created as
mean of 4-point ratings of caregivers sensitivity/ the mean of the standard scores on five Woodcock
responsivity, stimulation of cognitive development, Johnson subtests (applied problems, incomplete
intrusiveness (reflected), and detachment (reflected), words, letter word identification, memory for sen-
a 5 .72. tences, and picture vocabulary) and the PLS 3
To ensure that observers at the 10 sites were expressive and receptive subscales (a of composite
making comparable ratings, all observers were cer- index 5 .87). At first grade, a mean of the five
tified before beginning data collection and tested for cognitive and academic subscales from the Wood-
observer drift every 3 to 4 months. Agreement with cock Johnson was used to index cognitive skill
master-coded videotapes and with other observers in (a 5 .79), and at third grade a mean of the two cog-
live observations were evaluated using intraclass nitive subscalesFletter word and applied problems
correlations (i.e., Pearson correlations and repeated Fwas used (a 5 .83). The measures of academic,
measures analysis of variance [ANOVA]; Winer, cognitive, and language development were highly
1971). Reliability exceeded .80 at all ages. correlated across adjacent time points (rs 5 .70 to .87).
Exclusive maternal care. Some children were never Complete data at all five time points were available
observed in child care because they were cared for for 880 children.
exclusively by their mothers at each of the data col- Externalizing and internalizing behavior prob-
lection ages. A dummy variable labeled exclusive lems. Mothers and caregivers or teachers reports
800 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

of the childrens externalizing and internalizing data sets were created in which all observed data are
behavior problems were obtained using the Child represented and missing data are estimated.
Behavior Checklist (CBCL). The CBCL/2 3 (Achen- Analyses were conducted five times, using each of
bach, 1992), used at 24 and 36 months, lists 99 the five imputation data sets. Analyses were com-
problem behaviors that are rated by the mother as bined using the recommended procedures of Schafer
not true (0), somewhat true (1), or very true (2) of her (1997). The test statistics and regression coefficients
child over the last 2 months. For children in at least were combined by averaging them across the five
10 hr of child care a week, the childs regular care- analyses, and the standard errors for the coefficients
giver also completed these forms at 24 and 36 were combined by combining within- and between-
months. The CBCL/4 18 (Achenbach, 1991), com- model variability. Effect sizes (d) were computed as
pleted by mothers at 54 months and first and third the difference between the adjusted mean for the
grades, lists 113 problem behaviors rated on the never-poor group and the mean of the adjusted
same 3-point scale. The 100-item Teacher Report means for the other three groups (early, late, and
Form (TRF; Achenbach, 1991) of the CBCL was chronically poor, considered together), divided by
completed by caregivers at 54 months and teachers the sample standard deviation. This comparison
at kindergarten and first and third grades. Both the between poor and not-poor groups was highlighted
externalizing and internalizing scores, computed to allow a clear description of the magnitude of
using standard T scores based on normative data for poverty effects and the extent to which inclusion of
children of the same age, were used in this study. hypothesized covariates appeared to mediate this
Correlations across adjacent time points for exter- association.
nalizing problems ranged from .66 to .75 for mother There were two stages to the analyses: (a) exam-
reports and from .30 to .54 for caregiver or teacher ining poverty group differences in the quality of
reports, and for internalizing ranged from .54 to .64 parenting and (b) examining poverty group differ-
for mother reports and from .13 to .32 for caregiver or ences in child outcomes, with consideration of pos-
teacher reports. Correlations between maternal and sible mediating effects of parenting and child care
caregiver or teacher ratings ranged from .15 to .41. variables. In the first stage, parenting quality over
Complete mother data were available for 867 chil- time was examined using hierarchical linear models
dren. Complete caregiver or teacher data were (HLM; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Two measures
available for 260 children. (Most missing data on this were examined: (a) the measure of intellectual en-
measure occurred because the child did not have a richment in the family environment from the HOME
child care caregiver at least one age.) and (b) maternal sensitivity in interaction with the
child. The first model included only site, ethnicity,
and child gender as covariates; the second model
Analysis Plan
controlled maternal education, maternal depressive
Missing data occurred in this longitudinal project symptoms, and partner status. All models included
largely because of failure to complete all assess- the main effects of poverty group, the Child Age 
ments, although about 300 families were lost to fol- Poverty Group interaction (testing for linear change),
low-up by third grade. Missing data were imputed and the Age2  Poverty Group interaction (testing
using multiple imputation (Rubin, 1987; Schafer, for quadratic change).
1997; Schafer & Graham, 2002) under the assumption In the second step, to address poverty group dif-
that missing data were ignorably missing. That is, ferences in childrens cognitive and social develop-
given our many longitudinal measures on demo- ment longitudinally and to examine family and child
graphic, child, family, and child care measures, there care processes as potential mediators of these effects
was sufficient information in our data to estimate HLM analyses were used to examine the relations
missing data accurately. Schafers (1997) recom- between poverty and child outcomes and to test for
mended procedure, an iterative expectation max- possible mediators. Poverty groups were compared
imization algorithm was used. Missing values for in models that included only site, ethnicity, and child
each variable are estimated iteratively using a lo- gender as covariates, and then in models that in-
gistic or multiple regression from all the other vari- cluded family characteristics, parenting quality, and
ables using the data for individuals with observed child care quality and quantity. In these models the
values on that variable, and random variability is hypothesis that parenting quality and child care
added as the missing data are predicted. The process characteristics mediated the relation between pov-
is repeated for each variable until the differences in erty group and child outcomes was tested. The in-
predicted values across iterations are miniscule. Five direct path from poverty through the mediator to
Duration and Timing of Poverty 801

child outcome was computed as the product of the Home enrichment. Without the inclusion of family
regression coefficient of the mediator on poverty and characteristics (Model 1 in Table 1), poverty group
the coefficient of the child outcome on the mediator was highly related to enrichment, and the Poverty
(MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, Group  Child Age interaction was significant. The
2002). Separate HLM analyses were used to examine patterns over time are shown in Figure 1. The never-
repeated assessments of academic, cognitive, and poor families provided the highest quality environ-
language development and of externalizing and in- ments, the always-poor families provided the lowest
ternalizing behavior problems as indexed by the quality environments, and the other two fell in be-
CBCL (reported by mothers and caregivers or tween. The patterns of change also differed. Home
teachers). environment quality of families in the late and
In each analysis, quadratic group growth curves chronically poor groups dropped over time com-
were estimated. All predictors including child age pared with the never-poor group. Addition of the
were centered at the sample mean to enhance inter- family characteristics (Model 2) reduced the F value
pretation of the coefficients. In each analysis, we and regression coefficients for the main effect and
crossed child age and age2 with all other predictors. reduced the interaction to a nonsignificant level. The
Poverty group, maternal education, ethnicity, and poverty effect size for Model 1 was .57 and for Model
gender were time-invariant predictors, whereas 2 was .22, a reduction of 61%. Education, partner
child age, partner status, maternal depressive status, and depressive symptoms were all signifi-
symptoms, home enrichment, and maternal sensi- cant. For maternal education, the child age and age2
tivity were time-varying predictors. Child care ex- interactions were also significant, indicating that
periences were treated as a time-varying covariate education has a stronger relation to home enrich-
for the outcomes at ages in which children experi- ment as children get older.
enced child care and as a time-invariant predictor for Maternal sensitivity. There were significant pov-
the outcomes at subsequent ages. That is, time-var- erty group differences in maternal sensitivity in
ying covariates in the models included the concur- Model 1 (see Table 1). The never-poor mothers
rent assessment of family measures for all ages and showed the highest sensitivity and the always-poor
of child care experiences for 24-, 36-, and 54-month mothers showed the lowest. The early and late
outcomes. Time-invariant covariates included ma- groups fell in between and were not different from
ternal education, ethnicity, and gender at all ages, each other, and there were no differences in patterns
and cumulative, average-over-time measures of of change over time. In Model 2, controlling for
child care amount and quality for the school-age maternal education, partner status, and depressive
outcomes. A time-varying dummy variable indicat- symptoms, the income group differences were sub-
ing which version of the CBCL was used at each age stantially smaller. Although the never-poor and
was included in the analyses of the mother- and always-poor groups differed significantly, the early-
teacher-reported externalizing and internalizing and late-poor groups did not differ from the never-
scores. poor group. The early-poor and late-poor mothers
were significantly more sensitive than the always-
poor mothers. The poverty effect size (d) was
Results reduced from .43 in Model 1 to .20 in Model 2, a
All comparisons are reported using the multiply reduction of 53%, and differences in maternal edu-
imputed data sets. In all comparisons we conducted cation seemed largely to account for this change.
five sets of analyses and combined results across
them. Descriptive data for all variables in the anal- Child Cognitive and Social Development
ysis by poverty group are available from the author.
The next set of research questions involved (a) the
relations of poverty duration and timing to childrens
cognitive and social development, and (b) the extent
Home Enrichment and Parenting Quality
to which poverty group differences were accounted
The first set of analyses tested differences among for by home enrichment, parenting quality, or child
the four poverty groups in home enrichment and care quantity or quality. To address these questions
parenting quality over time. Maternal education, longitudinally, two sets of HLM analyses described
partner status, and maternal depressive symptoms longitudinal measures of cognitive and language
were examined as variables that might account for development and of externalizing and internalizing
any differences. Results are shown in Table 1. behavior problems. The first model included site,
802 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Table 1
Poverty Group Comparisons on Parenting Quality From 6 Months to Grade 3

Maternal sensitivity: Mother child


Home enrichment factor interactions

Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2

Poverty group F 116.16 20.20 64.38 15.97


Never M (SE) 0.21 (0.03)a 0.06 (0.03)a 0.16 (0.02)a 0.06 (0.03)a
Late M (SE) 0.25 (0.09)b 0.08 (0.08)a 0.16 (0.06)b 0.02 (0.10)a
Early M (SE) 0.14 (0.07)b 0.02 (0.06)a 0.15 (0.09)b 0.07 (0.06)a
Always M (SE) 0.70 (0.05)c 0.36 (0.05)b 0.55 (0.04)c 0.32 (0.05)b
Poverty Group  Child Age F 6.94 0.87 1.82 1.49
Never linear slope B (SE) 0.01 (0.01)a 0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01)
Late linear slope B (SE) 0.04 (0.02)b 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02)
Early linear slope B (SE) 0.00 (0.01)a 0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01)
Always linear slope B (SE) 0.04 (0.01)b 0.01 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01)
Poverty Group  Age2 F 0.69 1.50 1.34 1.20
Never quad slope B (SE) 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (.000) 0.00 (0.00)
Late quad slope B (SE) 0.01 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01)
Early quad slope B (SE) 0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01)
Always quad slope B (SE) 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00)
Family characteristics
Maternal education 0.12 (0.01) 0.09 (0.01)
Maternal Education  Child Age 0.01 (0.00)
Maternal Education  Age2 0.00 (0.00)
Partner in household 0.25 (0.05) 0.10 (0.05)
Maternal depression 0.01 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00)
Maternal Depression  Child Age 0.00 (0.00)
Maternal Depression  Age2 0.00 (0.00)

Note. Model 1 adjusted for site, child ethnicity, and child gender and interactions between those variables and both child age and age2.
Model 2 added maternal education, partner in household, and maternal depression and their interactions with child age and age2 (in-
teraction coefficients listed only when significantly different from zero to save space). Adjusted means and slope coefficients with different
subscripts are statistically significant from each other. Missing data were imputed and adjusted means and comparisons are combined
across separate analyses of the imputed data sets. The median F statistic is reported.
po.05. po.01. po.001.

child sex, and ethnicity as covariates, and the second Cognitive and language development. Table 2 shows
model added family characteristics and the hypoth- the adjusted means and their standard errors for the
esized parenting and child care mediators. four poverty groups for each model, the coefficients
for the the Income Group  Child Age and Income
1 Group  Age2 interactions, and the coefficients for
Never
Late the hypothesized mediators. None of the interactions
HOME Enrichment Scale

0.5
Early between the hypothesized mediators and child age
Always
or age2 was significant. Figure 2 shows the group
growth curves for the four income groups as esti-
0 mated with Model 1. Results of Model 1 indicate
significant poverty group differences in cognitive
0.5 status. As shown in Figure 2, children from families
who were never poor scored highest, and children
from families who were always poor scored lowest.
1
0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 6 6.5 7 7.5 8 8.5
Adding the family characteristics, parenting, and
Age in Years child care variables (Model 2) substantially reduced
the income group differences, but they remained
Figure 1. Home enrichment from 6 months to third grade in fam-
ilies from four income groups. Data represent standardized scores significant. Mediation analysis indicated that the
from the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment quality of the home environment (z 5 4.14, po.001)
(HOME) scale. and maternal sensitivity (z 5 3.34, po.01) were sig-
Duration and Timing of Poverty 803

Table 2 115
Poverty Group Comparisons on Development of Cognitive Skills From 2 Never
110 Late
Years to Grade 3 Early

Cognitive Composite
105 Always
Model 1 Model 2
100
Poverty group F 74.66 15.12
95
Never M (SE) 102.4 (0.39)a 100.5 (0.53)a
Late M (SE) 96.3 (1.39)b 98.1 (1.33)b 90
Early M (SE) 97.7 (1.04)b 98.8 (1.06)b
Always M (SE) 91.3 (0.65)c 95.1 (0.75)c 85

Poverty Group  Child F 2.99 2.38 80


Age 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 6 6.5 7 7.5 8 8.5
Never linear slope B (SE) 2.25 (0.07)ac 2.54 (0.19) Age in Years
Late linear slope B (SE) 1.79 (0.34)a 2.18 (0.38) Figure 2. Indexes of childrens cognitive language skill from 6
Early linear slope B (SE) 2.62 (0.17)b 2.90 (0.30) months to third grade by income group. Data represent a stand-
Always linear slope B (SE) 2.37 (0.13)bc 2.54 (0.25) ardized composite score based on age-appropriate measures at
Poverty Group  Age2 F 1.05 0.23 each time point.
Never quad slope B (SE) 0.05 (0.03) 0.11 (0.06)
Late quad slope B (SE) 0.00 (0.10) 0.12 (0.11) groups. The mothers in all four poverty groups re-
Early quad slope B (SE) 0.04 (0.08) 0.11 (0.09) ported fewer problems over time, but the patterns of
Always quad slope B (SE) 0.03 (0.04) 0.07 (0.08) change were different. Maternal ratings of external-
Family characteristics izing problems in the never-poor group declined less
Maternal education B (SE) 1.28 (0.15) during the preschool years and more during the
Partner in household B (SE) 0.51 (0.54)
school-age years; by contrast, externalizing ratings in
Maternal depression B (SE) 0.03 (0.02)
Exclusive maternal care B (SE) 0.24 (0.46)
the always-poor group accelerated as the children
Hypothesized mediators
reached school age.
Home enrichment B (SE) 1.45 (0.23) Adding family characteristics and hypothesized
Maternal sensitivity B (SE) 0.83 (0.22) mediators reduced the overall differences among the
Child care hours B (SE) 0.00 (0.01) income groups. All three covariatesFmaternal ed-
Child care quality B (SE) 0.38 (0.35) ucation, partner status, and maternal depressive
symptomsFpredicted externalizing ratings. In ad-
Note. Model 1 adjusted for site, child ethnicity, and child gender
dition, child care hours predicted declines in ma-
and interactions between those variables and both child age and
age2. Model 2 added parenting and child care variables (interac- ternal ratings of externalizing problems during the
tion coefficients listed only when significantly different from zero preschool years but became a nonsignificant pre-
to save space). Adjusted means and slope coefficients with dif- dictor after the transition to school. However, the
ferent subscripts are statistically significant from each other.
Missing data were imputed, and adjusted means and comparisons indirect paths from income group through the hy-
are combined across separate analyses of the imputed data sets. pothesized mediators to maternal report of exter-
The median F statistic is reported. nalizing problems were all nonsignificant. Overall,
po.05, po.001.
the addition of all covariates and mediators reduced
the poverty effect size from .30 to .16, a reduction of
nificant mediators, providing evidence that at least 47% from Model 1 to Model 2.
part of the poverty effect was due to indirect paths Teacher-reported externalizing problems showed
from poverty through parenting to cognitive skills. an overall effect of poverty group in Model 1 (see
The poverty effect size (d) was .52 for Model 1 and Table 3 and Figure 4). Again, the mean scores do not
.23 in Model 3, a reduction of 56%. suggest any group to be at risk for clinical levels of
Externalizing behavior problems. Significant pover- behavior problems. Children from families in the
ty group differences emerged in mother-reported always-poor group had higher scores than did chil-
externalizing T scores in Model 1 for both overall dren in the never- and early-poor groups. Adding
levels and patterns of change over time (see Table 3). the family characteristics and hypothesized media-
It is important to note, however, that none of the tors reduced the group effect to nonsignificance.
adjusted mean scores on the externalizing scale Lower ratings of externalizing problems were asso-
reached the at-risk or clinical level. As shown in ciated with higher maternal education, having a
Figure 3, mothers in the late- and always-poor partner in the household when children were older,
groups reported significantly more behavior prob- more sensitive mothers, and fewer child care hours.
lems than did mothers in the early- and never-poor In Model 2, three of the four hypothesized mediators
804 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Table 3
Poverty Group Comparisons on CBCL Ratings of Externalizing Problems From 2 Years to Grade 3

Caregiver and teacher ratings of


Maternal ratings of externalizing externalizing

Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2

Poverty group F 14.06 3.94 7.71 1.29


Never M (SE) 49.78 (0.30)a 50.03 (0.37)a 49.40 (0.29)a 50.07 (0.34)
Late M (SE) 54.34 (1.11)b 53.31 (1.07)b 50.62 (1.02)ab 50.31 (0.99)
Early M (SE) 50.55 (0.80)a 50.02 (0.81)a 49.68 (0.75)a 49.49 (0.72)
Always M (SE) 52.99 (0.56)b 51.35 (0.63)ab 52.18 (0.57)b 50.96 (0.64)
Poverty Group  Child Age F 0.41 0.21 2.45 1.10
Never linear slope B (SE) 0.52 (0.08) 0.73 (0.16) 1.01 (0.16) 1.04 (0.27)
Late linear slope B (SE) 0.35 (0.26) 0.65 (0.29) 0.96 (0.50) 1.08 (0.53)
Early linear slope B (SE) 0.58 (0.21) 0.72 (0.27) 0.29 (0.39) 0.35 (0.41)
Always linear slope B (SE) 0.74 (0.13) 0.92 (0.21) 0.99 (0.26) 0.95 (0.31)
Poverty Group  Age2 F 4.72 1.44 3.88 2.75
Never quadratic slope B (SE) 0.10 (0.02)a 0.06 (0.05) 0.12 (0.04)a 0.08 (0.07)a
Late quadratic slope B (SE) 0.09 (0.07) 0.05 (0.08) 0.09 (0.12) 0.10 (0.14)
Early quadratic slope B (SE) 0.02 (0.05) 0.02 (0.06) 0.08 (0.09)b 0.10 (0.10)b
Always quadratic slope B (SE) 0.03 (0.04)b 0.03 (0.06) 0.01 (0.07) 0.04 (0.09)
Family characteristics
Maternal education B (SE) 0.29 (0.10) 0.29 (0.11)
Partner in household B (SE) 1.11 (0.38) 0.65 (0.57)
Partner  Age B (SE) 1.10 (0.35)
Partner  Age2 B (SE) 0.22 (0.09)
Maternal depression B (SE) 0.10 (0.02) 0.03 (0.03)
Exclusive maternal care B (SE) 0.26 (0.43) 1.41 (2.46)
Hypothesized Mediators
Home enrichment B (SE) 0.25 (0.16) 0.49 (0.25)
Maternal sensitivity B (SE) 0.21 (0.13) 0.83 (0.19)
Child care hours B (SE) 0.01 (0.01) 0.06 (0.01)
Child Care Hours  Child Age B (SE) 0.01 (0.01)
Child Care Hours  Age2 B (SE) 0.00 (0.00)
Child care quality B (SE) 0.53 (0.29) 0.07 (0.38)

Note. Model 1 adjusted for site, child ethnicity, and child gender and interactions between those variables and both child age and age2.
Model 2 added parenting and child care variables (interaction coefficients listed only when significantly different from zero to save space).
Adjusted means and slope coefficients with different subscripts are statistically significant from each other. Missing data were imputed,
and adjusted means and comparisons are combined across separate analyses of the imputed data sets. CBCL 5 Child Behavior Checklist.
The median F statistic is reported.
po.05. po.01. po.001.

showed significant indirect paths: home enrichment and patterns of change over time (see Table 4 and
(z 5 2.07, po.05), maternal sensitivity (z 5 3.25, Figure 5). As with the externalizing scores, the mean
po.01), and hours of child care (z 5 3.73, po.001). level of internalizing behavior problems reported
Thus, it appears that poverty is related to teacher did not indicate clinical risk. Mothers in the late and
reports of externalizing problems in part through always-poor groups reported significantly more in-
less enriching and sensitive parenting, and that ternalizing problems than did mothers in the never-
fewer hours of child care counteract possible poverty poor group. The mothers in all four groups reported
effects. The effect size (d) was reduced from .15 to fewer problems over time, but only the never-poor
.02 for teacher ratings of externalizing problems, a group showed a linear pattern of decline. The ratings
87% change. by mothers in the other three groups showed more
Internalizing behavior problems. Significant poverty decline in internalizing problems during the pre-
group differences emerged in mother-reported in- school years and a leveling off during the school-age
ternalizing T scores in Model 1 for both overall levels years.
Duration and Timing of Poverty 805

60 four hypothesized mediators showed significant in-


Never
direct paths: home enrichment (z 5 2.05, po.05)
CBCL (Mother) Externalzing

Late
Early and hours of child care (z 5 2.21, po.05). The poverty
Always
55 effect size (d) was reduced from .10 to .02 for teacher
ratings of externalizing problems, an 80% change.

50 Discussion
This prospective longitudinal examination of dura-
tion and developmental timing of poverty confirmed
45
2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 6 6.5 7 7.5 8 8.5 the previously well-documented associations of
Age in Years negative outcomes with poverty but went beyond
Figure 3. Childrens externalizing behavior T scores reported by
these findings to suggest some of the processes by
mothers from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) from 2 years to which poverty affects children. In this sample, chil-
third grade by income group. dren in persistently poor families had the lowest
levels of performance of the four groups on tests of
Adding covariates reduced differences among the language and school-readiness skill, and their scores
income groups, eliminating significant main effects were significantly different from those in families
and reducing differences in patterns of change over that experienced shorter term poverty. Both mothers
time. In Model 2, fewer internalizing problem be- and teachers rated children in chronically poor and
haviors were reported by mothers who had more late-poor families as having more externalizing and
education, a partner, and fewer depressive symp- internalizing behavior problems than did those in
toms. In addition, internalizing scores were lower the other groups. Analyses of externalizing and in-
when mothers were more sensitive, but this trend ternalizing behavior problems yielded similar re-
was stronger when children were younger than sults, with no indication of different patterns in
when they were older. However, none of the poten- different poverty groups.
tial mediators made significant contributions. Over- It is clear that advantages and disadvantages
all, the addition of all covariates and hypothesized cluster in families; when there is economic disad-
mediators reduced the poverty effect size from .19 to vantage, there also tends to be psychosocial difficulty
.09, a reduction of 53% from Model 1. and lowered intellectual resources both at home and
Teacher-reported internalizing problems showed in child care. The chronically poor families were
an overall effect of poverty group (see Table 4 and more seriously and consistently disadvantaged than
Figure 6). Children from families in the always-poor those in transitory poverty, not only because they
group had higher scores than did children in the had lower incomes, but on almost every indicator
never- and early-poor groups. Adding the covariates measured. Unlike chronically poor families, those
reduced the group effect to nonsignificance, with that experienced transitory poverty maintained ad-
lower ratings of internalizing problems associated equate levels of childrearing quality even while their
with higher HOME enrichment scores. Two of the incomes were limited. Mothers in the early- and late-
poor families showed similar levels of sensitivity as
60 those in the never-poor families, suggesting an un-
Never derlying resiliency or access to supportive environ-
TRF (Teacher) Externalzing

Late
Early ments that may help some families avoid chronic
Always
55 poverty.
Overall, developmental timing of poverty had less
consistent relations to childrens development than
50
did poverty duration. For the most part, the cogni-
tive and social behavior scores of children in transi-
tory poverty fell between those in chronically poor
families and those in never-poor families. Where
45
2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 6 6.5 7 7.5 8 8.5 differences between early and late poverty occurred,
Age in Years children who experienced poverty later (age 4 9
Figure 4. Childrens externalizing behavior T score as reported by
years) had less favorable developmental outcomes
caregivers and teachers from 2 years to third grade by income than did those whose families were poor in the first 3
group. TRF 5 Teacher Report Form. years of their lives. Children in early- and late-poor
806 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Table 4
Poverty Group Comparisons on CBCL Ratings of Internalizing Problems From 2 Years to Grade 3

Caregiver and teacher ratings of


Maternal ratings of internalizing internalizing

Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2

Poverty group F 4.32 1.11 4.87 1.72


Never M (SE) 47.31 (0.28)a 48.10 (0.41) 48.60 (0.33)a 48.66 (0.62)
Late M (SE) 49.84 (1.06)b 49.66 (1.04) 49.80 (1.03) 49.09 (1.02)
Early M (SE) 48.70 (0.77) 49.01 (0.77) 48.25 (0.71)a 47.75 (0.83)
Always M (SE) 48.78 (0.49)b 48.17 (0.58) 50.68 (0.64)b 49.58 (0.62)
Poverty Group  Child Age F 10.47 2.86 0.76 1.77
Never linear slope B (SE) 0.37 (0.06)b 0.55 (0.12) 0.20 (0.09) .00 (0.16)
Late linear slope B (SE) 0.54 (0.24)b 0.54 (0.26) 0.07 (0.37) 0.28 (0.38)
Early linear slope B (SE) 0.38 (0.16)b 0.30 (0.19)b 0.29 (0.24) 0.19 (0.26)
Always linear slope B (SE) 1.03 (0.12)a 0.85 (0.16)a 0.17 (0.20) 0.12 (0.24)
Poverty Group  Age2 F 13.30 4.66 2.95 0.61
Never quadratic slope B (SE) 0.14 (0.02)b 0.11 (0.04)b 0.07 (0.03)a 0.13 (0.07)
Late quadratic slope B (SE) 0.26 (0.08)ab 0.21 (0.09) 0.15 (0.12) 0.18 (0.13)
Early quadratic slope B (SE) 0.19 (0.06)ac 0.11 (0.07)b 0.23 (0.11)b 0.24 (0.12)
Always quadratic slope B (SE) 0.40 (0.05)c 0.30 (0.06)a 0.21 (0.11)b 0.17 (0.09)
Family characteristics
Maternal education B (SE) 0.16 (0.11) 0.23 (0.12)
Maternal Education  Age B (SE) 0.09 (0.03)
Partner in household B (SE) 0.97 (0.45) 0.04 (0.68)
Maternal depression B (SE) 0.10 (0.02) 0.02 (0.04)
Exclusive maternal care B (SE) 0.72 (0.44) 0.45 (0.72)
Mediators
Home enrichment B (SE) 0.03 (0.18) 0.58 (0.31)
Maternal sensitivity B (SE) 0.07 (0.21) 0.01 (0.23)
Maternal Sensitivity  Age B (SE) 0.10 (0.05)
Child care hours B (SE) 0.01 (0.01) 0.03 (0.01)
Child care quality B (SE) 0.46 (0.38) 0.30 (0.68)

Note. Model 1 adjusted for site, child ethnicity, and child gender and interactions between those variables and both child age and age2.
Model 2 added parenting and child care variables (interaction coefficients listed only when significantly different from zero to save space).
Adjusted means and slope coefficients with different subscripts are statistically significant from each other. Missing data were imputed,
and adjusted means and comparisons are combined across separate analyses of the imputed data sets. CBCL 5 Child Behavior Checklist.
The median F statistic is reported.
po.05. po.01. po.001.

families did not differ significantly on cognitive the late-poor group is consistent with studies
performance or teacher-rated behavior problems, but showing that changes in economic conditions lead to
mothers in late-poor families rated their children increased behavior problems among young children
higher on externalizing and internalizing behavior (Hofferth, Smith, McLoyd, & Finkelstein, 2000). One
problems than did those in early-poor families. possible explanation for the differences in children
These findings are not consistent with the hypothesis from the early- versus late-poor groups may be
that early poverty is more detrimental to children found in their family environments. The quality of
than later poverty (e.g., Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, enrichment in the home appeared to be declining
1997). Instead, early transitory poverty seems less over time in the late-poor families, especially at the
detrimental to childrens cognitive and social de- ages at which many of the child outcomes were
velopment than long-term or concurrent poverty. collected.
A striking difference between children in early- Parenting quality and child care quality and
and late-poor families was the higher maternal rat- quantity were examined as mediators of poverty
ings on behavior problems for children in late group effects on child outcomes. Family investment
poverty. The finding of higher behavior problems in in parenting, as indexed by home enrichment and
Duration and Timing of Poverty 807

60 depression and a possible bias to describe children


Never more negatively among depressed mothers. Others
Late
Early have noted the strong relation between maternal
Always depressive symptoms and mother reports of child
55
behavior problems (Berg, Turid, Vika, & Dahl, 2003;
Friedlander, Weiss, & Traylor, 1986; Rose, Rose, &
Feldman, 1989). A link between depression and
50 mother reports of child behavior problems may have
masked any mediating effect of parenting quality.
Child care played a less important role in ex-
plaining differences among poverty groups. The
45 lower quality of nonmaternal care that poor children
2 3 4 5 6 7 8
received was not a significant mediator in any of the
Figure 5. Childrens internalizing behavior T score as reported by analyses. Earlier analyses of the NICHD SECC
mothers from 2 years to third grade by income group. showed higher teacher-reported behavior problems
for children with higher cumulative amounts of child
care from birth through age 412 (NICHD Early Child
maternal sensitivity, appeared to mediate poverty Care Research Network, 2003a). Children from poor
group effects on childrens cognitive and language families had lower cumulative amounts of care than
performance. This result is consistent with theories did those from nonpoor families, and this lower
that link economic disadvantage with poor outcomes quantity of care appeared to reduce poverty effects
for children through diminished quality of parenting on teacher-reported externalizing and internalizing
(Hashima & Amato, 1994; McLoyd, 1997, 1998) and behavior problems. Prior results from the NICHD
lowered material, human, and social capital (Foster, SECC have also shown that family factors predict
2002). Parenting quality less consistently mediated child outcomes in all domains better than do child
poverty group effects on social outcomes, in accord care factors (NICHD Early Child Care Research
with results reported by Yeung, Linver, and Brooks- Network, 2002, 2003b). The separation of family and
Gunn (2002). Although mediation analyses sug- child care characteristics is never as complete in the
gested that poverty effects on teacher reports of real world as it is in our analyses, however. Families
externalizing and internalizing behavior problems choose whether or not to use nonmaternal care and
were mediated through parenting quality, the select child care environments; these choices are
indirect paths through parenting quality were not based at least partially on income and on factors
significant for mother reports. Other family charac- closely related to income, such as work hours and
teristics, particularly maternal depressive symptoms, family composition. Indeed, there are complex in-
were consistently linked to mothers ratings of their terrelations among all characteristics of childrens
childrens behavior, and it is likely that these asso- environments.
ciations reflect both a negative impact of maternal Although the analyses reported here provide new
information about the role of persistent and inter-
mittent poverty in the lives of young children, sev-
60 eral important questions remain. First, far fewer
Never
Late
families experienced transitory poverty (either early
Early or late) than either persistent poverty or no poverty.
Always
55
Accordingly, there was much less power to distin-
guish the early- and late-poor groups from each
other or from the always- or never-poor groups.
Differences involving these groups could be identi-
50 fied if effect sizes were moderate or large, but not
small. Second, the NICHD SECC sample, although
drawn from 10 locations around the United States,
45
was not selected to be nationally representative. A
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 particular limitation to the present study, given the
Figure 6. Childrens internalizing behavior T score as reported by
ethnic disparity in income in the United States, is
caregivers and teachers from 2 years to third grade by income that representation of ethnic minority groups is not
group. adequate to allow analyses of ethnic differences
808 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

within groups. Further research into the duration other than those examined in this report, such as
and timing of poverty for families from different poor nutrition or inadequate health care, may ac-
ethnic and cultural groups is clearly needed if we are count for some of the findings. The results reported
to understand how childrens development is af- here provide only a broad overview of the relation
fected by poverty. Because the present sample was between poverty and family processes and resources
not selected specifically to examine poverty, the four but suggest some important questions for future in-
poverty groups differ substantially in size, limiting vestigation using selected samples of families.
the generality of the conclusions that can be made Clearly, families who live close to the poverty line
from these results. The proportion of families in our face many challenges that are interconnected and
sample who experienced poverty at some point that affect childrens experiences in complex ways.
mirrors national trends, however. Recent statistics
indicate that approximately 40% of U.S. families
have incomes under 200% of poverty; in our sample, References
37% of the families fell into one of the three poverty
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior
groups.
Checklist/4 18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of
Third, whenever family processes and childrens Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
development are examined over extended periods, Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the Child Behavior
as was done in the present study, measurement is- Checklist/2 3 and 1992 Profile. Burlington: University of
sues are of concern. Certain indicator variables that Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
could be measured repeatedly were used in this Ackerman, B., Brown, E., & Izard, C. (2004a). The relations
study, but these do not necessarily capture the in- between contextual risk, earned income, and the school
tended constructs in as much detail as would be adjustment of children from economically disadvan-
ideal. The CES-D was used as an indicator of overall taged families. Developmental Psychology, 40, 204 216.
maternal psychological well-being. This measure can Ackerman, B., Brown, E., & Izard, C. (2004b). The relations
be used repeatedly and gives an indication of de- between persistent poverty and contextual risk and
childrens behavior in elementary school. Developmental
pressive symptoms, but it does not yield a valid
Psychology, 40, 367 377.
measure of clinical depression and it does not cap- Bayley, N. (1991). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd
ture other aspects of maternal psychological func- ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.
tioning. Similarly, the CBCL was the index of child Berg, N., Turid, S., Vika, A., & Dahl, A. A. (2003). When
social development; this measure has been validated adolescents disagree with their mothers: CBCL YSR
for use with children from toddlerhood through discrepancies related to maternal depression and
adolescence and is therefore especially useful in adolescent self-esteem. Child: Care, Health, and Develop-
longitudinal analyses. Although widely used in ment, 29, 207 213.
similar studies, the CBCL provides only a limited Bolger, K., Patterson, C., Thompson, W., & Kupersmidt,
picture of childrens overall social development, as J. (1995). Psychosocial adjustment among children ex-
its intended use is to identify behavior problems periencing persistent and intermittent family economic
hardship. Child Development, 66, 1107 1129.
calling for clinical intervention. Within a normative
Bracken, B. A. (1984). Bracken Basic Concept Scale. San
sample such as the present one, relatively few chil- Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
dren fall in the clinical or even at-risk range. The goal Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic
of this study was not to identify such children but to status and child development. Annual Review of Psy-
index overall social development. As interest in chology, 53, 377 399.
long-term longitudinal research grows, the need for Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., Burchinal, M., Pipes-McA-
additional measures that provide a multidimen- doo, H., & Garcia Coll, C. (2001). The home environ-
sional look at important family processes and child ments of children in the United States Part II: Relations
developmental outcomes across a wide range of with behavioral development through age thirteen.
child ages becomes more crucial. Child Development, 72, 1868 1886.
The present study was designed to address spe- Burchinal, M., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Bryant, D., & Clifford,
R. (2000). Childrens social and cognitive development
cific questions about differences among groups de-
and child care quality: Testing for differential associa-
fined by poverty status at particular points in tions related to poverty, gender, or ethnicity. Applied
childrens lives. Further research is needed to iden- Developmental Science, 4, 149 165.
tify the underlying causes for family poverty and for Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation for
gains and losses of income over time and to examine Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock: University
more closely the mechanisms of influence that link of Arkansas, Center for Research on Teaching and
poverty to deleterious outcomes for children. Factors Learning.
Duration and Timing of Poverty 809

Campbell, S. B. (1995). Behavior problems in preschool McLeod, J., & Shanahan, M. (1993). Poverty, parenting, and
children: A review of recent research. Journal of Child childrens mental health. American Sociological Review, 58,
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36, 351 366.
113 149. McLoyd, V. C. (1997). The impact of poverty and low so-
Citro, C. F., & Michael, R. T. (1995). Measuring poverty: A cioeconomic status on the socioemotional functioning of
new approach. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. African-American children and adolescents: Mediating
Costello, E. J., Compton, S., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). effects. In R. W. Taylor & M. C. Wang (Eds.), Social and
Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: A emotional adjustment and family relations in ethnic minority
natural experiment. Journal of the American Medical As- families (pp. 7 34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
sociation, 290, 2023 2029. McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and
Dearing, E., McCartney, K., & Taylor, B. A. (2001). Change child development. American Psychologist, 53, 185 204.
in family income-to-needs matters more for children Mistry, R., Biesanz, J., Taylor, L., Burchinal, M., & Cox, M.
with less. Child Development, 72, 1779 1793. (2004). Family income and its relation to preschool
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1997). Consequences of childrens adjustment for families in the NICHD
growing up poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Study of Early Child Care. Developmental Psychology, 40,
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). Family poverty, 727 745.
welfare reform, and child development. Child Develop- National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
ment, 71, 188 196. ment Early Child Care Research Network. (1996).
Duncan, G., Yeung, W. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Smith, J. Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing
(1998). How much does childhood poverty affect the life to positive caregiving. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
chances of children? American Sociological Review, 63, 11, 269 306.
406 423. National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
Foster, E. M. (2002). How economists think about family ment Early Child Care Research Network. (1997).
resources and child development. Child Development, 73, Familial factors associated with the characteristics of
1904 1914. nonmaternal care for infants. Journal of Marriage and the
Friedlander, S., Weiss, D., & Traylor, J. (1986). Assessing the Family, 59, 389 408.
influence of maternal depression on the validity of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
Child Behavior Checklist. Journal of Abnormal Child ment Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Child
Psychology, 14, 123 133. care and mother child interaction in the first three
Garrett, P., Ngandu, N., & Ferron, J. (1994). Poverty ex- years of life. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1399 1413.
periences of young children and the quality of their National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
home environments. Child Development, 65, 331 345. ment Early Child Care Research Network. (2000).
Hashima, P. Y., & Amato, P. R. (1994). Poverty, social sup- Characteristics and quality of child care for toddlers and
port, and parental behavior. Child Development, 65, preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Science, 4,
394 403. 116 135.
Hofferth, S. L., Smith, J., McLoyd, V. C., & Finkelstein, J. National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
(2000). Achievement and behavior among children of welfare ment Early Child Care Research Network. (2001a).
recipients, welfare leavers, and low income single mothers. Before Head Start: Income and ethnicity, family charac-
Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan. teristics, child care experiences, and child development.
Korenman, S., Miller, J., & Sjaastad, J. (1995). Long-term Early Education and Development, 12, 545 576.
poverty and child development in the United States: National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
Results from the NLSY. Children and Youth Services Re- ment Early Child Care Research Network. (2001b).
view, 17, 127 155. Nonmaternal care and family factors in early develop-
Linver, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Kohen, D. (2002). Family ment: An overview of the NICHD Study of Early Child
processes as pathways from income to young childrens Care. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 22,
development. Developmental Psychology, 38, 719 734. 457 492.
Lipman, E. L., & Offord, D. R. (1997). Psychosocial mor- National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
bidity among poor children in Ontario. In G. J. Duncan ment Early Child Care Research Network. (2002). The
& J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Consequences of growing up poor interaction of child care and family risk in relation to
(pp. 239 297). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. child development at 24 and 36 months. Applied Devel-
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, opmental Science, 6, 144 156.
S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
test mediation and other intervening variable effects. ment Early Child Care Research Network. (2003a). Does
Psychological Methods, 7, 83 104. amount of time spent in child care predict socioemo-
McLeod, J., & Nonnemaker, J. (2000). Poverty and child tional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten?
emotional and behavioral problems: Racial-ethnic Child Development, 74, 1454 1475.
differences in processes and effects. Journal of Health and National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
Social Behavior, 41, 137 161. ment Early Child Care Research Network. (2003b).
810 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Families matterFeven for kids in child care. Journal of Schafer, J. L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data.
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 24, 58 62. London: Capman & Hall.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop- Schafer, J., & Graham, J. (2002). Missing data: Our view
ment Early Child Care Research Network & Duncan, G. of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147 177.
(2003). Modeling the impacts of child care quality on Vandell, D. L. (1979). Effects of a playgroup experience on
childrens preschool cognitive development. Child De- mother son and father son interaction. Developmental
velopment, 74, 1454 1475. Psychology, 15, 379 385.
Phillips, D., Voran, M., Kisker, E., Howes, C., & Whitbook, Votruba-Drzal, E. (2003). Income changes and cognitive
M. (1994). Child care for children in poverty: Opportu- stimulation in young childrens home learning envi-
nity or inequity? Child Development, 65, 472 492. ronments. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 341 355.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES D Scale: A self-report de- Winer, B. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design
pression scale for research in the general population. (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385 401. Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock
Raudenbush, S., & Bryk, A. (2002). Hierarchical linear mod- Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery Revised. Itasca, IL:
els: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Riverside.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1990). Woodcock
Reynell, J. (1991). Reynell Developmental Language Scale (U.S. Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery Revised. Allen, TX:
ed.). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. DLM Teaching Resources.
Rose, S. L., Rose, S. A., & Feldman, J. (1989). Stability of Yeung, W. J., Linver, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). How
behavior problems in very young children. Development money matters for young childrens development: Pa-
and Psychopathology, 1, 5 19. rental investment and family processes. Child Develop-
Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in ment, 73, 1861 1879.
surveys. New York: Wiley. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1992).
Rutter, M. (1989). Pathways from childhood to adult life. Preschool Language Scale 3. San Antonio, TX: Psycho-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Dis- logical Corporation.
ciplines, 30, 23 51.

You might also like