You are on page 1of 20

www.psicologia.

pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

PSYCHOPATHY OR PSYCHOPATHIC PERSONALITIES


A DIMENSIONAL APPROACH TO THE MASK OF EVIL

2016

Cludio Vieira
Mestrado Integrado em Psicologia (Universidade de Coimbra)
Mestrando em Psiquiatria Forense (Kings College of London)
Coordenador e Supervisor de Terapias Psicolgicas
Servios Forenses para Perturbaes da Personalidade
Sistema Nacional de Sade Britnico & Ministrio da Justia

E-mail de contato:
claudio.vieira@kcl.ac.uk

RESUMO

Psychopathy covers a wide range of emotional, interpersonal, and behavioural features,


including empathy deficits, callousness, impulsivity, and an overall indifference for the societal
norms and the rights of others . It shows a robust association with aggressive behaviour, and its
diagnosis is often used as a strong risk factor in predicting general and violent reoffending , even
when other factors such as delinquent peers, intelligence, self-control and family background are
controlled.
Psychopathy is commonly classified as a distinct disorder measured by an overall PCL-R
score; yet there is still a debate on whether it is a result of partially independent causal processes
or rather it constitutes a coherent syndrome. The aim of this article is thus to set a discussion on
this question, providing up-to-date evidences alongside significant pieces of research.

Palavras-chave: antisocial behaviour, psychopathy, violence, aggression.

Cludio Vieira 1 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

INTRODUCTION

Antisocial personality traits, including callousness, impulsivity and manipulativeness, have


probably been present since the ascent of man [1,2]. It is estimated that 0.61.2% of the community
and 1530% of the prison population in the Western world can be diagnosed as psychopath. Further
research also indicates that although psychopaths are represented by 1% in general population,
they were likely responsible for 50% of all violent offending [3,4].

Psychopathy covers a wide range of emotional, interpersonal, and behavioural features,


including empathy deficits, callousness, impulsivity, and an overall indifference for the societal
norms and the rights of others [5,6,7,15]. It shows a robust association with aggressive behaviour,
and its diagnosis is often used as a strong risk factor in predicting general and violent reoffending
[7], even when other factors such as delinquent peers, intelligence, self-control and family
background are controlled [8].

Psychopathy is commonly classified as a distinct disorder measured by an overall PCL-R


score; yet there is still a debate on whether it is a result of partially independent causal processes
or rather it constitutes a coherent syndrome. The aim of this essay is thus to set a discussion on this
question, providing up-to-date evidences alongside significant pieces of research.

PSYCHOPATHY AN OVERVIEW

Up to the 19th century, psychopathy was perceived as a demoniac personality or rather an


evil spiritual state as opposed to a mental disorder [8,9].
Philippe Pinel, a French psychiatrist, was the first clinician to suggest that a specific group
of patients exhibited manie sans dlire (insanity without delirium); that this, individuals with no
intellectual difficulties but with a profound deficit in behaviour typified by marked impulsivity and
aggressive acts, substance misuse, and immorality that would occur in the absence of confusion in
mind and intellect, in contrast with psychotic illness [4,8].

The current term psychopathy was not introduced until the end of the 19th century by the
German psychiatrist Julius Ludwig August Koch [4].

The following core publication came in 1941 from Hervey Cleckley, an American
neuropsychiatrist, with his work The Mask of Sanity in which he aimed to clarify the construct [1].
Cleckley, however, left the debate on whether psychopathy was characterized by a coalescence of
manifestations of dimensional attributes or categorized as a coherent syndrome. In his paper,

Cludio Vieira 2 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

Cleckley was on the view that psychopaths display a markedly positive impression of robust mental
health upon first sight; however also exhibit deficits in understanding and registering punishment,
which makes them incapable to adjust their maladaptive ways. To explain the dissonance between
the extreme lack of insight into their own emotional difficulties and their intellectual understanding
that their behaviour causes harm to others, Cleckley introduced the term semantic aphasia [1,4]
to describe a loss of recognition of the meaning of words and phrases. Thus, individuals with
psychopathy would articulately talk about of regret, guilt, and remorse, but they would not be in
touch with their emotional deeper meaning.

Coincident with the publication of DSM-III in


1980 which first included the Antisocial Personality
Disorder as a new diagnostic category, the Canadian
psychologist Robert D. Hare, based on Hervey
Cleckleys previous work, developed the 20-items
diagnostic tool to rate a persons psychopathic traits
The Psychopathy Checklist (PCL) [5,6,7,15], in which
a cut-off of 25 or greater is used to diagnosis
psychopathy in United Kingdom, whereas in United
States of America the threshold is 30 [15].
Early factor analysis of the PCL-R determined it
contained two main factors. Factor 1 includes traits related to interpersonal and affective deficits
of psychopathy whereas Factor 2 emphasizes symptoms emerged in antisocial behaviour (table 1).

Factor 1 has been distinctively associated to emotional deficits, instrumental and goal-
orientated aggression and has also been related with both adaptive and maladaptive behavioural
patterns such as emotional resilience to stress and threat, deliberate risk-taking, professional
success, and social dominance. This factor comprises a wide range of traits which are absent from
Cleckley's list (e.g. grandiose sense of self-worth), addressing a more callous and deceitful
personality [4,5,7,15].

On the other hand, Factor 2 comprises poor behavioural control, reactive/impulsive


aggression, antisocial personality disorder, and has been mainly attached to maladaptive
behavioural patterns (e.g., criminal aggression, poor academic achievement, impulsive risk-taking,
psychiatric care, suicide attempts, and financial problems) [10-13,15-19].

These psychopathic traits have been shown to be normally distributed both in the general
population and in offenders, and recidivism has been considerably related with higher scores on
the PCL-R [20]. Further, PCL-R scores indicate a close relation with higher rates of aggressive
behaviour even for individuals without psychopathy [21].

Cludio Vieira 3 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

Another important piece of research [6] has shown that individuals with psychopathic traits
share a wide range of cognitive and affective deficiencies linked to specific parts of the brain. Their
impairment in instrumental learning, response reversal and difficulties in executive functioning is
associated with deficits in their orbitofrontal cortex [22-26]. On the contrary, the impairment in
aversive conditioning [27] alongside dysfunctional response to anothers sad facial expressions
[28] and impaired processing expressions of fear [29] are strongly associated with deficits in the
functions of the amygdala [30].
The associations with particular neurobiological, cognitive and affective structures
highlighted above suggest the likelihood that certain neurobiological deficits may be essential
antecedents of this condition.

THE CALLOUS-UNEMOTIONAL TRAITS

Several research papers [34] have demonstrated that psychopathic traits are a robust predictor
of antisocial behaviours and are often used to describe a severe, chronic, and resistant-to-treatment
group of offenders A wide range of studies have also supported the view that antisocial behaviours
in adulthood have their roots in childhood [33,35]. Further, conduct problems associated to
difficulties in preserving social relationships during childhood are linked to an increase of violence
and aggression, a poorer clinical prognosis and also with a less response to treatment [36].
Gathering all these findings together, a bloom of studies on psychopathy in children and
adolescents started to rise [33].
The research on psychopathy in youth has been conducted under two main theoretical
approaches. The first approach, explored by Lynam [37], includes formulating a subtype of
behaviour disorder recognized on its comorbidity with hyperactivity and impulsivity. This
approach suggests that children who present both impulsive impulsive/hyperactive behaviour and
conduct disorder are more vulnerable to display more acute and persistent antisocial behaviour
when compared to those with either impulsivity or conduct disorder. Lynam [36] argued that
antisocial behaviours observed in boys with both impulsive/hyperactive and conduct disorder are
more severe and persistent than the antisocial behaviours displayed by boys with only impulsivity.
Furthermore, psychopathic traits (e.g. difficulties with response modulation) have been shown in
children with both conduct disorder and impulsive/hyperactive behaviours. However, this
correlation was not verified among children with either problem alone [39].

Other authors [39,86,87] have also correlated psychopathy with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) due to overlapping symptomology (e.g. substance misuse and
criminal activity and sensation-seeking) alongside parallels in the neurobiological function
underlying both ADHD and psychopathy. As result, both present high levels of impulsivity which

Cludio Vieira 4 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

often leads to interpersonal difficulties and antisocial behaviour in both childhood and adulthood.
Mathias and colleagues [85] also addressed a correlation between the impulsivity trait of
psychopathy with the hyperactive factor of ADHD and a relationship between the narcissistic and
callous traits with the inattentive element of ADHD.

While these findings appear promising, one limitation with this approach is its focus on an
impulsive-antisocial dimension of behaviour, which has not established to be exact to adults with
psychopath, i.e., a predisposition impulsive-antisocial seems to be high and robust in most adults
with significant criminal background and/or a diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disorder [15].
An additional approach amid the conceptualization of psychopathy in youth has been the
emphasis on the callous-unemotional (CU) traits which have been dominant to clinical formulation
of psychopathy in adulthood [1,15, 40,41, 45-47].

As observed within the affective dimension of adult psychopathy, CU traits incorporate lack
of guilt and empathy, and shallow affect. The prevalence of high CU traits in children and
adolescents with conduct disorder is estimated to range from 1046% in community samples to
2159% in clinic-samples [52] and display moderate to strong heritability (as greater as 50%)
among children and adolescents with behavioural problems [45,46,47,89].
Further, several studies [84] have demonstrated a robust stability of CU traits across youth.
Nevertheless, those do not suggest that CU traits are enduring as they seem to be somewhat flexible
and to be predisposed by environmental factors [58]. For instance, Frick and colleagues [88],
through a 4-year investigation, shown that a group of adolescents viewed their levels of CU traits
being reduced throughout the progress of the research, regardless their stability. Other authors [89]
have highlighted their findings for particular genetic stability in CU 13% of the genetic impact
in the CU traits at age 19 was shown with the equivalent effects at age 16 [89].

Frick, Bodin, & Barry [41] determined that in a sample of both clinic-referred and non-
referred children, CU traits develop a constant and distinct dimension from other psychopathic
traits such as impulsivity and narcissism. Whilst impulsivity does not seem to discriminate discrete
subgroups within children with acute and early-onset conduct disorder or adolescents with criminal
conducts [44,43,40], high levels of CU traits display a moderate role on the presence of antisocial
behaviours and conduct disorder on the risk of developing psychopathy [33]

Other authors, such as Loeber & Pardini [48], have suggested that causal factors (e.g. low
temperamental fear) could moderate the conduct disorder in children with callous-unemotional
traits. Additional research supported these findings by demonstrating a correlation between low
fearful arousal and development of severe antisocial behaviour and violence. Also, as highlighted
by Fowles & Kochanska [49] children with low fear disposition display deficits in empathy and
remorse due to experiencing reduced emotional arousal in response to distress or punishment. It

Cludio Vieira 5 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

appears that a fearless temperamental may contribute to the development of early antisocial
problems as it decreases the efficacy of punishment-oriented socialization techniques and
moderates the development of callous-unemotional traits [47].
In line with this, Hawes & Dadds [50] found that children with conduct problems alongside
callous-unemotional traits did not show any improvements from parenting when compared to the
control group. In their study, the callous-unemotional traits were the single predictor of less
engagement to treatment even after external variables such as parenting and family characteristic
and socioeconomic status were controlled.

Callous-unemotional traits can also be utilised as a robust predictor of instrumental antisocial


behaviours in children, discriminate them from children whose aggression and violence are mostly
impulsive and reactive [46,47]. Thus, and although a diagnosis of psychopathy in children would
lack sensibility and responsiveness, it has been well-documented that a specific subgroup of
children who shows conduct disorders alongside callous-unemotional traits are placed at enhanced
risk for fostering psychopathy in adulthood [33,47].

An additional prominent target of the current research on psychopathy has been the study of
how the brains of psychopaths process information in a different way from those without
psychopathy [52], highlighting and recognizing anatomical parts of the brain which operate in a
dysfunctional way [83].
The critical impairments of the brains of adults with psychopathy are observed in the
functionality of the ventrolateral/orbitofrontal cortex (e.g. represent a particular reward or
punishment) and the amygdala (e.g. social signs of emotions and fear conditioning) [53,54].
Research on psychopathy in children and adolescents assisted by functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) have emphasized that this age-group also display abnormalities in the
ventrolateral/orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala functioning and hippocampus, i.e., in areas of the
brain well-known for their role in emotional regulation and empathic processing [52,55,56].
Nevertheless, contrasting the findings in adult psychopathy where a decreased in the orbitofrontal
cortex grey matter is noted, children with CU traits and behavioural problems display an increased
grey matter concentration, observed particularly in the medial orbitofrontal cortex and in the
anterior cingulate cortex.

Additionally, children with callous-unemotional traits and behavioural problems, like adult
psychopaths, show difficulties in reversal learning and decision making mediated by the
orbitofrontal cortex [52,56]. Other critical findings in the neurobiological field came from Lozier
et al. [57] who aimed to examine whether neural impairments linked to callous-unemotional traits
play a moderate role in the relationship between callousness and proactive aggression in children
(7-10 years old) with behavioural problems.

Cludio Vieira 6 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

Consistent with previous literature, these findings suggested that proactive aggression in
children with callous-unemotional traits is related to response deficits in the amygdala to emotional
expressions. The amygdala is thus thought to constitute a phenotype that associates CU traits in
children with behavioural problems to a behavioural phenotype [57].

Additionally, genetics underlying various aspects of the serotonergic pathway have also been
accounted with regards to callous-unemotional traits. A wide range of evidences [90,91,92] suggest
that males with high levels of CU traits show deficits in levels of serotonin in the basolateral
amygdala which are associated to reductions in the conditioned fear response a response which
is consistently diminished in adults with high levels of psychopathic traits. Sadeh et al. [90] also
suggested that that 5-HTT genotype was highly correlated with the impulsivity dimension of
psychopathy [90].

In recent days a comprehensive discussion about the dimensions which best categorize the
construct of psychopathy in adulthood is still taking place. Three core dimensions have emerged
so far 1) the callous-unemotional traits labelled as deficient affective experience, 2) a deceiving
and narcissistic interpersonal style and 3) a pattern of impulsive and irresponsible behaviour
alongside proneness to boredom [58]. Nevertheless, in order to enhance the utility of psychopathy
as a construct it is also required to account significant areas of independence from broad measures
of antisocial behaviour.
Cooke & Michie [59] suggested that in samples of adults with psychopathy, the callous-
unemotional dimension is the most specific and determinant dimension used to distinguish
individuals with high levels of psychopathy to other individuals. Caputo et al. [43] found the same
results in children and adolescents, i.e., in their research aggressive sex offenders showed higher
callous-unemotional traits when compared to other offenders. However, other dimensions of
psychopathy did not show any statistically significant differences across the offender groups [43].

PSYCHOPATHY OR PSYCHOPATHIES

Although psychopathy is often classified as a distinct and specific construct measured by a


total PCL-R score, an ongoing debate which continues to be core of the research attention is the
extent to which individuals with psychopathy are a categorical class [31, 32,62] or share a
coalescence of extreme levels of distributed personality traits [66,63].

The controversy between dimensionality and taxonomy has a major impact on debate related
to psychopathy, specifically its etiology. For instance, a categorical approach might be explained
by a particular dependent factor (e.g., Downs syndrome which is caused by the presence of an

Cludio Vieira 7 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

extra chromosome 21); however, if psychopathy was proved to be dimensional, it would be


unlikely to result from a single underlying agent, such as an injury in the frontal lobe or from a
particular gene [62].
In psychopathology, taxon is well described by Meehl & Golden [62] as an entity, i.e., a
nonarbitary class of individuals that are authentic in nature and distinguished from the general
population.

Researchers like Harris, Skilling, and Rice [64] stressed that psychopaths are indeed a
discrete type of individuals who are fundamentally different from those who are classified as non-
psychopaths and who can be identified from a very early age. They proposed an alternative
psychopathy assessment tool Child and Adolescent Taxon Scale which supported psychopathy
as a taxon. Other studies have also sustained this approach in a community sample of children with
antisocial behaviours [65,60].

The evidences for psychopathy as a taxon expanded in 1994 with a study conducted by
Harris, Rice, and Quinsey [63], using a taxometric analysis of the PCL-R in a sample 653 male
offenders detained in Canadian mental health institutions. With their findings, Harris and
colleagues concluded that a taxon could be recognized using PCL-R scores, with a base rate in
their sample fluctuating from .44 to .46. Nevertheless, it was only found evidence of a taxon for
PCL-R Factor 2. That is, Factor 1 analyses were shown as inconsistent with a categorical structure
[62].

If psychopathy was shown to be dimensional, that would lead further researchers to focus
their studies on specific etiological and dimensional agents such as fearlessness. Further, it would
also support psychopathy research with community samples or with academic groups [62].
Although some researchers support the idea of psychopathy as a pattern of high scores on
distributed personality traits, such as low conscientiousness and low agreeableness [66], there are
not specific evidence for any dimensional model of psychopathy, as observed with the five-factor
model [67]. Other studies failed to support a categorical model of psychopathy. Marcus et al. [70]
led a research on a sample of prison inmates with the Psychopathic Personality Inventory and
consequent taxometric analysis. Consistent with previous studies no evidence supporting the
taxometric approach on psychopathy was found [60,70].

Hare [15] proposed a four-factor solution to the PCL-R in which the original Factor 1 and
Factor 2 are split into narrower facets. That is, Factor 1 would be composed of Interpersonal Facet
(e.g. manipulative) and Affective Facet (e.g. lack of remorse) whereas Factor 2 would include
Lifestyle facet (e.g. irresponsibility) and Antisocial Facet (e.g. poor behavioural controls). Hare
[15] suggested this model in order to enable taxometric analyses to be done at the scale level, which

Cludio Vieira 8 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

was not possible in previous studies [62]. Subsequently to Hares four-factor solution, several
debates started to emerge around its relevance and applicability [68,69]. Nevertheless, it has been
generally established that this new approach does account and emphasizes the multidimensional
nature of the construct of psychopathy and its emotional, behavioural, and interpersonal structures.
That is why in community and forensic services it is observed that most criminal offenders are not
diagnosed with psychopathy for this diagnosis is usually established for those who display
psychopathic features across these facets.

The multidimensional approach of the PCL-R proposes the likelihood of those who are
classified as psychopaths are a heterogeneous group of offenders. For instance, some psychopaths
dont reoffend during or after being in prison and engage fairly well in treatment [15,19,34].
Further, factor analyses indicates that the PCL-R Factor 1 highlights specific traits which are shown
in Cleckleys formulation of psychopathy (e.g. callousness), whereas Factor 2 stresses the criminal
and antisocial behaviour more consistent with the diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disorder
[79]. These findings demonstrate that there is a constellation of reasons to disaggregate
psychopathy into homogeneous subgroups and also challenge the hypothesis that antisocial
behaviour of those who are diagnosed with psychopathy is unchallengeable, whilst addressing the
opportunity of exploring specific variants that might lead to different behaviours.

Skeem et al. [19] proposed a configuration of traits which might help to understand the
variances in psychopathy within a dimensional approach. The trait anxiety is often used to distinct
primary and secondary variants of psychopathy. PCLR appears to classify a heterogeneous group
as psychopathic, instead of categorizing a specific group of psychopaths with low levels of anxiety
[4,19]. However, psychopaths can be distinguished regarding their levels of anxiety - low-anxious
psychopaths (primary psychopaths) display deficiencies in emotional responses [80], and fear
conditioning [19]. Other researchers [81] suggest that whilst primary psychopaths present more
noticeable traits of emotional detachment (PCLR Factor 1 or Facet 2), secondary psychopaths
show more traits associated to irresponsibility and aggression [19].
Another variant identified by Blackburn [19,82] is the interpersonal behaviour and the degree
of withdrawal. Blackburn [19,82] emphasized that primary psychopaths are self-confident and
arrogant, whereas the secondary psychopath is passive, temperamental with low self-confidence.
Also, Blackburn [82] classified secondary psychopaths as consistent with borderline personality
traits (PCL-R Factor 2), whereas the personality of primary psychopaths would be more consistent
with narcissistic personality features (PCL-R Factor 1) [19].
If variants of psychopathy can be shown to be reliable with empiric support, researchers and
clinicians may be able to progress their understanding of this construct in order to develop further

Cludio Vieira 9 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

management plans and specialist treatment services for a subgroup of individuals who have been
labelled as high-risky and hopeless [1].

FINAL CONSIDERATIONS

Psychopathy is still a broad concept which offers minor guides for clinical interventions in
forensic services, particularly for individuals who score 25 or above in the PCL-R. A wide range
of researchers have claimed that individuals with psychopathy fluctuate in their symptomology,
with distinctive patterns of aggression, and responsiveness to treatment. Thus, if those fluctuations
and variances were categorized by different risk factors for offending behaviour, those would have
critical implications for assessment, management, and treatment of psychopathy [79].
Several debates and controversy have aroused for the last decades regarding whether
psychopathy is a dimensional or categorical construct, i.e., whether individuals with psychopathy
differ from the rest of the population in degree or kind [74]. The PCL-R offers both a dimensional
and categorical approach to psychopathy by classifying an individual as psychopath-
nonpsychopath utilising either cut-off scores or continuous scores which can be determine where
an individual falls into the spectrum of psychopathic traits [15]. Also, psychopathic variants may
also be considered as distinct classes (primary psychopath and secondary psychopath) or as
dimensional constellations.
Further research should focus and direct their investigative efforts on whether variants in
psychopathy might fluctuate in their responsiveness to treatment as primary psychopaths are
conventionally perceived as unmanageable in therapeutic settings [15]. This may be due to low
levels of motivation and willingness to change their behaviours and challenge their schemas and
to low levels of insight into their psychopathology.

The difficulties in managing responsiveness to treatment in primary psychopathy seem to


have been overvalued [75] considering current findings that young offender psychopaths who are
exposed to long-term of treatment are less expected to engage in subsequent antisocial behaviours
than do those who are exposed to less or no treatment [76]. Given that, the treatment for primary
psychopaths should incorporate outlets for their risk-taking predispositions rather than targeting
enduring personality traits which may be an unrealistic and unachievable. On the other hand,
secondary psychopaths may engage in a wide range of interventions as they are able to experience
remorse and anxiety, thus may be motivated to seek treatment [77].

If practitioners emphasize simply a categorical approach suggesting that someone scoring 25


in the PCL-R is a psychopath and someone scoring 24 is not a psychopath, this could lead to
challenging consequences. For instance, whilst someone scoring 24 may be perceived as having

Cludio Vieira 10 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

good receptiveness to treatment and be at low risk, someone who scores 1 point higher would be
described as presenting poor treatment responsiveness and being at high risk for reoffending. Also,
clinicians are progressively adopting the psychopathy construct to address their decision-making
regarding treatment and assessment of individuals under this diagnosis. Hence, further
comprehensive research on the clinical implication of variants of psychopathy which are related to
significant repercussions in legal and clinical settings is desirable.

In conclusion several taxometric investigations suggest that psychopathy is a dimensional


construct when assessed by a wide range of instruments including self-report [70], PCL-R [60]
PCL-SV [71] and PCL-YV [72]. Those who argue that psychopathy is a dimensional approach
criticize the categorical view for building up limits where limits do not belong and for generating
specific classes that are overlapping, unclear, heterogeneous, and of dubious validity. In contrast,
those who argue for a categorical system stress that the categorical approach has shown to be a
rather operational shorthand form of communication among clinicians.
Investigative processes should also include cross-cultural studies of large community
samples using the PCL-R in order to extend our understanding of prevalence of psychopathic traits
in non-institutionalized samples. Taking into account cultural differences in incidence, exploring
how the picture of an institutionalized psychopath may differ from a non-institutionalized
psychopath (e.g. research on corporate psychopaths and how do they differ among themselves) and
also looking for additional variants on psychopathy might elucidate the heterogeneity within the
group within a robust framework. Also, most of the studies on psychopathy has been conducted in
males, thus it would be worth questioning variants of psychopathy in females as they may display
their traits differently from males (e.g. histrionic/borderline in women versus narcissistic/antisocial
in men).

Cludio Vieira 11 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

REFERNCIAS BIBLIOGRFICAS

1. Cleckley, H.M. (1976). The mask of sanity. St Louis, MO: Mosby.


2. Kiehl, K.A., & Hoffman, M.B. (2011). The criminal psychopath: History, neuroscience,
treatment, and economics. Jurimetrics Journal, 51, 355397.
3. Patrick, C.J., Fowles, D.C., & Krueger, R.F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of
psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness.
Development and Psychopathology, 21, 913938.
4. Yildirim, B., Derksen, J. (2015). Clarifying the heterogeneity in psychopathic samples:
Towards a new continuum of primary and secondary psychopathy. Aggression and
Violent Behaviour, 24, 9-4.
5. Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal
Justice and Behavior, 23, 2554.
6. Guay, J., Ruscio, J., Knight., R., Hare, R. (2007). A taxometric analysis of the latent
structure of psychopathy: evidence of dimensionality. Journal of abnormal psychology,
116(4). 701-716.
7. Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. N. (2006). The PCLR assessment of psychopathy:
Development, structural properties, and new directions. In C. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of
psychopathy (5888). New York: Guilford Press.
8. Fox, B., Jennings, W., Farrington, D. (2015). Bringing psychopathy into developmental
and life-course criminology theories and research. Journal of Criminal Justice. 43, 274-
289.
9. Millon, T., Simonsen, E., & Birket-Smith, M. (1998). Historical conceptions of
psychopathy in the United States and Europe. In T. Millon, & E. Simonsen (Eds.),
Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior (231). New York: Guildford
Press.
10. Babiak, P., Neumann, C.S., & Hare, R.D. (2010). Corporate psychopathy: Talking the
walk. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 28(2), 174193.
11. Blackburn, R., Logan, C., Donnelly, J.P., & Renwick, S.J. (2008). Identifying psychopathic
subtypes: Combining an empirical personality classification of offenders with the

Cludio Vieira 12 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

psychopathy checklist-revised. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22(6), 604622.


12. Blair, R.J.R. (2006a). The emergence of psychopathy; implications for the
neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 414442.
13. Blair, R.J.R. (2006b). Subcortical brain systems in psychopathy: The amygdala and
associated structures. In C.J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of Psychopathy (296312). New
York: Guilford Press.
14. Blair, R.J.R. (2010). Psychopathy, frustration, and reactive aggression: The role of
ventromedial prefrontal cortex. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 383399.
15. Hare, R.D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist Revised (2nd ed.). Toronto:
Multihealth Systems.
16. Kennealy, P.J., Skeem, J.L., Walters, G.D., & Camp, J. (2010). Do core interpersonal and
af- fective traits of PCL-R psychopathy interact with antisocial behavior and disinhibition
to predict violence? Psychological Assessment, 22(3), 569580.
17. Nioche, A., Pham, T.H., Ducro, C., de Beaurepaire, C., Chudzik, L., Courtois, R., et al.
(2010). Psychopathy and associated personality disorders: Searching for a particular effect
of the borderline personality disorder? Encephale, 36(3), 253259.
18. Reidy, D.E., Shelley-Tremblay, J.F., & Lilienfeld, S.O. (2011). Psychopathy, reactive
aggression, and precarious proclamations: A review of behavioral, cognitive, and biological
research. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(6), 512524.
19. Skeem, J.L., Johansson, P., Andershed, H., Kerr, M., & Louden, J.E. (2007). Two subtypes
of psychopathic violent offenders that parallel primary and secondary variants. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 116, 395409.
20. Dahle, K. (2006). Strengths and limitations of actuarial prediction of criminal re-offense in
a German prison sample: a comparative study of LSI-R, HCR-20 and PCL-R. International
Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 29, 431-442
21. Hare, R., Clark, D., Grann, M., Thornton, D. (2000). Psychopathy and the predictive
validity of the PCL-R: an international perspective. Behavioral Sciences and the Law,
18(5), 557-690.
22. Herve , H. F., Hayes, P. J., & Hare, R. D. (2003). Psychopathy and sensitivity to the
emotional polarity of metaphorical statements. Personality and Individual Differences, 35,
14971507.
23. Morgan, A. P., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between

Cludio Vieira 13 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical


Psychology Review, 20, 113156.
24. Blair, R. J. R., Mitchell, D. G. V., Richell, R. A., Kelly, S., Leonard, A., Newman, C., &
Scott, S. K. (2002). Turning a deaf ear to fear: Impaired recognition of vocal affect in
psychopathic individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 682686.
25. Patrick, C. J. (2001). Emotional processes in psychopathy. In A. Raine & J. Sanmartin
(Eds.), Violence and psychopathy (5777). New York: Kluwer Academic.
26. Williamson, S. E., Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing of affective
words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 28, 260273.
27. Newman, J. P., & Kosson, D. S. (1986). Passive avoidance learning in psychopathic and
nonpsychopathic offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 252256.
28. Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the
psychopath. Cognition, 57, 129.
29. Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neuro-cognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality
disorders and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neuro- surgery and Psychiatry, 71, 727
731.
30. Blair, R. J. R. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial
behavior. Brain and Cognition, 55, 198208.
31. Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Salekin, R. T., & Leistico, A. R. (2005). Convergent and
discriminant validity of psychopathy factors assessed via self-report: A comparison of three
instruments. Assessment, 12, 270 289.
32. Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001). Personality disorders
as extreme variants of common personality dimensions. Can the five-factor model of
personality adequately represent psychopathy? Journal of Personality, 69, 253276.
33. Essau, C., Sasagawa, S., Frick, P. (2006). Callous-Unemotional Traits in a Community
Sample of Adolescents. Assessment, 20(10), 1-16

34. Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and recidivism: A review.
Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 139-170.

35. Loeber, R. (1982). The stability of antisocial and delinquent child behavior: A review.
Child Development, 53, 1431-1446.

Cludio Vieira 14 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

36. Rogeness, G. A., Javors, M. A., & Pliszka, S. R. (1992). Neurochemistry and child and
adolescent psychiatry. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 31, 765-781.

37. Lynam, D. R. (1996). The early identification of chronic offenders: Who is the fledgling
psychopath? Psychological Bulletin, 120, 209-234.

38. Lynam, D. R. (1997). Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the fledgling psychopath in a
normological net. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 06, 425-438.

39. Lynam, D. R. (1998). Early identification of the fledgling psychopath: Locating the
psychopathic child in the current nomenclature. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107,
566-575.

40. Vincent, G. M., Vitacco, M. J., Grisso, T., & Corrado, R. R. (2003). Subtypes of adolescent
offenders: Affective traits and antisocial behavior patterns. Behavioral Science and the
Law, 21, 695-712.

41. Frick, P. J., Bodin, S. D., & Barry, C. T. (2000). Psychopathic traits and conduct problems
in community and clinic-referred samples of children: Further development of the
Psychopathy Screening Device. Psychological Assessment, 12, 382-393.

42. Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004) Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct
problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 54-68.

43. Caputo, A. A., Frick, P. J., & Brodsky, S. L. (1999). Family violence and juvenile sex
offending: Potential mediating roles of psychopathic traits and negative attitudes toward
women. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 338-356

44. Christian, R., Frick, P. J., Hill, N., Tyler, L. A., & Frazer, D. (1997). Psychopathy and
conduct problems in children: II. Subtyping children with conduct problems based on their
interpersonal and affective style. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 36, 233-241.

45. E., Viding, NM, Fountaine., EJ, McCrory (2012). Antisocial behaviour in children with and
without callous-unemotional traits. Journal of the Royal Society Medicine, 105(5): 195-200

46. Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Youngstrom E, Findling RL, Youngstrom JK. (2012). The effects of
including a callous-unemotional specifier for the diagnosis of conduct disorder. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 271282

Cludio Vieira 15 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

47. Pardini, D., & Frick, P. J. (2013). Multiple Developmental Pathways to Conduct Disorder:
Current Conceptualizations and Clinical Implications. Journal of the Canadian Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 22(1), 2025.

48. Loeber R, Pardini D. (2008). Neurobiology and the development of violence: Common
assumptions and controversies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Biological
Sciences, 363, 24912503.

49. Fowles DC, Kochanska G. (2000). Temperament as a moderator of pathways to conscience


in children: The contribution of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology, 37,788795.

50. Hawes D.M, Dadds M.R. (2002). Callousunemotional traits are a risk factor for poor
treatment response in young conduct problem children. Journal Consult Clinical
Psychology, 73, 737741

51. Dadds, M. R., & Rhodes, T. (2008). Aggression in young children with concurrent callous
unemotional traits: can the neurosciences inform progress and innovation in treatment
approaches? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
363(1503), 25672576.

52. De Brito SA et al (2009) Size matters: increased grey matter in boys with conduct problems
and callous-unemotional traits. Brain, 132, 843-52.

53. Blair, R., Mitchell, D., Blair, K. (2005). The Psychopath: emotion and the Brain. Oxford:
Blackwell.

54. Decety J, Shelly, L., Kiehl, K. (2013) Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios
involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA Psychiatry 70,638-45.
55. Singer T, Seymour B, ODoherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. (2004). Empathy for
pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157-62.
56. Blair, J. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. The British Journal of Psychiatry,
182(1), 5-7,

57. Lozier, L., Cardinale, E., Vanmeter, J., Marsh, A. (2014). Mediation of the relationship
between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear
among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry,71, 627-636.

Cludio Vieira 16 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

58. Frick, P., White, S. (2008). Research Review: The importance of callous- unemotional traits
for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behaviour. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 49 (4), 359375

59. Cooke, D.J, & Michie, C. (1997). An item response theory analysis of the Hare
Psychopathy Checklist. Psychological Assessment, 9, 314.

60. Edens, J. F., Marcus, D. K., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Poythress, N. G. (2006). Psychopathic,
notpsychopath: Taxometric evidence for the dimensional structure of psychopathy. Journal
ofAbnormal Psychology, 115, 131-144.

61. Cunningham, M. D., & Reidy, T. J. (2002). Violence risk assessment at federal capital
sentencing: Individualization, generalization, relevance, and scientific standards. Criminal
Justice and Behavior, 29, 512537.
62. Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Quinsey, V. L. (1994). Psychopathy as a taxon: Evidence that
psychopaths are a discrete class. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 387
397.
63. Lilienfeld, S. O., & Fowler, K. A. (2006). The self-report assessment of psychopathy:
Pitfalls, problems, and promises. In C. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (107132).
New York: Guilford Press.
64. Harris, G. T., Skilling, T., & Rice, M. E. (2001). The construct of psychopathy. Crime and
Justice, 28, 197264.
65. Skilling, T. A., Quinsey, V. L., & Craig, W. M. (2001). Evidence of a taxon underlying
serious antisocial behavior in boys. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 28, 450470.
66. Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2005).
Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: A step toward community
epidemiological investigations. Assessment, 12, 318.
67. Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Using the five-factor model to represent the DSM
IV personality disorders: An expert consensus approach. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
110, 410412
68. Cooke, D. J., Michie, C., Hart, S. D., & Clark, D. (2005). Searching for the pan-cultural
core of psychopathic personality disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 283
295
69. Hill, C., Neumann, C., & Rogers, R. (2004). Confirmatory factor analysis of the
Psychopathy Checklist: Screening version in offenders with Axis I disorders. Psychological

Cludio Vieira 17 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

Assessment, 16, 9095.


70. Marcus, D. K., John, S. L., & Edens, J. F. (2004). A taxometric analysis of psychopathic
personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(4), 626-635.

71. Hart, S., Cox, D., & Hare, R. D. (1995). Manual for the Psychopathy Checklist:
ScreeningVersion (PCL:SV). Toronto: Multihealth Systems.Jackson, R.

72. Murrie, D. C., Marcus, D. K., Douglas, K. S., Lee, Z., Salekin, R. T., & Vincent, G.
(2007).Youth with psychopathy features are not a discrete class: a taxometric analysis.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 714-723.

73. Hart, S. D., & Dempster, R. J. (1997). Impulsivity and psychopathy. In C. D. Webster, &
M. A. Jackson (Eds.), Impulsivity: theory, assessment and treatment (212232). New York:
Guilford.

74. Baldwin, J. (1995). Continua outperform dichotomies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18,
543544

75. Wong, S. (2000). Psychopathic offenders. In S. Hodgins, & R. Muller-Isberner (Eds.),


Violence, crime and mentally disordered offenders: concepts and methods for effective
treatment and prevention (87112). New York: Willey

76. Skeem, J., & Mulvey, E. (2001). Psychopathy and community violence among civil
psychiatric patients: results from the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 358374.

77. Woody, G., McLellan, A., Luborsky, L., & OBrien, C. (1985). Sociopathy and
psychotherapy outcome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 10811086

78. Blackburn, R. (1988). On moral judgments and personality disorders: the myth of
psychopathic personality revisited. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 505512.

79. Skeem, J., Poythress, N., Edens, J., Lilienfeld, S., Caled, E. (2003). Psychopathic
personality or personalities?Exploring potential variants of psychopathy and their
implications for risk assessment. Aggression and Violent Behaviour, 8(5), 513-546.

80. Newman, J. P., & Schmitt, W. (1998). Passive avoidance in psychopathicoffenders: A


replication and extension. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 527532.

Cludio Vieira 18 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

81. Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic
attributes in a noninstitutional population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
68,151158.
82. Blackburn, R. (1975). An empirical classification of psychopathic personality. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 127,456460.
83. Kiehl, K., Sinnott-ARmstrong, W. (2013). Handbook on Psychopathy and Law. Oxford
Series in Neuroscience, Law, and Philosophy.
84. Munoz, L.C., & Frick, P.J. (2007). The reliability, stability, and predictive utility of the
self-report version of the Antisocial Process Screening Device. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 48, 299312.
85. Mathias, C. W., Furr, R. M., Daniel, S. S., Marsh, D. M., Shannon, E. E., & Dougherty, D.
M. (2007). The relationship of inattentiveness, hyperactivity, and psychopathy among
adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(6), 13331343.
86. Eisenbarth, H., Alpers, G.W., Conzelmann, A., Jacob, C.P., Weyers, P. (2008).
Psychopathic traits in adult ADHD patients. Aggressive and Violent Behavior, 45,(6), 468-
472
87. Smith, C., Hung, L. (2012). The relative influence of conduct problems and attention-
deficit hyperactivity disorder in the development of adolescent psychopathy. Aggression
and Violent Behavior, 17(6), 575-580.
88. Frick, P.J., Kimonis, E.R., Dandreaux, D.M., & Farrell, J.M. (2003c). The 4-year stability
of psychopathic traits in non-referred youth. Behavioral Sciences and the Law,21 713736
89. Viding, E., McCrory, E. (2012). Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the
development of psychopathy. Developmental Psychopathology, 24(3), 969-983.
90. Sadeh, N., Javdani, S., Jackson, J. J., Reynolds, E. K., Potenza, M. N., Gelernter, J.,
Verona, E. (2010). Serotonin Transporter Gene Associations with Psychopathic Traits in
Youth Vary as a Function of Socioeconomic Resources. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
119(3), 604609.
91. Newman JP, Curtin JJ, Bertsch JD, Baskin-Sommers AR (2010) Attention Moderates the
Fearlessness of Psychopathic Offenders. Biological Psychiatry 67: 6670.
92. Moul,C., Dobson-Stone, C., Brennan, J., Hawes, D., Dadds, M. (2013). An Exploration of
the serotonin system in antisocial boys with high levels of callous-unemotional traits. Plos
One, 8, 2.

Cludio Vieira 19 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt
www.psicologia.pt
ISSN 1646-6977
Documento produzido em 13.03.2016

Cludio Vieira 20 Siga-nos em


facebook.com/psicologia.pt

You might also like