You are on page 1of 4

ARTICLE

Gally, 2001; Tononi et al., 1999). We will


also suggest that sleep plays a novel role
in emphasising differences between the
various neural representations of time,
and perhaps even in creating degeneracy.
Time and the sleeping brain Degeneracy of neural systems
Penelope A. Lewis and Warren H. Meck argue that the importance of timing leads for timing
the brain to protect against damage to the system while we sleep Confronted with the vast diversity of
brain areas recruited by timing tasks,
Lewis and Miall (2003a) proposed that
Our ability to measure time hy are so many different parts time can be measured by at least two
persists in the face of a wide variety
of neural insults. In combination
with the large array of neural
W of the brain involved in time
perception? This simple function
appears to draw on a vast array of
distinct systems. One, the automatic
system involves structures associated with
motor control (e.g. cerebellum, striatum
structures that have been shown different neural substrates, and these vary and supplementary motor cortex). The
to activate during timing tasks, this greatly depending on the precise timing other, the cognitively controlled system,
resilience suggests that multiple task at hand. This is a major obstacle to draws flexibly upon the pluripotent
brain networks are capable of the study of temporal processing: research prefrontal and parietal cortices. These
measuring time. This article is continually constrained by the systems are not mutually exclusive but
explores this apparent degeneracy observation that duration range, signal form a continuum, with some timing
(a concept explained in the article) modality, response type, hemispheric tasks recruiting elements from both,
in neural systems for timing in the lateralisation, level of awareness, and type while other timing tasks draw much
context of recent evidence that such of timing task all impact upon the neural more clearly on one or the other.
degeneracy may be promoted and structures involved in time measurement To investigate the importance of the
enhanced by consolidation across in complex ways (e.g. Harrington et al., job at hand for determining which parts
a night of sleep. We also raise the 2011; Jahanshahi et al., 2010; Jones et al., of the brain are used, Lewis and Miall
possibility that offline consolidation 2008). analysed three specific task characteristics.
may promote degeneracy in other An early meta-analysis of the brain These were the duration of the interval to
spheres (not just in timing). areas that activate when participants are be timed, whether or not timing involved
asked to time specific stimuli or responses movement by the participant, and the
implicated 33 different structures across continuousness of the timing (e.g. whether
Could degeneracy allow timing to be the 35 studies examined (Lewis & Miall, the start time of each sequential interval
questions

spared following brain damage to either 2003b, 2003c). The timing tasks in was predictable as in the case of rhythmic
cognitively controlled or automatic question encompassed everything from events). Interestingly, this analysis revealed
temporal processing? paced finger tapping to passive viewing of that sub-second intervals, automatic tasks,
How does sleep-dependent memory a visually presented interval. Early models and motor involvement all biased neural
consolidation contribute to degeneracy suggested that the same neural timing recruitment towards the automatic system,
in timing systems? mechanism might be involved in all timing while the opposing characteristics
tasks, but this type of evidence argues durations of more than a second, non-
strongly against the concept of a single motor timing and a discrete, non-
neural clock. Instead, the diversity of brain rhythmical response biased recruitment
resources

Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. (2010). The


memory function of sleep. Nature regions recruited for timing suggests that towards the cognitively-controlled system.
Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 114126. time can be measured by multiple Most tasks, however, contained a
Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time degenerate systems. combination of these characteristics and
perception. Attention, Perception & In this article, we will explain the thus recruited elements of each system
Psychophysics, 72, 561582. concept of degeneracy and point out a (Lewis & Miall, 2003a, 2003b, 2003c).
Price, C.J. & Friston, K.J. (2002). parallel between the apparent degeneracy Although no subsequent study has
Degeneracy and cognitive anatomy. of neural timing systems and the well- examined the impact of all three of these
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6,
416421. characterised degeneracy in many other task factors within the same dataset, a
aspects of biological function (Edelman & recent meta-analysis did categorise the
references

Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Balteau, E. et time distinguishes between synchrony and hippocampal function. neurochemical substrates of
al. (2008). Both the hippocampus and perception and action. Perception, 39, Brain Research, 1237, 176194. timing. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36,
striatum are involved in consolidation 8190. Cheng, R.K., Williams, C.L. & Meck, W.H. 325.
of motor sequence memory. Neuron, Buhusi, C.V. & Meck, W.H. (2005). What (2009). Neurophysiological Coslett, H.B., Shenton, J., Dyer, T. &
58, 261272. makes us tick? Nature Reviews mechanisms of sleep-dependent Wiener, M. (2009). Cognitive timing.
Aparicio, P., Diedrichsen, J. & Ivry, R.B. Neuroscience, 6, 755765. memory consolidation and its Brain Research, 1254, 3848.
(2005). Effects of focal basal Buonomano, D.V. & Laje, R. (2010). facilitation by prenatal choline Coslett, H.B., Wiener, M. & Chatterjee,
ganglia lesions on timing and force Population clocks. Trends in Cognitive supplementation. Chinese Journal of A. (2010). Dissociable neural
control. Brain and Cognition, 58, Sciences, 14, 520527. Physiology, 52, 223235. systems for timing. PLoS ONE 5(4),
6274. Cheng, R.K., Williams, C.L. & Meck, W.H. Coull, J.T., Cheng, R.K. & Meck, W.H. e10324.
Bueti, D. & Walsh, V. (2010). Memory for (2008). Oscillatory bands, neuronal (2011). Neuroanatomical and doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010324

594 vol 25 no 8 august 2012


time

www.jamesgrover.com
different patterns of brain activity can
be elicited by time measurement, and
that this is partially dependent upon
the nature of the task as well as the
participants neurological status is
now well established. As we will argue
below, such seemingly complex results
can potentially be explained by the
relatively simple concept of degeneracy
in neural timing systems.
In the simplest sense, degeneracy
can be thought of as a strategy by
which an organism protects itself
against loss of vital abilities via
distribution of function, across
structurally different mechanisms.
When it comes to purely neural
systems, however, degeneracy can be
more complex and subtle. It is unlikely
that any two separate neural systems
will perform a given function in
exactly the same way. Instead, the
brain frequently provides several
alternative routes to any given goal,
with each of these drawing upon quite
separate machinery.
Evidence for this comes from
the remarkable resilience shown by
patients with brain damage, who
frequently perform well on tasks such
as semantic judgement even when the
brain regions that are most strongly
associated with these functions have
been completely removed. Other
evidence comes from neuroimaging
studies of healthy participants,
showing that different people
consistently use completely different
neural systems to perform the same
task (e.g. Noppeney et al., 2006). The
time-perception data discussed above
are a good example of this latter
multiplicity of function (Coslett et al.,
2009). Furthermore, these types of task-
neuroimaging literature based on both Other work has further supported the related differences in time perception are
interval duration and motor demands recruitment of distinct timing mechanisms consistent with the selective recruitment
(Wiener et al., 2010). This again showed for sub- and supra-second intervals (e.g. and context dependency of degenerate
that different task characteristics are Gooch et. al., 2010, Sysoeva et al., 2010; systems (e.g. Lewis & Miall, 2003b;
associated with distinct patterns of neural Wiener et. al. 2011), as well as for motor Whitacre & Bender, 2010).
activity although the brain regions and non-motor tasks (e.g. Coslett et. al. Those who study degeneracy in
implicated were not identical to those 2010, Bueti & Walsh, 2010). biological systems argue that it is highly
found by Lewis and Miall (2003a, 2003b). The overall finding that many adaptive because it facilitates learning via

Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural 98, 1376313768 48, 10221031. connectivity during motor timing in
recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, Gais, S., Rasch, B., Wagner, U. & Born, J. Harrington, D.L., Castillo, G.N., Fong, Parkinsons disease. Brain, 133,
56, 384398. (2008). Visualprocedural memory C.H. & Reed, J.D. (2011). Neural 727745.
Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. (2010). consolidation during sleep blocked underpinnings of distortions in Jones, C.R.G., Malone, T.J.L., Dirnberger,
The memory function of sleep. by glutamatergic receptor the experience of time across G. et al. (2008). Basal ganglia,
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, antagonists. Journal of Neuroscience, senses. Frontiers in Integrative dopamine and temporal processing.
114126. 28, 55135518. Neuroscience 5, 32. doi: Brain and Cognition, 68, 3041.
Edelman, G.M. & Gally, J.A. (2001). Gooch, C.M., Wiener, M., Wencil, E.B. & 10.3389/fnint.2011.00032 Lewis, P.A., Couch, T.J. & Walker, M.P.
Degeneracy and complexity in Coslett, H.B. (2010). Interval timing Jahanshahi, M., Jones, C.R.G., Zijlmans, (2011). Keeping time in your sleep.
biological systems. Proceedings of the disruptions in subjects with J. et al. (2010). Dopaminergic Neuropsychologia, 49, 115123.
National Academy of Sciences, USA, cerebellar lesions. Neuropsychologia, modulation of striato-frontal Lewis, P.A. & Miall, R.C. (2003a). Brain

read discuss contribute at www.thepsychologist.org.uk 595


time

prevention of interference among new such findings in an interesting way. This predispositions could explain the marked
memories and pre-existing representations. study examined both passive monitoring cross-cultural consistency observed in
Degeneracy also allows flexibility at an of an auditory rhythm for sounds that were the brain regions used for tasks such as
evolutionary level. The relevance of this misplaced in time (a task that draws more language processing and arithmetic.
line of reasoning to time perception should strongly on the cognitively controlled Dehaene and Cohen (2007) suggest that
be obvious. The fundamental importance system for timing), and active tapping of these regions are not only predisposed to
of timing to all perception and action that rhythm with a forefinger (a task that perform such functions, but have also
demands degeneracy, while the ease with draws more strongly on the automatic evolved to facilitate these kinds of tasks,
which it can be performed by a range of system for timing). This showed that becoming more and more specialised as
neural architectures facilitates such a consolidation of memory for the rhythm specific, related, demands are
design (Buhusi & Meck, 2005; across sleep led to both enhanced incrementally placed upon them.
Buonomano & Laje, 2010). It is very activation of the hippocampus (associated Combining this idea with the
difficult to completely abolish the ability to with cognitively controlled timing) during neuroplasticity observed in brain
measure time, especially as a result of focal subsequent monitoring, and enhanced structures recruited for rhythmic timing
brain lesions, where redundancy from the activation in parts of the motor system after sleep (Lewis et. al., 2011), it is
opposite hemisphere is likely to contribute (associated with automatic timing) during tempting to speculate that sleep may
to recovery (e.g. Aparicio et al., 2005; subsequent rhythmic tapping. Importantly, facilitate the evolution of temporal
Coslett et al., 2009 but see Coslett et al., the same participants performed both memories along similar lines. That is,
2010 and Meck, 2006 for examples of tasks, so these distinct activation patterns sleep may facilitate a gradual shift in the
bilateral lesions of the basal ganglia that do not reflect a difference in consolidation, neural representation of temporal
can lead to timing deficits). but instead a difference in the way brain memories away from flexible brain
areas were recruited after
consolidation, depending upon
Sleep and degeneracy the task that was performed. This
a potential synergism not only supports the idea that the
It is now well established that sleep temporal representation that had
plays a role in memory consolidation been learned and consolidated
(Diekelmann & Born, 2010; Walker, was accessed differently
2009). Subconscious processing that depending upon the nature of the
occurs while we slumber is believed to task at hand, but also suggests
allow the re-coding of new memories in that sleep led to a greater
different brain structures, as well as the dissociation in the brain areas
integration of these memories into recruited (e.g. that motor timing
existing knowledge. Both the mechanisms was more automatic, and non-
by which such consolidation occurs, and motor timing was more
how it differs for different types of cognitively controlled after
memory, remain controversial. Several consolidation during sleep).
theories have proposed that sleep
facilitates the transfer of episodic
information from hippocampal A tempting speculation
representations to the neocortex (Cheng The brain can be thought of as
et al., 2008, 2009; Takashima et al., a compartmentalised structure,
2009). Sleep has also been shown to with different types of hardware
facilitate the transfer of procedural available in different areas. Some
representations from hippocampus into types of hardware are more suited
structures that are more associated with to some types of processing than
motor control (Albouy et al., 2008; Coull others. For instance the
et al., 2011). cerebellum is well suited to the
Recently, an examination of how fine-tuning of motor control, and
overnight sleep impacts on the pattern the primary visual cortex is well
of brain activity associated with rhythmic suited to the detection of line
timing (Lewis et al., 2011) has built on orientations. Such structural

activation patterns during timing. In W.H. Meck (Ed.), Functional Neurobiology, 18, 145152. Takashima, A., Nieuwenhuis, I.L.C.,
measurement of sub- and supra- and neural mechanisms of interval Noppeney, U., Penny, W.D., Price, C.J. et Jensen, O. et al. (2009). Shift from
second intervals. Neuropsychologia, timing. (pp.515532). Boca Raton, FL: al. (2006). Identification of hippocampal to neocortical centered
41, 15831592. CRC Press. degenerate neuronal systems based retrieval network with consolidation.
Lewis, P.A. & Miall, R.C. (2003b). Distinct Meck, W.H. (2006). Neuroanatomical on intersubject variability. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
systems for automatic and localization of an internal clock. NeuroImage, 30, 885890. 1008710093.
cognitively controlled time Brain Research, 1109, 93107. Sysoeva, O.V., Tonevitsky, A.G. & Tononi, G., Sporns, O. & Edelman, G.M.
measurement. Current Opinion in Meck, W.H., Penney, T.B. & Pouthas, V. Wackermann, J. (2010). Genetic (1999). Measures of degeneracy and
Neurobiology, 13, 250255. (2008). Cortico-striatal determinants of time perception redundancy in biological networks.
Lewis, P.A. & Miall, R.C. (2003c). representation of time in animals mediated by the serotonergic system. Proceedings of the National
Overview: An image of human neural and humans. Current Opinion in PLoS ONE, 5(9), e12650. Academy of Sciences, USA, 96,

596 vol 25 no 8 august 2012


time

structures that can perform many types this property of the network. If a separate of degeneracy at the evolutionary level.
of timing and deeper and deeper into representation exists in a different network Recent work on how sleep influences
structures that are more specialised for specialising in horizontal lines, then the degenerate representations of time
a particular type of processing (e.g. motor a holistic memory for the suggests that offline
timing). picture might draw upon consolidation may
If we follow this idea up, the selective both networks, providing emphasise the
increases in the involvement of the motor a more complete visual it is tempting to differences between
system and hippocampus observed by representation. If one of speculate that sleep may timing systems, and
Lewis et al. (2011) after sleep could the networks is damaged, facilitate the evolution of that there is a parallel
suggest that the copy of the temporal the partial existing temporal memories between this
memory stored within each structure is memory might still be magnification effect and
gradually re-coded in the way that is most sufficient to allow success the impact of sleep in other
compatible with the specific neural on a range of memory tests, but deficits domains such as visual procedural
hardware in question (Yin & Troger, would be apparent if the tests specifically memory consolidation (Gais et al., 2008).
2011). To take a simplistic example, if targeted the damaged line orientation We draw on this information to suggest
a neural network is good at representing (horizontal or vertical). that, in the process of more firmly
vertical, but not horizontal lines, then a With regard to the impact of sleep ingraining new memories into the brains
consolidating pictorial representation may upon temporal memory mechanisms, neural structures, consolidation may also
gradually lose its horizontal component we speculate that the automatic and subtly tailor them to fit the circuitry in
while retaining all vertical components as cognitively controlled systems could fill which they are coded. Distinct
it becomes increasingly dependent upon similar roles to the horizontal and vertical representations of the same memory may
networks outlined above (Lewis et al., therefore gradually evolve along quite
2011). The observation of increased separate trajectories such that the brain
activation in the striatum, cerebellum must draw on different representations
and supplementary motor area (all in order to respond to specific task
associated with the automatic system) demands. In some cases more than one
during motor timing, and in the complementary representation may be
hippocampus (more associated with needed. This proposal is especially
the cognitive system) after sleep would relevant for temporal memories, as these
then be in line with the idea that the can be used in many different ways (e.g.
mnemonic representations in both for movement or for cognitive decisions),
neural systems may have been altered but the possibility that this general
as they gradually settled into the principle applies to all consolidation in
hardware available, producing two the brain should also be considered.
separate specialised representations Inevitably, unresolved issues remain.
instead of the original more flexible Under what conditions does degeneracy
single representation. In other words, occur in timing? How is it influenced by
we are suggesting that temporal selective lesions? How does one evaluate
memories may become more and more the degree of degeneracy, and are there
specialised to fit the neural structures individual differences? Is there a specific
by which they are coded when they order in which alternative timing systems
consolidate across sleep. are engaged, or is this dependent upon the
circumstances? How does memory
consolidation, especially across sleep,
Conclusions impact upon degeneracy in timing?
In this article we have considered the This is sure to remain a fascinating area
multiplicity of brain regions for timing of research for many years to come.
and time perception in the context of
established ideas about neural
degeneracy. We have discussed
evidence for the degeneracy of neural Penelope A. Lewis
timing systems, as well as the benefits is at the School of
Psychological Sciences,
University of Manchester
plewis@manchester.ac.uk
32573262. humans. Journal of Cognitive
Walker, M.P. (2009). The role of sleep in Neuroscience, 23, 28112821.
cognition and emotion. Annals of the Wiener, M., Turkeltaub, P.E. & Coslett,
New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, H.B. (2010). The image of time.
168197. NeuroImage, 49, 17281740. Warren H. Meck
Whitacre, J. & Bender, A. (2010). Yin, B. & Troger, A.B. (2011). Exploring the is at the Department of
Degeneracy. Journal of Theoretical 4th dimension. Frontiers in Integrative Psychology and
Biology, 263, 143153. Neuroscience, 5, 36. doi: Neuroscience, Duke
Wiener, M., Lohoff, F.W. & Coslett, H.B. 10.3389/fnint.2011.00036
University, Durham, North
(2011). Double dissociation of
dopamine genes and timing in
Carolina

read discuss contribute at www.thepsychologist.org.uk 597

You might also like