You are on page 1of 23

Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.

org

Statistical limitations in functional neuroimaging. I.


Non-inferential methods and statistical models
Karl Magnus Petersson, Thomas E. Nichols, Jean-Baptiste Poline and Andrew P. Holmes
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 1999 354, 1239-1260
doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0477

References Article cited in:


http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/354/1387/1239#related-urls

Email alerting service Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top
right-hand corner of the article or click here

To subscribe to Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B go to: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptions

This journal is 1999 The Royal Society


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Statistical limitations in functional neuroimaging


I. Non-inferential methods and statistical models

Karl Magnus Petersson1*, Thomas E. Nichols2,3, Jean-Baptiste Poline4,5


and Andrew P. Holmes5,6
1
Cognitive Neurophysiology R2- 01, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Karolinska Hospital, S-171 76 Stockholm,
Sweden (karlmp@neuro.ks.se)
2
Department of Statistics, Carnegie Mellon University, 5000 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA (nicholst@stat.cmu.edu)
3
Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University and University of Pittsburgh, 4400 Fifth Avenue, Pittsburgh,
PA 15213, USA
4
Commissariat l'Energie Atomique, Direction de la Recherche Medicale, Service Hospitalier FredericJoliot, 4 Pl General Leclerc,
91406 Orsay, France (poline@shfj.cea.fr)
5
Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Functional Imaging Laboratory, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
(andrew@l.ion.ucl.ac.uk)
6
Robertson Centre for Biostatistics, Department of Statistics, University of Glasgow, Boyd Orr Building, University Avenue,
Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK (andrew@stats.gla.ac.uk)

CONTENTS PAGE

1. Introduction 1240
2. Non-inferential methods for signal characterization 1241
(a) Principal components analysis 1241
(b) Independent components analysis 1242
(c) The scale subprole model approach 1243
(d) Inspection of functional neuroimaging data 1243
3. Statistical models for functional neuroimaging 1244
(a) Baseline uctuations and global normalization 1244
(b) Univariate statistical models 1246
(c) Bayesian models for functional neuroimaging data 1250
(d) Multivariate statistical models 1251
(e) Functional connectivity and network analysis 1254
4. Conclusion 1255
References 1256
Functional neuroimaging (FNI) provides experimental access to the intact living brain making it possible
to study higher cognitive functions in humans. In this review and in a companion paper in this issue, we
discuss some common methods used to analyse FNI data. The emphasis in both papers is on assumptions
and limitations of the methods reviewed. There are several methods available to analyse FNI data indi-
cating that none is optimal for all purposes. In order to make optimal use of the methods available it is
important to know the limits of applicability. For the interpretation of FNI results it is also important to
take into account the assumptions, approximations and inherent limitations of the methods used. This
paper gives a brief overview over some non-inferential descriptive methods and common statistical
models used in FNI. Issues relating to the complex problem of model selection are discussed. In general,
proper model selection is a necessary prerequisite for the validity of the subsequent statistical inference.
The non-inferential section describes methods that, combined with inspection of parameter estimates and
other simple measures, can aid in the process of model selection and verication of assumptions. The
section on statistical models covers approaches to global normalization and some aspects of univariate,
multivariate, and Bayesian models. Finally, approaches to functional connectivity and eective connec-
tivity are discussed. In the companion paper we review issues related to signal detection and statistical
inference.
Keywords: functional neuroimaging methods; PET; fMRI; statistical models; non-inferential methods;
model selection
*
Author for correspondence.

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999) 354, 1239^1260 1239 & 1999 The Royal Society
Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1240 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

also be discussed. The last section of this paper describes


1. INTRODUCTION
approaches to functional connectivity (covariance
During the last two decades a body of well-described analysis, partial correlation coecients, covariance elds)
theories and empirically validated methods has been and network analysis of eective connectivity (structural
developed, providing a framework for investigating equations modelling). In the companion paper (Petersson
functional neuroimaging (FNI) data and making neuro- et al., following paper) we give an overview and discuss
scientic inferences. This provides a background for the some issues relating to signal detection and methods for
development of new analytical tools and experimental statistical inference used in FNI. The emphasis of both
methods. The methods for analysing FNI data are in papers is on assumptions and limitations. The descrip-
rapid development, illustrating the need for descriptive tions of the methods reviewed are necessarily brief and
tools, validated statistical models, and methods for for more details the reader is referred to the appropriate
eective statistical inference. Descriptive and exploratory literature.
methods are used to characterize the nature of the signals Several methods for FNI data analysis have been
present in the data, including possible unsuspected eects. proposed indicating that none is optimal for all purposes.
Inferential methods are used to test hypotheses and deter- In this context, it is important to know the limitations
mine condence intervals addressing the reproducibility inherent in the dierent approaches enabling the optimal
or predictability of the observed eects. use of available methods. It should be noted that focusing
Progress in science is dependent on the long-term on assumptions and limitations of the methods reviewed
consistency and convergence of empirical results. Discus- is dierent from the claim that these methods should or
sion and evaluation of the methods used in a scientic should not be used. On the contrary, this indicates the
eld is of central importance in this process (e.g. Aguirre limits of applicability and usefulness inherent in any given
et al. 1998a; Clark & Carson 1993; Ford 1995; Ford et al. method. In order to interpret accurately FNI results it is
1991; Frackowiak et al. 1996; Friston 1993, 1995; Halber et of importance to know the assumptions, approximations
al. 1997; Holmes et al. 1998; McColl et al. 1994; Petersson and inherent limitations of the methods used. When the
1998; Roland & Gulyas 1996; Strother et al. 1995b; Taylor assumptions and limitations are taken into account, the
et al. 1993; Van Horn et al. 1995; Worsley et al. 1993). In dierent methods and approaches reviewed in this paper
line with this perspective, several investigations of (and its companion paper) generally serve their purposes
dierent approaches to the statistical analysis of FNI data well. The benets and examples of their applicability are
(Arndt et al. 1995; Grabowski et al. 1996; Holmes 1994; well described in the original literature and are not
McColl et al. 1994; Xiong et al. 1996), as well as dierent repeated here.
statistical models (Holmes 1994; McColl et al. 1994; Statistical models make explicit assumptions about
Senda et al. 1998; Woods 1996), have been reported. Relia- data. Both the explicit and implicit assumptions about
bility across dierent variables has also been studied, for data need to be critically examined (Lange 1997). The
example, across laboratories (Ojemann et al. 1998; Poline methods used in FNI dier in assumptions made
et al. 1996; Senda et al. 1998), groups, sample sizes, and regarding the data and in the approximations used in the
imaging modalities (Andreasen et al. 1995; Grabowski et statistical analysis. What are of importance in this context
al. 1996; Ojemann et al. 1998; Strother et al. 1997). In addi- are not the assumptions or approximations themselves but
tion, studies of statistical power (Friston et al. 1994b, how well these are fullled by empirical data. Of crucial
1996a; Van Horn et al. 1998) and activation pattern repro- importance, in the case where these assumptions or
ducibility (Strother et al. 1997) have recently been approximations are not fully met, is the robustness of the
reported. methods used. This notion emphasizes the importance of
The eld of FNI methodology has developed into a theoretical and empirical investigations of its robustness
mature but still evolving area of knowledge. The scope of in addition to empirical validation and explicit character-
this overview is limited to the common methods used ization of the inherent limitations of a given method.
when analysing data from positron emission tomography The classic strategy for data analysis starts with data
(PET) or functional magnetic resonance imaging exploration and model selection, tting of a statistical
(fMRI), concentrating on regional cerebral blood ow model, assessing the goodness-of-t and investigating
(rCBF) PET and blood oxygenation level dependent diagnostics for violations of assumptions. If the model
(BOLD) fMRI. In this paper we give an overview of does not t or assumptions are seriously violated then the
some non-inferential methods (principal components model selection starts anew. When an appropriate model
analysis (PCA), independent components analysis (ICA), has been selected and assumptions are not seriously
and the scaled subprole model (SSM)) that in conjunc- violated, valid statistical inferences can be made. In the
tion with inspection of parameter estimates and other case of ill-tting models or violated assumptions the
simple measures can be used in the process of model ensuing inference may be statistically invalid. However, it
selection and verication of assumptions in an informal should be stressed that model selection is a complex
sense. Next, we cover some models for global eects and process and it is dicult to fully account for the inter-
approaches to global normalization. Univariate (the action between model selection and statistical inference
general linear model (GLM), xed versus random unless model selection and statistical inference are
eects), multivariate (MANCOVA, canonical variates performed on independent sets of data.
analysis (CVA), partial least squares (PLS), multivariate In general, to study a phenomenon of interest, the
linear models (MLM)), and Bayesian models, including investigator chooses an experimental design and primary
some specic issues relating to fMRI (temporal auto- FNI data are collected. The primary data are commonly
correlation, models of the haemodynamic response) will pre-processed (e.g. realigned, anatomically normalized,

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1241

and low-pass ltered), a statistical model and a test each voxel in the brain being an element in a long row
statistic chosen, model parameters estimated, and statis- vector. Stacking the rows will create a data matrix, X,
tical inference obtained, taking into account multiple where each column represents a voxel and each row a
non-independent comparisons and possible temporal scan of centred (mean corrected) data. The voxel-by-
autocorrelation (gure 1). Most FNI methods are based voxel sample variance ^ covariance matrix, proportional
on voxel (volume element) data, even though some use to XX', expresses the rst-order relationship between
regions of interest (ROI) data. Most often, the ROI each pair of voxels. A PCA of XX' can be accomplished
approach presupposes prior regionally specic hypotheses by a singular value decomposition (SVD) of X (Jollie
and brain regions outside the chosen ROIs are not investi- 1986). The SVD produces three objects: principal compo-
gated potentially leading to undetected eects. The voxel- nents (PCs), which are spatial patterns; component
by-voxel approach, pioneered by Fox et al. (Fox & scores, which are temporal or scan-order patterns; and
Mintun 1989; Fox et al. 1988) and Friston et al. (1990, singular values (eigenvalues), which express the relative
1991), was proposed as a less arbitrary alternative. variability accounted for by each PC.
Several standard approaches used in image processing PCs are dened in a natural order: the rst component
and signal detection (e.g. optimal ltering theory, linear is the single image or brain pattern (also called eigen- or
and nonlinear systems theory) are naturally applied to singular image or vector) that explains the most varia-
voxel data. Voxel approaches also preserve more of the bility across all images. Each subsequent component
inherent resolution of the imaging system and make it explains the most variability, subject to the constraint that
possible to investigate brain functions without a regional it is orthogonal to all the previous components (Jollie
specic hypothesis. However, small structures may natu- 1986). The component scores indicate the temporal
rally be viewed as ROIs and with sucient prior informa- pattern corresponding to each PC, and the singular
tion regionally specic hypothesis can be formulated and values allow for a qualitative assessment of the impor-
an ROI approach is natural. Here the prior information tance of each PC.
is used to restrict the search volume and correspondingly The SVD actually produces eigenvalues and eigenvec-
increase the sensitivity of the hypothesis testing. tors of both XX' and X'X; the former is proportional to
the sample voxel-by-voxel covariance matrix, the latter is
similar to the scan-by-scan covariance matrix. The voxel-
2. NON-INFERENTIAL METHODS FOR SIGNAL
by-voxel eigenvectors are the PCs, and the scan-by-scan
CHARACTERIZATION
eigenvectors are the component scores. The eigenvalues
Non-inferential or exploratory methods are used to are the same for both and, when scaled to sum to 1, are
characterize the nature of the signal present in data in a the proportion of variability that each eigenvector or PC
manner that does not strongly depend on a particular accounts for. There is a symmetry between PCs and
choice of model for the data. Exploratory methods can component scores: the rst component is the pattern over
play an important role both before and after statistical voxels (i.e. brain pattern) that accounts for the most
inference. Before inference, they can aid the process of variability across all scans. The rst component score is
model building and model selection by pointing to sources the pattern over scans (i.e. temporal pattern) that
of variability that might not have been expected. After accounts for the most variability across all voxels. In fact,
inference, they can similarly serve as a check that the the components and corresponding scores should be
model has adequately accounted for most of the systematic viewed as spatio-temporal objects, and interpreted
variability in the data. In this section we review three non- conjointly.
inferential methods: PCA, ICA and the SSM. We close by PCA may be used after inference to check for unex-
suggesting some basic exploratory approaches that can be pected or unaccounted patterns in the data. If the PCA
applied to almost any inferential model. produces a small number of PCs whose components
correspond to the experimental paradigm, and these PCs
(a) Principal components analysis account for a large part of the observed variability, then
PCA provides a means to identify spatio-temporal one can be fairly condent that the experiment incurred
patterns in a data-driven manner. In general, PCA is a the majority of the variability in the data, and that this
way of summarizing the sample variance ^ covariance variability is appropriately modelled. If, on the other
structure of multivariate data. As will be seen later, PCA hand, there are spatio-temporally structured components
plays a key role in multivariate statistical techniques, used that explain a large amount of variability but do not
as an exploratory tool to guide model building, to identify correspond to the experimental paradigm, then there
patterns, and to estimate the approximate dimensionality may be important additional sources of variance not
of the data, and may be used as a dimensionality reduc- included in the statistical model, potentially biasing both
tion device. Here we will concentrate on its descriptive the model coecients and variance estimates making
use. In addition, Friston et al. (1993) interpret PCA results subsequent inference invalid (cf. Petersson et al., following
as information on functional connectivity, dened as the paper). Typical examples of such sources are temporal
observed correlation over time between dierent brain eects (e.g. trends in levels of anxiety or attention,
areas. This is distinguished from eective connectivity, increasing boredom or discomfort, and eects of practice),
which is dened as the direct inuence one neural system evidenced by components similar to temporal trends or
has on another (see also 3(e) on functional connectivity PCs consisting of a few strong weightings on just the rst
and network analysis). or last replications of an experimental condition. In addi-
The denition of PCA is as follows: consider each tion, it is possible to investigate the standardized residual
brain image to be a single multivariate observation, with images with a PCA (Ford 1995), to check if there are any

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1242 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

time-series convolution experimental exploratory data statistical


of data kernel design analysis/ image
model selection

parameter
realignment filtering statistical model
estimates
random field
theory

statistical non-parametric
anatomical inference tests
normalization
predicted Monte-Carlo
time-series simulations

anatomical
reference
template

Figure 1. The classic strategy for data analysis starts with data exploration and model selection, tting of a statistical model,
assessing the goodness-of-t and investigating diagnostics for violations of assumptions. In general, proper model selection is a
necessary prerequisite for the validity of the subsequent statistical inference. Model selection is a complex process and it is dicult
to account for the interaction between model selection and statistical inference unless model selection and statistical inference are
performed on independent sets of data. In FNI, the primary data are commonly pre-processed, e.g. realigned, anatomically
normalized, and ltered. A statistical model is chosen, determined by the experimental design and the previous model selection
procedures. A test statistic is chosen and model parameters are estimated. Finally, statistical inference, taking into account
multiple non-independent comparisons and possible temporal autocorrelation, represented as a statistic image is obtained.

structured components left in the residual. If structured is possible that a given PC represents a mixture of eects,
components are found, then an assumption of indepen- and understanding spatio-temporal patterns that are
dence may be violated (Petersson et al., following paper). constrained to be orthogonal may be challenging unless
The result of a PCA may be sensitive to outliers, and is the observed variability has a natural elliptic structure.
crucially dependent on the type of pre-processing This is particularly the case with fMRI data, where there
performed on data. For example, if the image data are are multitudes of eects. While the rst component
normalized by dividing by the standard deviation image, always has the intuitive explanation of expressing the
then the PCs are based on the correlation matrix rather greatest amount of variability in the data, each of the
than the covariance matrix and the inuence of high subsequent components must be interpreted as expressing
variance regions will be reduced possibly giving a the greatest amount of variability orthogonal to the
dierent result (Chateld & Collins 1980; Jollie 1986). previous components. The easiest way to conceptualize
Another example is how the PCA of Friston et al. (1993) this is to think of the PCA as a sequential process. After
only examines voxels that survive an F-test of signicant the rst component is identied, the data are regressed
experimental variance, the result of so-called F-masking. on it and any patterns it can account for are removed.
This implies that variability not suciently well Then the next component is the pattern that expresses the
accounted for by the statistical model will not be consid- greatest amount of variability in this modied data set.
ered in the PCA, and this changes the interpretation of The same process is repeated for each subsequent compo-
the results. Rather than partition all the observed varia- nent.
bility, the PCs only partition the variability among those
voxels in which the model explains a sucient part (as (b) Independent components analysis
determined by the F-threshold) of the observed variation. The ICA is another non-inferential method closely
We note, though, that the F-masking may not always related to PCA. The ICA approach was originally
have a great impact on the results (Strother et al. 1995a). proposed by Common (Common 1994) and it has
In addition, since the PCA (Friston et al. 1993) is typically recently been applied to fMRI data (McKeown et al.
based on data after adjustments for nuisance eects, the 1998). For an introduction to ICA see McKeown et al.
results will be dependent on the particular nuisance (1998), and for theoretical details see Bell & Sejnowski
eects modelled, their model t, and the proper removal (1995); here we describe ICA as it has been applied to
of these. fMRI. The application of ICA to fMRI data so far has
There is nothing inherent in PCA to guarantee a focused on the spatial aspects of data, which may be
straightforward interpretation of the PCs. For example, it called `spatial ICA'. Like PCA, it is also possible to

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1243

analyse the temporal aspects of data with `temporal ICA'. components and scores describe the covariance structure
In contrast to PCA, the spatial patterns or component of the remaining variability (the components are named
maps (CM) generated by ICA are constrained to be not group invariance subproles, and the scores subprole
just orthogonal but statistically independent and spatially scaling factors (SSF)). These eects are estimated on the
sparse, that is, with only a few voxels having large values log-transformed data by a serial process. For purposes of
in each CM. Further, while PCA constrains the compo- explanation we assume that there is only one scan per
nent scores (temporal patterns) to be orthogonal, ICA subject, though this is not a limitation of SSM (Moeller et
puts no constraint on the temporal components. al. 1987; Strother et al. 1995a). First the SSF are estimated
ICA is motivated through information theory (Ash from a PCA of centred data (i.e. after voxel and subject
1965; Cover & Thomas 1991) with the objective of maxi- eects have been removed). The SSF consists of the rst k
mizing the joint entropy of suitably transformed CMs eigenvectors of the subject by subject sample covariance
(McKeown et al. 1998). Maximizing the joint entropy is matrix. A multivariate test of dimensionality is used to
equivalent to minimizing the mutual information, which determine k. The GSF are then estimated as the global
produces independent components. Independent compo- mean activity orthogonalized with respect to the SSFs.
nents have zero mutual information and the transforma- The SSFs represent the principal subject covariance
tion of the CMs biases the method in favour of spatially patterns across the volume, after removal of the main
sparse CMs. It must be stressed that the independence at eects of subject and location, and thus give an estimate
hand is not linear independence, a geometric property, of the vector space of subject-by-location interactions.
but statistical independence, a distributional property. This approach is intended to prevent the GSF from being
Statistical independence is a notion of separability. Two confounded with individual dierences. The group eect
random variables or random vectors are independent if, (GMP) is then estimated from the log-transformed data
and only if, their joint distribution can be written as a adjusted for the global eect; the GMP with the SSF are
product of the marginal distributions: p(x, y) p(x)p( y). used to estimate the group invariance subprole (cf.
ICAs principle strength is that the interpretation of the Moeller & Strother 1991; Strother et al. 1995a).
temporal proles may be easier compared with the Originally the authors presented evidence that their
temporal proles generated by PCA, since they are not method for estimating global eects allows for more accu-
forced to be orthogonal. As a result, several temporal rate modelling and more specic estimates of regional
patterns can be found which are very similar but not activity (rA) (Moeller et al. 1987). The imaging work
identical to the main task component. In contrast, PCA (Strother et al. 1995a) suggested that SSM was less suscep-
would not identify transiently task-related temporal tible to artefactual decreases than other methods. SSM
components since they would tend not to be orthogonal stands out for its consideration of intra-subject and inter-
to the task. The main weakness is that the spatial modes subject variation as well as the thoughtful estimation of
are required to be independent. For example, brain areas global eects. The latter is important since the method of
activated by task performance must be spatially indepen- intensity normalization can have a strong impact on the
dent of the distributions of areas aected by artefacts analysis of spatial covariance structure (Ford 1986).
(McKeown et al. 1998), though this seems not to be a Working with ROIs, Moeller et al. (1987) presented
problem in the data analysed by McKeown et al. (1998). evidence that their method for estimating global eects
However, it has been argued that functional integration allows for more accurate modelling, and more specic
among brain regions implies that large-scale cognitive estimates, of rA. Theoretical comparisons are not possible,
neural networks may have substantial anatomic overlap, since a statistical model does not back the SSM.
and that ICA precludes nonlinear interaction between
spatial modes subserving context-sensitive modulation of (d) Inspection of functional neuroimagimg data
one area by another (Friston 1998b). This issue could be We close this non-inferential section by noting some
addressed by performing a `temporal ICA', where the basic descriptive approaches that can be employed with
temporal proles are independent and the spatial proles almost any statistical model of data. Simple devices such
are unconstrained. This requires a dimensionality reduc- as viewing the parameter estimate images can be most
tion step so that the spatial dimensionality of the data is valuable in assessing model t and increasing under-
reduced below that of time. standing of the properties of data. For example, in the
case of FNI activation studies, these images would be the
(c) The scale subprole model approach parameter estimates of the dierence between two
The SSM was introduced by Moeller et al. (1987) to conditions, or in the case of a covariate regressor, the
study subject-by-region interactions while addressing the slope estimates of rA against the covariate. Examining
issue of confounding between the global and regional the images of the parameter and its estimated standard
signal. SSM is a non-inferential multivariate method error can indicate possible problematic features in data.
initially described for ROI analyses of IP-uorodeoxy-D- For example, a parameter image and corresponding t-
glucose (FDG) data and has subsequently been extended statistic image that are starkly dierent may indicate an
to 15O water activation data (Strother et al. 1995a). SSM unexpected pattern in the standard error image. Such
works with log-transformed data and is a type of factor features may be artefacts due to a failure in some pre-
ANOVA, that is, a two-way ANOVA with factor analysis processing step, but other possibilities exist. For a given
of the residuals. SSM decomposes variability into three statistical model, it may be useful to examine the dierent
dierent components: a multiplicative global scale factor components of variation represented by, for example,
(GSF), a group mean image or group mean prole subject, condition or various interactions. In this context,
(GMP), and components and scores akin to PCA. These useful images to inspect are the root of the mean sum of

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1244 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

squares related to these components. For example, Senda in a non-quantitative mode. Therefore the focus is on
et al. (1998) illustrate the usefulness of these procedures relative regional changes which are related to a baseline.
examining the mean sum of squares images in order to This can be problematic since, for example, variability
investigate the eect of dierent anatomical normal- in global factors often induces baseline uctuations.
ization procedures and dierent statistical models on the Dierent measures of global activity (gA) have been
results of the data analysis. used to account for some of the baseline variability. An
In conclusion, non-inferential methods can be most often used simple estimator of gA is the intracerebral
useful in describing properties or characteristics of parti- average of rA. Global activity dened in this way varies
cular data sets and also in outlier detection. In particular, between subjects and over time. The variability depends
simple methods can be of great value, like direct inspec- on several variables (Frackowiak et al. 1997; McColl et al.
tion of the mean dierence, variance, sums of squares 1994), for example, physiological (e.g. changes in pCO2
and residual images, or after transformation using more levels and circulatory system changes), factors relating to
sophisticated methods like PCA or SSM. The nature of the measurement procedures (e.g. dierences in injected
FNI data often makes the systematic diagnosing of model radioactive dose) and the imaging system (e.g. between-
t and verication of assumptions challenging. However, run variability in fMRI gain). Global changes are there-
many problems can be identied by simple methods and fore dicult to interpret without quantication.
close examination of the image data at all steps, from When there is a lack of absolute quantication and the
initial data collection through model tting. Unexplained experimentally induced regional changes are assessed
structure in the normalized residual image may be inves- relative to a baseline, changes in this baseline are often
tigated by PCA. Recent elaboration of the PCA concept considered a nuisance eect. Since baseline uctuations
in terms of subspace transformations like projection may be large, potentially hiding the eects of interest, it
pursuit (Huber 1985), or other related approaches like is necessary to account for or remove this variability in
ICA, may turn out to be valuable exploratory tools and some appropriate manner. The notion of baseline varia-
are naturally applied to FNI data. bility as a nuisance eect implicitly assumes that the
scan-to-scan baseline uctuations are independent of the
experimental manipulations. In order to properly account
3. STATISTICAL MODELS FOR FUNCTIONAL
for baseline variability there are two issues. First, how to
NEUROIMAGING
measure or estimate the baseline uctuations, and second,
As a prelude to statistical inference, non-inferential how these measurements are used to explicitly model or
descriptive methods can aid in the process of model remove the variability in baseline activity. Measurements
building and model selection. The importance of reason- of global eects, and consequently global normalization,
able model t and verication of assumptions cannot be is predicated on the assumption that the variability in
overestimated since this is of central importance for the global eects adequately represent the baseline uctua-
validity of the subsequent statistical inference (cf. tions and that the experimentally induced regional
Petersson et al., following paper). In general, statistical changes are superimposed on this according to some
inference requires a suciently well-tting statistical model.
model and several statistical models have been described
and applied to FNI data. In this section, models for (i) Dierent approaches to global normalization
global eects and approaches to global normalization are Several approaches to account for global changes have
described. In particular, the importance of correctly been proposed and compared. For example, proportional
modelling a uctuating baseline, potential confounding scaling (Fox & Raichle 1984; Kanno et al. 1996), log-
between global and regional eects, and limitations of the linear regression models (Herholz et al. 1993), histogram^
commonly used models in the case of several dierent rank equalization (Arndt et al. 1996), Z-score transforma-
ranges of global eects will be considered. Dierent issues tion of data (McIntosh et al. 1996b), or modelled as a
relating to univariate statistical inference (e.g. sources of nuisance covariate in the GLM (Friston et al. 1990). Both
random variation, xed versus random eects and scope the ANCOVA (Friston 1995; Friston et al. 1990; Ramsay et
of inference), Bayesian, and multivariate models (e.g. al. 1993) and the proportional scaling (Kanno et al. 1996)
dimensionality reduction, the need to characterize the approaches have been empirically validated for PETdata.
multivariate signal when detected) are also discussed. The relation between rCBF and global cerebral blood
Last, some issues specic to fMRI (e.g. temporal autocor- ow (gCBF) is most likely nonlinear. However, over
relation, models of the haemodynamic response) are small ranges it can be expected that the relation is well
covered. approximated as linear. For normal subjects and small
ranges of gCBF, the incorporation of the gCBF as a
(a) Baseline uctuations and global normalization covariate in a linear model aords a reasonably good
FNI experiments usually test hypotheses regarding model of the relationship between rCBF and gCBF
regionally specic changes in neuronal activity. These (Frackowiak et al. 1997). The additive ANCOVA model
changes are, in the case of PET, indirectly indicated by was proposed under the assumption that changes in gCBF
changes in rCBF or regional cerebral counts (rCC), and and the experimentally induced changes in rCBF are well
by changes in regional susceptibility in the case of approximated as independent (Ramsay et al. 1993). It was
BOLD fMRI. (Below regional activity will represent pointed out that the results of this approach may be
rCBF, rCC, or regional fMRI BOLD time-series, problematic to interpret if changes in gCBF are correlated
depending on the imaging modality.) For practical and with experimentally induced changes in rCBF (Ramsay
other reasons, the imaging systems are commonly used et al. 1993; see also Aguirre et al. 1998b; Andersson 1997)

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1245

or if the gCBF estimation is biased. This is also the case estimated in this way is more or less confounded with the
for proportional scaling. Recent fMRI studies indicate experimental paradigm. Whether this confounding is
that there may be complex interactions between induced appreciable to the point of aecting results is an empirical
vasodilation (CO2 modulation, Bandettini & Wong 1997; matter. However, there are indications that this sometimes
acetazolamide modulation, Bruhn et al. 1994) and activa- can be the case. Suppose, for example, that state A activates
tion-related BOLD signal changes. For example, Bandet- several brain regions compared to a reference state B. Then,
tini & Wong (1997) observed that the amplitude of in addition to components reecting baseline uctuations in
activation-induced signal changes was damped during the two states, the estimate of gA in A will contain a
hypercapnia. However, there are some indications that component reecting regional activations in relation to B.
the interaction between activation-related signal changes This implies that voxels will tend to get relatively overcor-
and vasodilation induced by hypercapnia can be limited rected in A but not in B, biasing the distribution of voxel
(Coreld et al. 1998) in certain cases. values, and increasing the risk of artefactual deactivations
The variability in global cerebral counts (gCC) is often or decreased sensitivity for detecting activations. Consistent
larger than in gCBF. Even if gCBF is relatively constant, with this are observations of a greater than expected
subject dierences in head fraction and variability in the proportion of negative Z-score voxel values (Andersson
introduced radioactive dose cause variability in gCC. In 1997; Strother et al. 1995a, 1996). This problem should be
the case of count data, rCC is proportional to gCC when less important if closely matched activation and reference
rCBF is constant. If it is expected that the variability in, states are used. However, with increasing activation dier-
for example, head fraction or introduced radioactive dose ences between states, this may become a signicant
is dominating, proportional scaling is a reasonable problem, reiterating the need for carefully designed experi-
approach. It has been suggested that the regional ments that includes active reference conditions.
variance may increase with increased radioactive dose It has been suggested that when the global signal is
reecting the Poisson origin of data (Frackowiak et al. signicantly confounded with the experimental para-
1997). In the case of a large range or variability in gCC, digm, it may be preferable in some situations to omit
a proportional model may therefore be preferred, in global normalization entirely and examine absolute
particular since this approach has variance-stabilizing changes (Aguirre et al. 1998b). An alternative strategy is
properties (Holmes 1994). The Z-score transformation to use a more robust measure of gA, that is estimating gA
also has variance-stabilizing properties (McIntosh et al. independent of task-induced changes in rA to give a more
1996b). The use of variance-stabilizing transforms is a accurate estimate of baseline uctuations. One possibility
well-known approach in statistics (Bickel & Docksum is to estimate the background activity from white matter
1977) and estimation of variance-stabilizing transforms or by examining brain regions known to be relatively
with applications to metabolic PET data have recently unaected by the experimental paradigm (Frackowiak et
been described (Moeller & Strother 1991; Ruttimann et al. 1997). An iterative solution to the latter suggestion has
al. 1998). The empirical comparisons performed so far been proposed that successively eliminates voxels that
have yielded little dierences between the proposed indicate experimental eect from the set used to compute
approaches to global normalization, neither for PET data gA (Andersson 1997). These suggestions should work su-
(Arndt et al. 1996; Frackowiak et al. 1997; Holmes 1994; ciently well in the case of relative moderate or localized
McIntosh et al. 1996b) nor for fMRI data (Aguirre et al. activations but may become problematic in the case of
1998b). More specically, in most FNI studies using large spatially extended activations that are not matched
normal subjects, the results are similar using either in the reference condition.
proportional scaling or the ANCOVA approach.
(Provided the eect of gA in the ANCOVA approach is (iii) Global normalization in group studies and pharmacological
allowed to vary between subjects, i.e. a subject-specic neuroimaging
ANCOVA model of gA. This allows for a subject-by-gA Since the relationship between rA and gA is most likely
interaction. However, if the eect of gA is included as a nonlinear it may be inappropriate to use simple additive
single eect in the GLM, with a single global slope used or proportional models to account for global eects in
for all subjects, there may be dierences in results situations in which the gA varies over large or dierent
between the ANCOVA and proportional scaling ranges. For example, in pharmacological neuroimaging
approach.) there may be several separated ranges of gA,
corresponding to several drug-altered states, each
(ii) Confounding of global and regional eects demanding a dierent linear approximation. Comparing
Another important issue in the context of global normal- these states, either directly or via their interaction on a
ization is whether global and regional eects are task^baseline comparison, eectively extrapolates the
confounded; that is, global and regional eects can be data beyond the dierent gA ranges observed to compare
more or less dependent. If the gA is estimated as the intra- with a gA attained in neither condition. The validity of
cerebral spatial average of rA there is a risk that the varia- such comparisons is dependent on the appropriateness of
bility in gA may be signicantly confounded with the extrapolation, which in turn is determined by the
behavioural manipulations or experimentally induced accuracy of the local gA model over the larger gA range
eects. Since the gA is the average of rA, and rA is implied by the comparison. Most of the simple models
hypothesized to correlate with experimental manipulations, described so far may not be comprehensive enough to
the gA may be correlated with changes in experimental approximate the actual behaviour of gA over large
conditions, unless all regional increases are matched by ranges. Using an inappropriate model may imply that the
regional decreases. This seems unlikely, implying that gA results of dierent comparisons simply highlight areas

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1246 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

where the gA model is inappropriate; that is, real eects First, there are usually far more voxels than observations,
are confounded by apparent eects that are related to an which prevents the standard use of an arbitrary spatial
ill-tting model. In addition, if the drug greatly aects covariance structure. (The covariance structure may be
the gA, then it is likely that the global value and drug estimated, but the estimate is singular, precluding most
concentration covariates are highly correlated. Under multivariate analyses, which require the determinant of
such circumstances the eects are highly parallel, and if the estimated variance ^ covariance matrix.) Second,
an ANCOVA approach is used, estimates of the global multivariate techniques characterize image volumes
and drug eects will be unstable. Similar problems arise (observations) as a whole, and not as individual elements.
with the proportional scaling approach. Hence they do not directly address regionally specic
Other examples when simple models of global eects questions, and commonly do not allow for any statistical
may be problematic are when patients with gross struc- inference at specic voxels. In this section, we review
tural or functional abnormalities (signicantly aecting some common univariate statistical models used in FNI;
the gA) are compared with normal controls, or, when the process of assigning signicance to the results of the
ictal scans are compared with inter-ictal scans in model, statistical inference, is reserved for the companion
epileptic patients. In the ictal scans there may be intense paper (Petersson et al., following paper).
focal activity in the seizure area, contributing to the Univariate modelling is a well-developed area, whose
global mean measure of gA. Normalizing for the global methods form the core toolbox of statistics (e.g. Bickel &
eect by simple standard approaches, in eect inter- Docksum 1977; Edgington 1995; Good 1994; Winer et al.
preting changes relative to the global measure, will then 1991). The enterprise consists of estimating the relation-
tend to underestimate the magnitude of the ictal activa- ship between known eects (e.g. condition, time, subject,
tion and other areas which have not changed their level performance) and the data, then using the estimated
of activity will appear deactivated, biasing the results. eects to eliminate systematic variability from the data,
One approach here has been to use reference regions to leaving only residual variability, which is used to estimate
estimate the baseline ow and normalize in relation to the variance parameter(s) in the model. There is a trade-
this measure. However, the results of such an approach o between including all conceivable explanatory vari-
are sensitive to the choice of reference region (McCrory ables (eects), and parsimony, using the fewest number of
& Ford 1991). eects to form an adequate model. Including too many
In summary, several dierent approaches to global eects can make the t of the model too specic to the
normalization have been proposed and empirical data at hand, degrading the generalizability of the
comparisons indicate that there is little dierence results, while not including an eect that is present in the
between them. Which model is most appropriate depends data generally will inate the residual variability and
on the sources of global variability. For example, if the hence bias the estimate of the model variance.
major part of the variability is due to dierences in the Suppose there is a parsimonious or true model, such
amount of injected activity (PET), or drifts in gain that the residuals are independently identically distrib-
(fMRI), then a proportional model may be appropriate. uted. Using an over-parameterized model reduces the
If, on the other hand, the variability is due to physiolo- power, since each additional eect modelled consumes
gical variability (e.g. heart rate, pCO2) the choice is more degrees of freedom (d.f.), while the additional variability
dicult. With proportional scaling, the noise is scaled, explained is limited. This reduction in power may be
but is unaected by the additive ANCOVA model. This negligible when there are many d.f., but when there are
has implications for the validity of subsequent statistical few d.f. (e.g. in PETor random eects models), additional
analysis. The gA can be confounded with the experi- eects will, by reducing d.f., increase uncertainty in the
mental induced eects if gA is estimated as a simple estimate of the model variance. Hence with fewer than
spatial average of rA. Hence, it may be necessary to use about 20 d.f., adding an eect to the model will reduce
more robust methods to estimate the global eects inde- the certainty of the variance estimate and the signicance
pendent of experimentally induced regional eects. of a given eect (unless including the additional eect
Furthermore, when large or several dierent ranges of gA signicantly reduces the residual variability). Using
are expected, it may be necessary to rst empirically instead a reduced model has two consequences. First, the
study the behaviour of gA over the relevant ranges, in unmodelled eects will appear in the residuals in a struc-
order to develop a more comprehensive model of gA. This tured way, introducing dependencies among the residuals.
is necessary in order to base comparisons on appropri- This implies that the assumptions are not fullled,
ately modelled gA. More comprehensive models of global making standard inference invalid. Practically, the unmo-
eects may also be necessary to account for baseline delled eects usually, but not always, inate the variance
uctuations in fMRI data. estimate. Second, there are more d.f. available for esti-
mating the variance, implying increased condence in the
(b) Univariate statistical models estimated variance, and therefore greater signicance for
A single observation in FNI is generally a volume or a given change, with increased eect at lower d.f. So, on
image of voxels. When individual observations are not the one hand the variance increases, which would suggest
scalars, multivariate strategies are often the natural decreased power, but on the other hand the d.f. increases,
approach to statistical modelling. However, for unique suggesting increased power. In general, all that can be
reasons, the most common FNI analysis strategy is essen- said is that the inference is invalid, reiterating the impor-
tially a (massively) univariate approach, where identical tance of appropriate model selection. However, at high
univariate models are tted at each voxel, so-called d.f. (e.g. fMRI and xed eects models) the additional
image regression. There are two main reasons for this. d.f. are unlikely to have much impact, and one might

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1247

expect inference to be conservative in general. Note that linear combinations of the parameters can be made under
the `true' model may still be over-tted (under-tted) in the normality assumption, which gives
that parameters may be included (excluded) just because
there is evidence for (against) them in this particular ,C(X 0 X) 1 C 0 ),
Cb  N(C
data set, such that the model is larger (smaller) than need
be when applied to a subsequent data set, degrading where C is a row vector of length p, often called a
generalizability of the results. contrast (see further Frackowiak et al. 1997). As an
Another consideration is the sources of random varia- example of how this framework accounts for the basic
tion. The most basic models consider only one source, the models, consider the ubiquitous t-test for comparing the
residual error variation. These models are called xed sample A with B. In this case, the X-matrix will consist
eects models as all the model eects are considered xed of two columns composed of ones and zeros; for obser-
(but unknown). A hierarchical mixed or random eects vations belonging to group A, the corresponding
model has multiple sources of random variation. An elements in the rst column of X will be one, the
eect is considered to be a random eect if it is the reali- elements of the second column zero; for observations
zation of a stochastic variable, that is, its values can be belonging to group B, the corresponding elements in the
considered random draws from a population. For rst column of X will be zero, the second column one.
example, subjects that are studied are typically considered Then applying the machinery above with C 1 1
as randomly drawn from some population. In contrast, will eect a t-test. Note that we had to make no special
the levels of parametric manipulation of an experimental accommodations for unequal group sizes. Note, also, if
task are systematic, and not randomly drawn from a we had three groups instead of two, we simply add
greater population. Random eects models will be another column to X and create contrasts C that will
further discussed below. express tests of interest; this is equivalent to estimating
The general linear model (GLM) is a framework that contrasts for a xed eect ANOVA. When the X-matrix
encompasses all basic univariate xed eects models, e.g. consists of a column of ones and column containing a
ANOVA, ANCOVA, and multiple regression models. continuous covariate, a linear regression is aected, and
(Note that while we abbreviate general linear models as the test for zero slope (Cb,C 0 1) is equivalent to a
GLM, it should be recognized that statisticians currently test of a zero correlation coecient (Snedecor &
use GLM to refer to generalized linear models, a regres- Cochran 1967).
sion framework for non-normal, possibly discrete data One application when the homogeneity of variance
(see McCullagh & Nelder 1989). Fortunately the GLM is assumption may not be valid is the case of pharmacolo-
a special case of the generalized linear model, so the gical neuroimaging. The pre- and the post-drug scans
ambiguity is not a source of problems.) In the GLM may show dierences in terms of variance. For example,
framework n observations (from a single voxel) are repre- the neurophysiological response may be more stable than
sented as a column vector of length n, Y; the p eects and the post-drug response, implying that the variance appro-
predictor variables are represented as p column vectors priate for assessing the post-drug scans is underestimated.
also of length n, forming an n p matrix X called the The GLM cannot directly account for heterogeneity of
design matrix. The xed unknown parameters are repre- variance and including the pre- and post-drug scans into
sented as a column vector of length p; the residual a comprehensive analysis introduces bias violating the
random error is written as the column vector  of length common assumption of homogeneous variance. Similar
n. With the assumption of mean zero, independent and problems may arise in FNI studies of patients. Instead
identically distributed error of magnitude 2, the concise other approaches have to be used.
representation of the GLM is
(i) Random versus xed eects models the scope of inference
and var(Y) 2 I,
E(Y) X Fixed eects model parameters are linear combina-
tions of the data (see equations above), and so can be
where I is the n  n identity matrix. Note that we have estimated very eciently. In contrast, random eect
made no specic distributional assumptions; the usual parameters are generally not linear functions of the
normality assumption is only needed for statistical infer- data, and requiring nonlinear, iterative methods of esti-
ence. Using only the general assumptions above, mation (e.g. expectation maximization, Laird & Ware
according to the Gauss^Markov theorem (Bickel & (1982)). There is one notable special case where the stan-
Docksum 1977), the linear unbiased estimates of and 2 dard GLM can be used with a random eects model,
that are best in terms of minimizing squared estimation that is, when there is only one random eect, the design
error are given by balanced, and the model is separable into individual
subject models. In this case the data can be analysed in
b (X 0 X) 1 X 0 Y, two stages. For the rst stage, each subject is analysed
s2 1/(n p)(Y Xb)0 (Y Xb), individually, creating images of parameters of interest,
one for each subject. For the second stage, the parameter
where b and s2 are the estimate of the true unknown images are analysed, with a one-sample t-test or, if there
and 2, respectively. The form of b can be found from are two groups, with a two-sample t-test (Holmes &
algebraic manipulation of Y X . Note that Y Xb is Friston 1998).
the residuals, so that the form of s2 is just the mean The scope of inference of xed eects is for those
squared residuals (the n p reecting the dimensionality values of the xed eects, that is, if a subject eect is
of the residuals that are left after tting p eects). Tests of regarded as xed then the inferences are for the cohort of

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1248 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

subjects studied. Random eects models are used to make (ii) Statistical challenges for the analysis of fMRI data
inferences about the population sampled. The only In this subsection, three principal statistical challenges
variance modelled in a xed eects model of repeated introduced by fMRI will be addressed. First, the problem
observations on a group of subjects is the within-subject, to identify a function of time that predicts the haemo-
within-condition variability (the variability from scan-to- dynamic response, the haemodynamic response function
scan of the same condition within an individual). This (HRF). Second, methods to account for the temporal
includes measurement variability, confounded with other autocorrelation of fMRI data, and third, modelling of
physiological, physical and cognitive eects. The random slow variations drift in fMRI data. The rst issue relates
eects model additionally accounts for the between- to the fact that the BOLD response is delayed and
subject variability. This implies that a xed eect analysis dispersed in time, the second that the residual error of
can declare a signicant eect in a set of subjects, while a fMRI time-series are dependent (Aguirre et al. 1997;
random eects analysis can declare an eect signicant Boynton et al. 1996; Friston et al. 1994a, 1995; Purdon &
for the population sampled. This is especially important Weissko 1998; Weissko et al. 1993; Worsley & Friston
for group comparisons. 1995; Zarahn et al. 1997), and the last issue regards slow
While it might seem that one would only want to changes in the fMRI signal whose sources are unknown,
perform population inference, there are several critical but whose occurrence is common.
issues to consider. First, the ability to randomly sample a The BOLD contrast is the result of an interaction
broad population of human subjects is often practically between neuronal activity, oxygen extraction, blood ow
impossible. When non-random samples are used, the and blood volume (Buxton et al. 1998; Ogawa et al. 1998;
random eects models will have questionable validity. Vazquez & Noll 1998). While the exact relationship
The lack of correct random sampling is an argument for between the quantity of interest, the neuronal activity,
the use of xed eects models. It has been argued that in and the BOLD response is unknown, the qualitative char-
the context of non-random sampling, xed eect (non- acter of the HRF is well known. Given a discrete on ^ o
parametric) models should be used in conjunction with stimulus, the BOLD response is delayed and dispersed in
non-statistical generalization, as a natural part of science time; from time of stimulus start, there is approximately
(Edgington 1995). Note that even if the non-random a 2^3 s delay until an appreciable response (some labora-
sample can be viewed as a `representative' sample from tories report an immediate, small-magnitude negative
some population of interest, dening the correct popula- response (Hu et al. 1997)), a maximum is reached after
tion, if it exists, seems dicult. Hence the results from approximately 4^7 s, and after stimulus cessation there is
random eects models of non-random samples are inher- a delay before the signal falls and it generally falls to
ently dicult to interpret. Second, if it can be argued that negative (the `undershoot') before returning to baseline.
a population is randomly sampled, random eects models The exact character, especially the delay until response,
have the additional assumption of normality of the popu- is variable both between subjects, and across the brain
lation sampled. This is often an assumption that is di- within a subject (Aguirre et al. 1998c).
cult to verify, in particular for a broad population of The earliest approaches used t-tests and simple tests on
human subjects. Last, the second-level analysis of the the correlation coecient (Bandettini et al. 1993) to deter-
random eects model (Holmes et al. 1998) has degrees of mine signicant changes. F-test methods comparing the
freedom determined by the number of subjects. This power at the paradigm frequency with the average power
implies that the number of subjects included in a random have also been used (Bullmore et al. 1996; Lange & Zeger
eects study is central for sensitivity and statistical power. 1997), with the advantage that they are insensitive to the
This has implications for the use of random eld theory exact phase or delay of the response. However, they suer
methods for statistical inference which are best at large when the haemodynamic response departs signicantly
d.f. (cf. Petersson et al. 1999; Worsley et al. 1996). In the from a sinusoidal response, since they are equivalent to
situation of low d.f., the use of the smooth random eld regressing the data on a sine and cosine with frequency
approach to statistical inference can be problematic and identical to the paradigm. Recently, a 2 random eld
lack sensitivity for voxel level inference. However, alter- approach for detecting sinusoidal signals has been
native approaches are available, for example, the use of described (Worsley 1999; Worsley et al. 1997a).
variance smoothing and pseudo t-tests in combination Many recent approaches have focused on treating the
with non-parametric inference (Holmes et al. 1996); this is BOLD response as a linear time-invariant (LTI) system
further discussed in Petersson et al. (following paper). (Jain 1989; Oppenheim & Schafer 1989). The linearity
It should be noted that group comparisons are not implies that the predicted haemodynamic response is the
served well by xed eects models, since a between-group convolution of a xed impulse response function (IRF or
dierence must be compared with a measure of between- HRF) with the waveform of the experimental paradigm.
group variation, that is, a between-subject estimate of The time invariance implies that the IRF is stationary, that
subject variability within group. A xed eects model is, independent of time and prior responses. This approach
with repeated measures only accounts for within-subject is attractive as the IRF completely characterizes the system
variability and measurement error. For fMRI the within- (Oppenheim & Schafer 1989). Usually the IRF is assumed
subject variability is most certainly smaller than the to be known and the statistical modelling reduces to esti-
between-subject variability, and hence signicance of mating the amplitude of the response. We begin by giving
between-group dierences will be overestimated. It an overview of models for the IRF and then review the
follows that group comparisons aiming at inference about evidence for the BOLD response as an LTI system.
population dierences are crucially dependent on the use Use of the t-tests corresponds to a Dirac-delta HRF.
of random eects models. Early work used functions corresponding to common

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1249

probability distributions, though there is no theoretical delay of the HRF, while not expressible in a statistical
motivation for this, just a qualitative similarity between linear model, can be approximated for small delays by
the distributions and the HRF. Friston et al. (1994a) rst including the temporal derivative of the HRF in the
used a Poisson probability mass function; others have model (Friston et al. 1998).
used the slightly more exible gamma density (Cohen In general, arbitrary HRF models that are not linear
1997; Lange & Zeger 1997). Friston et al. (1995) later used in their parameters must be tted with nonlinear
the Gaussian density not so much as an IRF but as part methods. Recently models that do not make the assump-
of a ltering approach motivated by the matched lter tion that the BOLD response is an LTI system have been
theorem (Rosenfeld & Kak 1982). Others have also used described (Frank et al. 1998; Genovese 1997; Genovese &
the Gaussian kernel (Rajapakse et al. 1998). An alterna- Sweeney 1998; Lange & Zeger 1997). These approaches
tive approach is to specify not a single IRF but a family can yield richer information on the form of the HRF.
or basis of IRFs; this will potentially enhance the t of a Frank et al. (1998) parameterize the response amplitude
model, though possibly at the expense of the interpret- as a proportion of baseline to demonstrate a nonlinear
ability of the t. A third approach is to rst estimate the model. Genovese also uses proportional response ampli-
IRF on an independent data set. tudes, but elaborately parameterizes the shape of the
HRF with eight parameters. In addition, recent work has
(iii) Models of the haemodynamic response function and linear time been focusing on directly modelling blood ow, blood
invariance volume and blood oxygenation (Buxton & Frank 1997;
It is generally accepted that the transformation from Buxton et al. 1998; Davis et al. 1994); these approaches
the experimentally induced signal via the neural response may improve the understanding of the BOLD response
and the BOLD response generated to the fMRI signal and hold the potential for quantication in fMRI.
(i.e. input!neural activity!BOLD response!fMRI
measurement) is not a perfect LTI (Boynton et al. 1996; (iv) Temporal autocorrelation and dependent residuals
Dale & Buckner 1997; Friston et al. 1998; Rosen et al. 1998; The development of the described GLM above was
Vazquez & Noll 1998). It is an open question whether it is based on the assumption that the residual errors were
well approximated as an LTI system or not. The central mean zero, had constant variance and were independent.
issues are identifying the conditions under which is it well It has been widely observed that fMRI time-series display
approximated as linear and time invariant, and charac- temporal autocorrelation (Aguirre et al. 1997; Boynton et
terizing the forms and sources of the nonlinearities. The al. 1996; Friston et al. 1994a, 1995; Purdon & Weissko
primary visual system seems satisfactorily approximated 1998; Weissko et al. 1993; Zarahn et al. 1997). While auto-
as an LTI, over a range of stimulus duration (5^22.5 s) correlation does not bias estimates of the eects (e.g.
and contrasts (0^100%) (Boynton et al. 1996). A recent mean dierence), it does bias estimates of variability, thus
investigation of this question, also in the primary visual aecting the signicance of eects and altering the false
system (Vazquez & Noll 1998), found evidence indicating positive rates. Here, we review two general approaches to
that stimuli shorter than 4 s and less than 40% contrast coping with correlated errors, and how they have been
yielded signicant nonlinear responses. So, the transfor- applied in the literature.
mation from neural activity to the BOLD response may There are two linear model approaches to autocorrela-
(under suitable conditions) be well approximated as an tion, generalized least squares (GLS) and ordinary least
LTI system in primary sensory (visual) areas, when squares (OLS) with adjustment for correlated errors.
driven by simple sensory input. Whether this generalizes GLS in essence de-correlates, or `whitens', the data and
to other brain regions and experimental paradigms then applies OLS. The second approach uses OLS on the
engaging higher cognitive function is an open question correlated data (in violation of its assumptions) and then
(for an interesting example see Buckner et al. (1998a,b)). It approximates the null distributions of test statistics by
is presently hypothesized that higher cognitive functions adjusting the conventional degrees of freedom to the so-
are subserved by nonlinear network interactions (Amit called eective degrees of freedom. The GLS approach is
1989; Arbib 1995; Koch & Davis 1994; Rumelhart & the optimal linear estimator (minimum variance in the
McClelland 1986; Schuster 1991). In addition, learning, class of unbiased estimators), and corresponds to the
memory and the capacity of the brain to adapt in a non- maximum likelihood estimator for Gaussian data, but is
stationary environment indicate that the transformation sensitive to the correct form of the covariance matrix.
input!neural activity is neither linear nor time invar- The latter approach is not optimal, but it is more robust
iant. This represents challenging problems for the analysis to mispecications of the covariance structure (Worsley
of fMRI data. & Friston 1995; Worsley et al. 1997c). However, there are
Statistical modelling under the assumption of linearity special cases when the OLS-then-correct approach is
is straightforward, as the predicted haemodynamic optimal, for example, when the regressors are sinusoidal
response may be used as a covariate in a multiple regres- (Worsley & Friston 1995). Time-series methods provide
sion. A distinction must be made between the assumption alternative means to account for autocorrelation (Chat-
of linearity of the BOLD response and linearity of the eld 1996; Wei 1990), though these fall into the former
statistical models, which simply means the data is model GLS category as they tend to use whitening to provide
as an (unknown) linear combination of (known) predic- unbiased variance estimates.
tors. For example, Friston and colleagues (Friston & To our knowledge, the GLS approach has not been
Buechel 1998; Friston et al. 1998) model nonlinearities in used directly, but rather through the machinery of time-
the HRF with a linear model using a truncated Volterra series methods. Bullmore et al. (1996) performed a
series expansion. Another example is that the variable thorough statistical analysis of a collection of voxels and

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1250 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

found that a rst-order autoregressive model (AR(1)) comprehensive models improving the quality of the statis-
seemed appropriate. Locascio et al. (1997) applied general tical modelling.
autoregressive moving average (ARMA) modelling at a
voxel-by-voxel basis. Prudon & Weissko (1998) suggest (c) Bayesian models for functional neuroimaging
the use of an AR(1) plus white noise model. data
The OLS model with adjusted null distribution was Bayesian methods can be used to incorporate prior
rst presented to the FNI community by Worsley & knowledge in a systematic fashion (Bretthorst 1990a,b;
Friston (Friston et al. 1995; Worsley & Friston 1995). In Descombes et al. 1998; Frank et al. 1998; Genovese 1997;
these works, a temporal smoothing lter was applied Genovese & Sweeney 1998; Holmes & Ford 1993). In
referring to the matched lter theorem (Rosenfeld & Kak general, Bayesian methods can be used in two dierent
1982). It was then assumed that the intrinsic autocorrela- ways; either for Bayesian estimation, or as a method for
tion was negligible relative to the temporal smoothing stochastic regularization (cf. Petersson et al., following
kernel, such that the form of the covariance matrix can paper). Bayesian estimation is naturally biased towards
be approximated by that determined by the temporal the prior information used (Billingsley 1995) and can be
smoothing kernel. The temporal smoothing of fMRI regarded as enforcing soft constraints on parameter
time-series acts as a low-pass lter, discarding high- values. However, the Bayesian approach provides a
frequency eects. An alternative approach is to empiri- coherent framework for statistical analysis (Box & Tiao
cally model the intrinsic temporal autocorrelation, for 1992; Lee 1997), of which the classical approach can be
example, using a 1/f form in the square root of the power viewed as a special case. The bulk of statistical tools are
spectrum, estimated from null data and pooled across the labelled frequentist, and their justication is built on
brain (Aguirre et al. 1997; Zarahn et al. 1997). It was found repeated sampling from a theoretical population. The
that the convolution with an empirically determined observed data are a realization of a random process and
HRF, and an assumed 1/f intrinsic autocorrelation, the parameters of interest are xed and unknown quanti-
produced the most accurate false-positive rates with ties. In contrast, the Bayesian approach regards the para-
smoothed data (Aguirre et al. 1997; Zarahn et al. 1997). meters as random variables as well. Before an experiment
Signicantly, the use of the 1/f intrinsic autocorrelation is performed, the parameters have an a priori distribution,
function with no HRF convolution did not control the called the prior. After the experiment is performed the
false-positive rate (using the local maximum statistic), a prior is updated to give the posterior distribution, which
result explained by spatially inhomogeneous autocorrela- reects the information gained from the data. Questions
tion, that is, the degree of autocorrelation varied across about the parameters are addressed via the posterior
the brain. This inhomogeneity has also been noted by distribution: for example, the probability that the
others (Purdon et al. 1998). Finally, the temporal response amplitude is greater than zero is simply the inte-
smoothing may not be benecial for event-related fMRI gral of the posterior from zero to innity.
data. Since the BOLD HRF has high-frequency compo- While regarding unknown, unobservable quantities
nents, temporal smoothing discards the high-frequency (the parameters) as random may seem intuitively reason-
components of the signal, and this portion of the signal able, it is an area of contention in statistics. The main
may convey specic localizing information (Paradis et al. criticism is the potential subjective element inherent in
1998). assigning prior distributions. Despite this issue, and the
Finally, the source of slow variations in fMRI time- ubiquity of classical statistical tools, Bayesian methods
series is not well understood, but represents a large source are gaining popularity. This growth is due in large part
of variability (Genovese 1997). The likely causes include to increased access to the necessary computing power.
slow subject motion artefacts, biorhythms (respiratory Bayesian methods can also have a greater intuitive
and cardiac pulsation artefacts) and physiological defor- appeal than frequentist methods. For example, the
mation of the brain. These drifts have been modelled in a frequentist 95% condence interval is often misinter-
variety of ways, including linear slopes (Fitzgerald 1996), preted as the range in which the parameter lies with
exponentials (Vazquez & Noll 1998), and discrete cosine probability 0.95; in fact, this is the correct interpretation
bases (Holmes et al. 1997). The last approach essentially for the Bayesian credible interval. The correct inter-
implements a high-pass lter as a part of the model. pretation for a condence interval is that with repeated
There have also been attempts to directly correct physio- experiments with identical conditions, 95% of the con-
logical eects, such as respiration and the cardiac cycle dence intervals created will contain the true, xed,
(Hu et al. 1995). unknown parameter.
We note that most approaches described use the same Bayesian methods are also understood as a formalized
model at every voxel. For independent data this amounts version of regularized optimization, or penalized likeli-
to using the same regressors at every voxel, but for hood, where the standard maximum-likelihood approach
temporally correlated data it additionally means using is biased towards favourable parameter values. From this
the same autocorrelation function model at every voxel. approach, one can understand the Bayesian and frequen-
An alternative is to use model selection techniques and tist perspectives together. With increasing number of
consider ARMA models of arbitrary order at each voxel observations, the prior becomes less and less important,
(Locascio et al. 1997). Locascio et al. (1997) found that and hence the Bayesian results will converge to the
some voxels required higher order ARMA terms, while likelihood-only frequentist result. One can also think of
others passed a test for white noise without whitening. increasing the spread of the prior until it is at, at which
New approaches may leave behind the computational point the Bayesian machinery will in general produce the
convenience of image regression in exchange for more frequentist result.

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1251

Frank et al. (1998) present a good introduction to the straightforward applications of standard multivariate
general Bayesian framework for fMRI, calling it a `prob- statistics, since the estimated covariance structure of the
abilistic analysis', considering illustrative examples. A key data will be singular. In the case of a few pre-selected
point of this work is that if one considers more general ROIs and large enough number of measurements, stan-
models than permitted by regression, a much richer variety dard multivariate statistics are applicable. However,
of questions can be answered. For example, a haemo- several multivariate voxel approaches have been adapted
dynamic delay with xed HRF could be directly modelled. to the context of FNI. These approaches often aim at
For prior distributions they constrain themselves to so characterizing the overall distributed pattern of experi-
called non-informative priors, which are invariant under mentally induced changes in brain activity regardless of
natural transformations of the parameter (Jaynes 1968). location. In general, this precludes statistical inference
Genovese (1997; Genovese & Sweeney 1998) presents a about regional specic eects; that is, the regional struc-
Bayesian model which considers a very general HRF. ture of the experimental eect can only be interpreted
Dened as a sum of polynomial bells, the HRF smoothly descriptively. This lack of localizing power in multivariate
rises to, and falls from, a at plateau. In addition, the approaches implies that univariate techniques are
shape and response amplitude is separately parameter- complementary to the multivariate. Some of the
ized. For example, the shape is described by at least four suggested multivariate techniques (for an overview, see
parameters: the delay until initial rise; time of plateau Worsley 1997b) are closely related to CVA (Chateld &
start; delay until fall from plateau; and time of return to Collins 1980). CVA is a standard multivariate technique
baseline (delays are relative to stimulus onset and cessa- for selecting the linear compound of the multivariate
tion, respectively). Further, the model includes baseline response, which demonstrates the greatest inconsistency
drifts modelled with cubic smoothing splines. This repre- between the experimentally induced eect and the null
sents an alternative to the use of high-pass lters to hypothesis (Chateld & Collins 1980). The CVA can be
reduce the inuence of low-frequency noise. In contrast to used to characterize the signal present in data when the
Frank et al. (1998), this work also uses informative priors. null hypothesis has been rejected. The CVA therefore
For example, the response amplitude prior is the sum of shares some features with the descriptive non-inferential
an impulse at zero amplitude and a gamma density, methods. For example, it has been pointed out that CVA
reecting the expectation that most voxels are not acti- informally may be viewed as a PCA that accounts for
vated (zero amplitude) by the stimulus, and that, if acti- error eects (Friston et al. 1996b).
vated, the expected range of positive activation is likely to
be ca. 1^5% (as described by the parameters of the (i) Optimal linear dimensionality reduction, MANCOVA and CVA
gamma density). While both these works allude to the One of the earliest voxel-based multivariate strategies
need to account for autocorrelation in the time-series, for PET data analysis suggested that the problem with the
neither has addressed this in the models presented. high dimensionality of FNI data may be handled by
Finally, it should be pointed out that there are alterna- optimal linear dimensionality reduction (Friston et al.
tives to the Bayesian approach for incorporating prior 1993, 1996b). In this approach, dimensionality reduction
knowledge. For example, pre-processing, various regulari- is achieved by applying a PCA to the adjusted PET data
zation approaches and the use of appropriate subspaces (mean corrected data adjusted for confounding eects,
spanned by sets of basis functions may be viewed as ways e.g. global and block eects) and keeping only the most
of incorporating prior knowledge. For example, a set of important eigenimages (e.g. the PCs with eigenvalues
temporal basis functions can oer some exibility in the greater than unity). This dimensionality reduction
modelling of a (partially) unknown response form (e.g. represents the optimal linear approximation in a mean
the HRF) and at the same time incorporate prior knowl- square sense. Another way of looking at this is to consider
edge of the response form. Fundamentally, the whole the linear transformation as representing that projection
model-building process and the choice of a statistical of data on a subspace of given dimensionality which
model reect prior knowledge. preserves the maximum amount of observed variability.
The outcome of the PCA is sensitive to the pre-proces-
(d) Multivariate statistical models sing performed on the primary data (e.g. image
FNI data are inherently multivariate and multivariate smoothing, tting and adjusting for confounding eects
approaches are natural alternatives for data analysis. or standardization of voxel-per-voxel variance). For
Three dierent multivariate approaches (PCA^ example, unless the components of interest are rst ortho-
MANCOVA^ CVA, PLS, and MLM) will be reviewed in gonalized to components of no interest, part of the
this section. Issues discussed include linear dimensionality components of no interest may be left in the adjusted
reduction, the need to characterize the multivariate data. In addition, the result of the PCA may be sensitive
response, some non-parametric approaches to multi- to outliers in the data, if the particular experimental
variate data, the generalized S-test and dimensionality eects are relatively weak. The dimensionality reducing
estimation. Since it is dicult to clearly separate the PCA also alters the multivariate distribution of data. If
modelling aspects from the inferential aspects in the the observations are temporally uncorrelated, as in PET,
multivariate approaches described, some of the inferential then the inference procedure described by Friston et al.
aspects are naturally commented upon in this section. A (1996b) is still valid. However, if the observations are
more general picture of statistical inference is given in the temporally correlated, this is no longer the case (Worsley
companion paper (Petersson et al., following paper). et al. 1997c).
As already noted, the high dimensionality of FNI data The result of the PCA is a set of pairs, each pair
relative to the number of observations often excludes consisting of a time component (component scores or

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1252 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

temporal pattern) and a space component (eigenimage or models for data. If the models can be organized in a hier-
spatial pattern, cf. 2(a)). Taking the N rst pairs reduces archical tree-structure, the Wilks' statistic may be used
the dimensionality of the data to N. The component for model selection purposes in a way analogous with a
scores represents the temporal expression over observed sequential hierarchical F-test (Holmes 1994). This allows
time points or scans of the corresponding eigenimage. In for a comprehensive model selection procedure, relatively
eect, a transformed data set is created; it has the same independent of any assumptions on the spatial covariance
number of observations as the original data set but now structure of the PETdata (Friston et al. 1996b).
each observation only consists of N elements, instead of
the number of voxels (i.e. an N-dimensional time-series is (ii) PLS approach and bootstrapping
generated). This multivariate data set is then modelled A dierent technique to analyse spatial patterns is the
with a multivariate GLM (MANCOVA). Statistical infer- PLS approach (Jreskog & Wold 1982; Wold 1985),
ence is achieved using the Wilks' -statistic, and a 2- described and applied to PET data by McIntosh et al.
distributional approximation for the log-transformed (1996a). Hypotheses are often tested in the form of
Wilks' statistic (Chateld & Collins 1980). This is valid to specic contrasts related to the experimental design, and
the extent that the residuals of the multivariate GLM are are used in PLS to study multivariate eects induced by
distributed identically and independently multivariate experimental manipulation. Briey, the so-called cross-
normal. The Wilks' test is comparable to an F-test in the correlation matrix between a matrix of contrasts (e.g.
univariate case, that is, the Wilks' statistic is a test of representing dierences between states) and the data
whether the model as a whole explains a signicant part matrix is generated. The PLS approach avoids dimension-
of the observed variability. In other words, the overall ality reduction by directly studying the cross-correlation
signicance of the multivariate GLM, allowing for the matrix. An SVD is then performed on the cross-correlation
covariates of no interest, is assessed. The Wilks' statistic matrix generating singular images, the corresponding
may in this way also be used as a test of specic eects in singular values, and eect proles. Subject scores are
a way analogous to the use of an F-statistic under the generated by the inner product of a given singular image
extra sum of squares principle (Draper & Smith 1981). and the subject's brain images. The subject score reects
When the null hypothesis is rejected this indicates that the degree to which the singular image is expressed in a
there is an experimentally induced signal present in data, given brain image, that is, how parallel the singular and
but without giving a detailed characterization of the the brain image are. The eect proles reect the degree
signal. It is therefore necessary to characterize the dis- a given singular image is related to (or expresses) the
crepancies between the experimental eects and the null contrasts of interest. Similar to the canonical images, the
hypothesis. Friston et al. (1996b) suggest the use of a CVA singular images have to be interpreted post hoc, often
to this aim. The multivariate experimental eect is char- guided by the form of the eect proles or the variation
acterized as so-called canonical variates (orthogonal or of subject scores over states (McIntosh et al. 1996a). As
uncorrelated by construction, Chateld & Collins with PCs or canonical images, a singular image may
(1980)). Since this characterization is performed on trans- represent a mixture of structured eects.
formed data (i.e. transformed into the space spanned by The PLS approach entails statistical test procedures
the N eigenimages) it is necessary to transform the char- that requires careful interpretation. The singular images
acterized eects back into image space, where the eects and their eect proles are assessed in an indirect way
are represented as canonical images (i.e. canonical using a permutation test. Specically, the explanatory
variate!canonical image). The eects described by the signicance represented by the matrix of contrasts in rela-
canonical images need to be interpreted post hoc, tion to the subject scores is assessed. This means that the
commonly guided by the form of the temporal response subject scores are regressed on the matrix of contrasts and
prole of the corresponding canonical variate. Any local a test statistic, R2 (representing the proportion of the
structure in the canonical image has to be interpreted overall observed variance explained by the model) is used
descriptively. As with PCA, there is nothing inherent in to assess the overall signicance of the eects represented
the CVA approach to guarantee a straightforward inter- in the matrix of contrasts. The signicance of the R2-
pretation of the results, unless the signal is naturally statistic is assessed using a permutation test (Edgington
divided into several orthogonal components. In general, it 1995; Good 1994). The R2-test is generally not a test on
is possible that a canonical variate ^ image represents a any specic eect represented by the singular images or
mixture of eects. In principle, characterization of the the subject scores, but tests whether the matrix of
experimentally induced response necessitates an estimate contrasts as a whole explains a signicant part of the
of its dimensionality; that is, the number of canonical observed variability in the subject scores. However, since
variates required to describe the experimental eect. With the scores are derived from the singular image and the
each canonical variate there is associated a canonical contrasts are the same as those used in the original
value that may be interpreted as a variance ratio between analysis, this variant of the PLS approach may in an
the experimental eect as expressed by the corresponding informal sense be viewed as assessing the link between
canonical variate in relation to its expression in the resi- the two elements. The permuting and redoing of the PLS
dual. Friston et al. (1996b) suggest that the canonical for each permutation in eect assesses how many other
value can be interpreted heuristically as an F-value and pairings of contrasts and brain images produces as good a
used as an indication of the more important eects and link as the original pairing, measured as the proportion
their corresponding canonical images. of the overall variance in the subject scores explained by
An interesting application of the PCA^ MANCOVA^ the matrix of contrasts. An alternative to this PLS scheme
CVA approach is the possibility to investigate dierent has been proposed (Grady et al. 1998), in which the

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1253

singular values are sequentially assessed by permutation the omnibus tests section in Petersson et al. (following
tests. It should be noted that permutation tests are valid paper)). The approach described did not attempt to
only if the data full the assumption of exchangeability characterize the experimental response. Recently, this
under the null hypothesis (cf. the non-parametric section approach was generalized to arbitrary GLMs and a
in Petersson et al. (following paper)). strategy for characterizing the departure from the null
As with the PCA^ MANCOVA^ CVA approach, it is hypothesis, that is, the experimentally induced response
dicult to use the signicance assessment as a basis for was described (Worsley et al. 1997c). This general MLM
more detailed empirical conclusions in any direct sense, for approach has similarities with the MANCOVA^CVA
example, interpreting specic eects characterized by the approach. The MLM approach uses a test for distributed
contrasts used in the analysis or in terms of regional speci- change in combination with a PCA of the normalized
city. Instead, the singular images are often arbitrarily thre- eects. This approach can also be used to analyse tempo-
sholded and the local structure in these descriptively rally correlated data and is thus applicable to fMRI
interpreted. It has been suggested that this aspect of the data.
PLS approach can be made more rigorous by generating In brief, given a GLM, the voxel F-statistic image is
condence intervals corresponding to the voxel values of a generated and the F-statistic averaged over all voxels in
given singular image, using non-parametric bootstrap the search volume, representing a generalization of the
estimates (Efron & Tibshirani 1986). Under the assump- S-statistic described by Worsley et al. (1995). In this way
tion of independent and identically distributed observa- the MLM approach avoids the need for explicit dimen-
tions, the bootstrap procedure is asymptotically exact (for sionality reduction. Rather than lter the data with a
a precise denition of exactness, see Petersson et al. pre-whitening lter in combination with maximum-
(following paper)). This implies that for a suciently large likelihood estimation, this approach uses the more robust
sample size the bootstrap estimates can be considered OLS estimation procedure (cf. 3(b)(iv)). The general-
approximations of the exact p-value or condence interval. ized S-statistic is approximately F-distributed with estim-
The PLS approach is, like PCA, sensitive to the type of able eective degrees of freedom (d.f.410 is required for
pre-processing performed on the data. The singular the approximation to be suciently accurate). The S-
images depend on what eects are incorporated in the statistic is used to assess the null hypothesis that no signal
matrix of contrasts and what pre-processing has been explained by the model is present in the search volume. If
performed on data. This implies that the singular images the null hypothesis is rejected, indicating that experimen-
depend on the context in which they are computed. Even tally induced changes are present in the search volume,
if there is a hierarchical or nested relationship between the spatio-temporal response needs to be characterized in
dierent matrices of contrasts (e.g. if other contrasts greater detail. Note that the F-statistic image may
representing subject or repetition eects are included or alternatively be submitted to a univariate approach and
not), there may not always be a simple relation between searched for activations using the unied p-value for
the corresponding sets (or subsets) of singular images F-elds (Worsley et al. 1996).
(McIntosh et al. 1996a). McIntosh et al. (1996a) suggest As indicated above, CVA is a technique for nding
that the results from dierent matrices of contrasts may the linear compounds of multivariate response which
be compared to see if results are stable or not. If the demonstrates the greatest inconsistency between the
result is judged unstable, then it is suggested that these experimentally induced eect and the null hypothesis
eects are adjusted for during pre-processing. (Chateld & Collins 1980). In this context, Worsley et al.
The description above has focused on the use of PLS to (1997c) suggest that a PCA of the normalized eects
study the cross-correlation between a matrix of contrasts may be used to nd the linear combination of predictors
and PET data. The approach has been extended to study that optimally describes the distributed response. Speci-
the cross-correlation between a matrix of behavioural cally, a PCA is performed on the mean sums of squares
covariates and PET data, as well as the cross-correlation and cross-products matrix, and the dimensionality of the
between dierent PET data sets (McIntosh et al. 1998a,b). multivariate response is estimated. The dimensionality is
It should be noted that the PLS approach (as described) estimated using a test similar to the Lawley ^ Hotelling
is generally not invariant under arbitrary linear transfor- trace, sequentially testing the signicance of the PCs.
mations of the matrix of contrasts. For example, if the The sequential testing procedure presupposes that the
matrix of contrasts is scaled dierently the results will be signal is large enough to be detected. If the signal is
dierent. The same may be the case if dierent predictors weak it may remain undetected, or perhaps the signal
in the same predictor space are chosen (Worsley et al. PCs may become contaminated by lower order sample
1997c). As a solution to these problems, Worsley et al. PCs (noise). Simulations may be used to investigate these
(1997c) suggest an orthonormalized PLS approach. (Note issues further (K. Worsley, personal communication).
that PLS with orthonormal matrices of contrasts is a Furthermore, the MLM approach assumes that the
special case of the orthonormalized PLS approach.) Gaussian random eld theory is applicable and that the
Finally, it may also be noted that it is not straightforward spatial autocorrelation function can be accurately
to extend the use of non-parametric tests to temporally approximated by a Gaussian point spread function (cf.
correlated fMRI data (cf. the section on non-parametric the section on random eld theory in Petersson et al.
inference in Petersson et al. (following paper)). (following paper)). The MLM approach is validated on
simulated data, indicating that the method works well
(iii) General MLMs and the generalized S-test when data full the assumptions made. There remains
Worsley et al. (1995) described a test for comparing the question of how robust the method is to departures
distributed non-focal dierences between two states (cf. from these assumptions.

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1254 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

(e) Functional connectivity and network analysis interregional covariances are global eects. Variability in
The statistical models so far described have been used any global signal will introduce correlations that most
to investigate the relationship between the experimental often are of no interest. This reiterates the importance of
paradigm and the changes induced in brain activity. adequate global normalization. It was suggested that the
These approaches study changes in regional activity and problem of global eects may be discounted by using
how these changes covary with specic external experi- partial correlation coecients (Horwitz et al. 1984) or
mental manipulations (i.e. changes over which experi- proportional scaling, both of which yield similar results
mental control is exerted). From the perspectives of (Horwitz & Rapoport 1988). However, it has been indi-
theoretical modelling (Amit 1989; Hertz et al. 1991; cated that accounting for global eects with simple
Rumelhart & McClelland 1986), cognitive psychology approaches such as ANCOVA, proportional scaling or
(Horgan & Tienson 1996; Macdonald & Macdonald the use of partial correlation coecients may not always
1995), and cognitive neuroscience (Mesulam 1990, 1998), yield appropriate results. Ford (1986) pointed out that
as well as from lesion observations (Squire 1992; Zola- these approaches could yield highly biased results intro-
Morgan & Squire 1993) and FNI data (Friston 1994; ducing spurious correlations. In this context, it was
Friston et al. 1993; Gonzalez-Lima & McIntosh 1994; argued that the spurious correlations most often are small
Horwitz et al. 1984), it has been suggested that higher and their presence can (to some extent) be tested for
cognitive functions are the result of the network interac- (Horwitz & Rapoport 1988). However, the presence of
tions between dierent brain regions. This indicates that spurious correlations will bias the results unless properly
the understanding of dierent brain functions may benet removed or accounted for.
from analysing the interactions between brain regions. If the covariances are estimated over subjects, it is
Based on the idea that brain regions that constitute necessary to assume that the subjects implement a su-
components of a functional network will have activities ciently similar functional organization. In this case,
that are correlated this is often done by studying the speaking informally, the covariance structure may reect
covariance pattern observed in FNI data. The previously an average common functional organization. However,
described models and approaches, which are used to the functional organization can vary substantially
analyse the relative changes in regional activity, can be between subjects, that is, the covariance structure of a
used to identify the components of such large-scale subject may or may not be related to a common func-
cognitive networks. tional organization (Friston 1995). In PET studies the
Functional and eective connectivity was originally number of intra-subject observations is limited (restricted
dened in the context of electrophysiology (Aertsen et al. by radiation exposure), so, in order to increase sensitivity
1989; Aertsen & Preissl 1991) and these concepts were data is often pooled over subjects. With fMRI, it is
introduced into FNI with a modied connotation by possible to study functional and eective connectivity in
Friston (1994). Functional connectivity was dened as the single subjects (Buechel & Friston 1997). One advantage
observed correlations over time between dierent brain of performing several single subject studies is that this can
areas, independent of the sources of these correlations, give an indication of the generalizability of the results.
and eective connectivity refers explicitly to the inuence Within- and between-subjects studies are complementary,
that one neural system exerts over another (Friston 1994). and both group and single-subject analyses may be
In this section, we will discuss some issues related to confounded by the factors described above.
covariance sources, dierent covariance approaches
(partial correlation coecients and covariance elds) and (ii) Partial correlations and covariance elds
network analysis of eective connectivity (structural Several dierent approaches have been proposed to
equations modelling). study interregional covariances. Early attempts studied the
matrix of partial correlation coecients between pairs of
(i) Covariance sources pre-selected ROIs (Horwitz et al. 1984). Subsequent
Two dierent ways to estimate the covariances within a approaches concentrated on selecting a reference region
cognitive state have been described: over time within (Horwitz et al. 1992a) or a reference voxel (Horwitz et al.
subject (Buechel & Friston 1997), and over subjects 1995), and then studying the correlations with the rest of
(Horwitz et al. 1995). The basic hypothesis is that the the brain or a set of pre-selected regions. In both cases, the
intrinsic variability in the neural response of a cognitive multiple comparisons problem needs to be addressed
state will emulate the relevant functional interactions and (cf. the multiple comparisons section in Petersson et al.
that these interactions will be reected in the covariance (following paper)). For the reference region or voxel
structure. It should also be noted that the sources of approach known results for t-elds may be applied
within-state interregional covariances are beyond experi- (Worsley 1994; Worsley et al. 1996, 1998). In the case of
mental control. voxel-by-voxel correlations, a recently developed theory
Several sources of interregional covariances have been for so-called autocorrelation elds may be used to handle
proposed (Horwitz et al. 1992b) and the actual sources of the multiple comparisons problem (Cao & Worsley 1999;
the observed covariances are largely unknown. However, Worsley et al. 1998). These new theoretical advances in
several of the proposed sources of observed covariances random eld theory also include cross-correlation and
may give rise to spurious correlations that are necessarily homologous correlation elds (Cao & Worsley 1999).
confounded with correlations arising because of eective
connectivity (e.g. adaptation, fatigue, attentional drift, (iii) Structural equation modelling
anomalous task response, however, cf. Horwitz et al. To characterize eective connectivity in FNI data a
(1992b)). Obvious potential confounders in the study of network approach based on structural equation modelling

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1255

(SEM) (Bollen 1989; Hayduk 1987) was suggested by modication indices or using a hierarchical model-
McIntosh & Gonzalez-Lima (1994). SEM provides the building approach when subsets of weights are estimated
opportunity to investigate functional ^anatomical models recursively. The data-driven approach is vulnerable to
subserving dierent cognitive functions in terms of which over-tting, since sample specic characteristics may be
regions are involved and how they interact in a given modelled, which may limit the generalizability of the
network model. SEM commonly assumes multivariate results. For example, outliers or noise may be modelled
normally distributed data. In order to characterize a increasing the risk that the model becomes over-tted.
functional network a specic functional^anatomical Alternatively, model selection may be theory driven,
model is used in conjunction with SEM to model the running the risk of investigating incomplete models where
observed covariance structure between the regions there may be regions or links missing in the model. These
included in the model. The functional ^anatomical model aspects illustrate the model selection problem.
is specied by selecting the network components (ROIs or The results of a stacked models comparison can be
voxels) and the connections between the components dicult to interpret, unless reasonable goodness-of-t can
based on theoretical or empirical considerations. Dierent be achieved with a given model in all states or groups
constraints on the connections may also be specied (cf. investigated. For example, using an under-parameterized
McIntosh & Gonzalez-Lima 1994). The interregional model to test dierences between states or groups in a
covariances are computed and nally the connection stacked approach may yield results due to an ill-tting
strengths or path coecients are estimated within condi- model (in one of the states or groups). The eect of using
tion. Dierences between conditions or groups can be under-parameterized models (i.e. omission of one
estimated using a stacked models approach (Bollen 1989; network component, connection, or feedback loop) has
Hayduk 1987; McIntosh & Gonzalez-Lima 1994). been investigated in a relatively simple model (McIntosh
SEM commonly uses a linear system of equations to & Gonzalez-Lima 1994). This simulation study indicates
describe the interrelation between regional activities in that the results from analysing moderately reduced
the functional ^anatomical model with the connection models can be fairly stable and the modication indices
coecients as free parameters. Nonlinear extensions have can to some extent provide indications of such omissions.
been described (Kenny & Judd 1984) and applied to These and other issues may be of interest for further
fMRI data (Buechel & Friston 1997). The connection investigation in more complex models, for example, the
strengths are estimated in an optimization process. This implications of introducing dierent constraints on the
procedure recreates the observed covariance between weights, the residuals, as well as not taking nonlinear
regions as closely as possible by nding optimal values of eects into account. More severely under-parameterized
the path coecients. There are several optimization algo- models may also be studied. The aspects so far
rithms available. Commonly, the optimization process investigated relate to model selection. The inferential
uses estimated starting values in combination with an consequences of ill-tting models are also relevant to
iterative maximum-likelihood estimation procedure. For investigate, when a stacked models comparison is
example, the standard implementation in the LISREL attempted.
program (Jreskog & Srbom 1996; see also Boomsma Alternative approaches to eective connectivity have
1985) uses instrumental variables and a two-stage least been proposed. For example, McIntosh & Gonzalez-
square approach in combination with the Davidon ^ Lima (1994) describe a simple model for studying the
Fletcher ^ Power algorithm and line search (other alter- eects of experimental manipulation on both regional
natives are available, cf. Jreskog & Srbom (1996)). With activity and interregional covariance. This approach has
reasonably well-tting models, the initial estimates are since become known as psychophysiological interactions
often close enough to the nal maximum-likelihood esti- (Friston et al. 1997). Other approaches suggest the use of
mate for the optimization algorithms to quickly converge nonlinear techniques (Friston & Buechel 1998; see also
to this estimate. It should be noted that when the esti- Friston et al. 1998) in the form of truncated Volterra series
mates depend nonlinearly on the model parameters there expansion (Priestley 1988), or variable parameter regres-
is no guarantee that the global optimum will be reached sion (Buechel & Friston 1998; cf. Chateld 1996) in
with deterministic gradient descent algorithms or non- conjunction with Kalman ltering (Chateld 1996; Wei
exhaustive search procedures. Alternatively, a simulated 1990). Note that when these network approaches or SEM
annealing approach to optimization can be used (Geman are applied to fMRI data it is necessary to take the
& Geman 1984; Kirkpatrick et al. 1983) even though temporal autocorrelation into account. Finally, it has
practical annealing schedules generally only generate been suggested that the interface between FNI, network
good sub-optimal solutions. analysis and large-scale neural modelling may add to our
The results of SEM analysis are potentially dicult to understanding of human cognition (Friston 1998a;
interpret for several reasons. There is no guarantee that the Horwitz 1998; Horwitz et al. 1999).
connections modelled actually reect direct eective
connections it is possible that they are mediated through
4. CONCLUSION
areas or connections not included in the model. Similarly,
observed changes in the weights between states or groups FNI methods provide experimental access to the living
may reect common input from regions not modelled. This human brain and have been rapidly developing during the
touches on the general problem of model selection, that is, last two decades. A framework of well-described theories
the problem of matching model and data complexity. In and empirically validated methods are available providing
the case of SEM, model selection may be performed in a a background for the development of new analytical tools.
data-driven mode, guided by goodness-of-t values, The FNI methods used dier in assumptions and these

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1256 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

need to be examined in order to indicate the boundaries K.M.P. was supported by grants from the Swedish Medical
of optimal use. Central to this is, on the one hand, how Research Council (8276) and the Karolinska Institute. A.P.H
well empirical data full the assumptions and the approx- was supported by the Wellcome Trust for part of this work. The
authors are grateful for many fruitful discussions concerning
imations made, and on the other, the robustness of the methodological issues with Peter Fransson, Karl J. Friston,
methods used. This notion emphasizes the importance of Anthony R. McIntosh, John Ollinger and Keith Worsley.
empirical validation, investigation of robustness, and the
explicit characterization of the inherent limitations of a
given method. In this paper we have focused on assump- REFERENCES
tions and inherent limitations of the methods reviewed. Aertsen, A. M. H. & Preissl, H. 1991 Dynamics of activity and
This indicates the limits of applicability and denes connectivity in physiological neuronal networks. In Nonlinear
constraints on the interpretations of results obtained. dynamics and neuronal networks (ed. H. G. Schuster), pp. 281^302.
When these assumptions and limitations are taken into NewYork:VHC Publishers Inc.
account, the dierent methods and approaches described Aertsen, A. M. H., Gerstein, G. L., Habib, M. K. & Palm, G.
generally serve their purposes well. 1989 Dynamics of neuronal ring correlation: modulation of
`eective connectivity'. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 900^917.
We have discussed some aspects of the complex
Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E. & D'Esposito, M. 1997 Empirical
problem of model selection. Model identication is of analyses of BOLD fMRI statistics. II. Spatially smoothed data
particular importance for future developments in the collected under null hypothesis and experimental conditions.
analysis of fMRI data. In general, proper model selection NeuroImage 5, 199^212.
is a necessary prerequisite for the validity of the subse- Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E. & D'Esposito, M. 1998a A critique
quent statistical inference, which depends on the use of of the use of the Kolmogorov ^Smirnov (KS) statistic for the
suciently well-tting models. Assessing model t and analysis of BOLD fMRI data. Magn. Reson. Med. 39, 500^505.
verication of assumptions are challenging tasks and Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E. & D'Esposito, M. 1998b The inferen-
eective tools for assessing the goodness-of-t of models tial impact of global signal covariates in functional
and diagnostics for violations of assumptions are gener- neuroimaging analyses. NeuroImage 8, 302^306.
ally lacking in FNI. However, there are several non- Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E. & D'Esposito, M. 1998c The varia-
bility of human, BOLD hemodynamic responses. NeuroImage
inferential descriptive methods that, combined with
8, 360^369.
inspection of parameter estimates and other simple Amit, D. J. 1989 Modeling brain function: the world of attractor neural
measures, can help in the process of model selection, networks. Cambridge University Press.
outlier detection, and verication of assumptions. In addi- Andersson, J. L. R. 1997 How to estimate global activity inde-
tion, multivariate methods can be used to perform model pendent of changes in local activity. NeuroImage 6, 237^244.
selection in a comprehensive way. Andreasen, N. C., Arndt, S. A., Cizadlo, T., O'Leary, D. S.,
It is of importance for the interpretation of FNI results Watkins, G. L., Boles Ponto, L. L. & Hichawa, R. D. 1995
to take into account the assumptions, approximations and Sample size and statistical power in [O15]H2O studies of
inherent limitations of the methods used. There are human cognition. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 16, 804^816.
several areas in need of attention. One is the characteriza- Arbib, M. A. 1995 The handbook of brain theory and neural networks.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
tion of the baseline and its uctuations relative to which
Arndt, S. A., Cizadlo, T., Andreasen, N. C., Zeien, G., Harris,
regional activations are measured and eective methods G., O'Leary, D. S., Watkins, G. L., Boles Ponto, L. L. &
for estimating global eects independent of experimen- Hichawa, R. D. 1995 A comparison of approaches to the
tally induced changes. In addition, comprehensive models statistical analysis of [O15]H2O PET cognitive activation
of gA are lacking, which are necessary when large or studies. J. Neuropsychiat. Clin. Neurosci. 7, 155^168.
several dierent ranges of activity are expected. Another Arndt, S. A., Cizadlo, T., O'Leary, D. S., Gold, S. &
issue is the need for HRF models that eectively incorpo- Andreasen, N. C. 1996 Normalizing counts and cerebral
rate potential regional specicity of the haemodynamic blood ow intensity in functional imaging studies of the
response as well as its variability over subjects and time. human brain. NeuroImage 3, 175^184.
There may also be a need for more comprehensive statis- Ash, R. B. 1965 Information theory. New York: Dover Publications.
tical modelling allowing for regionally specic models Bandettini, P. A. & Wong, E. C. 1997 A hypercapnia-based
normalization method for improved spatial localization of
moving beyond the image regression approach. We have
human brain activation with fMRI. NMR Biomed. 10,197^203.
also indicated the possibility of using a general Bayesian Bandettini, P. A., Wong, E. C., Hinks, R. S., Tikofsky, R. S. &
framework. This allows for the systematic incorporation of Hyde, J. S. 1993 Time course EPI of human brain function
prior knowledge, informed modelling, and more exible during task activation. Magn. Reson. Med. 25, 390^397.
models than permitted by regression and a richer variety Bell, A. J. & Sejnowski, T. J. 1995 An information-maximization
of questions can potentially be answered. Last, it is impor- approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural
tant to acknowledge the importance of random eects Comput. 7, 1129^1159.
models when the scope of inference is to the whole popula- Bickel, P. J. & Docksum, K. A. 1977 Mathematical statistics: basic
tion sampled. Group comparisons are not served well by ideas and selected topics. Oakland, CA: Holden-Day.
xed eects models when population inference is Billingsley, P. 1995 Probability and measure, 3rd edn. New York:
Wiley.
attempted, instead random eects models are necessary.
Bollen, K. A. 1989 Structural equations with latent variables. New
Finally, one of the great challenges to the eld of FNI is York: Wiley.
the development of eective methods to study higher Boomsma, A. 1985 Nonconvergence, improper solutions, and
cognitive functions, subserved by nonlinear network inter- starting values in LISREL maximum likelihood estimation.
actions and non-stationary dynamics, in greater detail, Psychometry 50, 229^242.
using the full potential of the methods available in terms Box, G. E. P. & Tiao, G. C. 1992 Bayesian inference in statistical
of spatio-temporal resolution. analysis. New York: Wiley.

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1257

Boynton, G. M., Engel, S. A., Glover, G. H. & Heeger, D. J. Draper, N. R. & Smith, H. 1981 Applied regression analysis, 2nd
1996 Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic reso- edn. New York: Wiley.
nance imaging in human V1. J. Neurosci. 16, 4207^4221. Edgington, E. S. 1995 Randomization tests, 3rd edn (revised and
Bretthorst, G. L. 1990a Bayesian analysis. I. Parameter estimation expanded). New York: Marcel Dekker.
using quadrature NMR models. J. Magn. Reson. 88, 533^551. Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R. 1986 Bootstrap methods for standard
Bretthorst, G. L. 1990b Bayesian analysis. II. Signal detection errors, condence intervals and other measures of statistical
and model selection. J. Magn. Reson. 88, 552^570. accuracy. Statist. Sci. 1, 54^77.
Bruhn, H., Kleinschmidt, A., Boecker, H., Merboldt, K.-D., Fitzgerald, M. 1996 Registration and estimation of functional
Hnicke, W. & Frahm, J. 1994 The eects of acetazolamide on magnetic resonance images. PhD thesis, University of
regional cerebral blood oxygenation at rest and under stimulation Carnegie Mellon, PA, USA.
as assessed by MRI. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 14,742^748. Ford, I. 1986 Confounded correlations: statistical limitations in
Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Dale, A. M., the analysis of interregional relationships of cerebral meta-
Rotte, M. & Rosen, B. R. 1998a Functional ^anatomic study bolic activity. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 6, 385^388.
of episodic retrieval. II. Selective averaging of event-related Ford, I. 1995 Commentary and opinion. III. Some nononto-
fMRI trials to test the retrieval success hypothesis. NeuroImage logical and functionally unconnected views on current issues
7, 163^175. in the analysis of PET datasets. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 15,
Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Wagner, A. D. & 371^377.
Rosen, B. R. 1998b Functional^anatomic study of episodic Ford, I., McColl, J. H., McCormack, A. G. & McCroy, S. J.
retrieval using fMRI. I. Retrieval eort versus retrieval 1991 Statistical issues in the analysis of neuroimages. J. Cerebr.
success. NeuroImage 7, 151^162. Blood-Flow Metab. 11, A89^A95.
Buechel, C. & Friston, K. J. 1997 Modulation of connectivity in Fox, P. T. & Mintun, M. A. 1989 Non-invasive functional brain
visual pathways by attention: cortical interactions evaluated mapping by change distribution analysis of averaged PET
with structural equation modelling and fMRI. Cerebr. Cortex 7, images of H215O tissue activity. J. Nucl. Med. 30, 141^149.
768^778. Fox, P. T. & Raichle, M. E. 1984 Stimulus rate dependence of
Buechel, C. & Friston, K. J. 1998 Dynamic changes in eective regional cerebral blood ow in human striate cortex demon-
connectivity characterized by variable parameter regression strated with positron emission tomography. J. Neurophysiol. 51,
and Kalman ltering. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6, 403^408. 1109^1121.
Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C. R., Rabe-Hesketh, Fox, P. T., Mintun, M. A., Reiman, E. M. & Raichle, M. E.
S., Janot, N., David, A., Mellers, J., Howard, R. & Sham, P. 1988 Enhanced detection of focal brain responses using inter-
1996 Statistical methods of estimation and inference for func- subject averaging and change-distribution analysis of sub-
tional MR image analysis. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 261^277. tracted PET images. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 8, 642^653.
Buxton, R. B. & Frank, L. R. 1997 A model for the coupling Frackowiak, R. S. J., Zeki, S., Poline, J.-B. & Friston, K. J. 1996
between cerebral blood ow and oxygen metabolism during A critique of a new analysis proposed for functional neuro-
neural stimulation. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 17, 64^72. imaging. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8, 2229^2231.
Buxton, R. B., Wong, E. C. & Frank, L. R. 1998 Dynamics of Frackowiak, R. S. J., Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J. &
blood ow and oxygenation changes during brain activation: Mazziotta, J. C. 1997 Human brain function. San Diego, CA:
the balloon model. Magn. Reson. Med. 39, 855^864. Academic Press.
Cao, J. & Worsley, K. J. 1999 The geometry of correlation elds Frank, L. R., Buxton, R. B. & Wong, E. C. 1998 Probabilistic
with an application to functional connectivity of the brain. analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data.
Ann. Appl. Prob. (In the press.) Magn. Reson. Med. 39, 132^148.
Chateld, C. 1996 The analysis of time series: an introduction. Friston, K. J. 1993 Letter to the editor: author's reply. J. Cerebr.
London: Chapman & Hall. Blood-Flow Metab. 13, 1038^1040.
Chateld, C. & Collins, A. J. 1980 Introduction to multivariate Friston, K. 1994 Functional and eective connectivity: a synth-
analysis. London: Chapman & Hall. esis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 56^78.
Clark, C. & Carson, R. 1993 Letter to the editor: analysis of Friston, K. J. 1995 Commentary and opinion. II. Statistical
covariance in statistical parametric mapping. J. Cerebr. Blood- parametric mapping ontology and current issues. J. Cerebr.
Flow Metab. 13, 1038. Blood-Flow Metab. 15, 361^370.
Cohen, M. S. 1997 Parametric analysis of fMRI data using Friston, K. J. 1998a Imaging neuroscience: principles or maps?
linear systems methods. NeuroImage 6, 93^103. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 796^802.
Common, P. 1994 Independent component analysis: a new Friston, K. J. 1998b Modes or models: a critique of independent
concept ? Signal Processing 36, 11^20. component analysis for fMRI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 373^375.
Coreld, D. R., Murphy, K., Josephs, O., Adams, L. & Friston, K. J. & Buechel, C. 1998 A nonlinear analysis of func-
Turner, R. 1998 Modulation by hypercapnia of activation- tional brain architectures. NeuroImage 7, S776.
related BOLD signal changes in the visual cortex. NeuroImage Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Liddle, P. F., Dolan, R. J.,
7, S259. Lammertsma, A. A. & Frackowiak, R. S. 1990 The relation-
Cover, T. M. & Thomas, J. A. (eds) 1991 Elements of information ship between global and local changes in PET scans. J. Cerebr.
theory. New York: Wiley. Blood-Flow Metab. 10, 458^466.
Dale, A. M. & Buckner, R. L. 1997 Selective averaging of Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Liddle, P. F. & Frackowiak, R. S. J.
rapidly presented individual trials using fMRI. Hum. Brain 1991 Comparing functional (PET) images: the assessment of
Mapp. 5, 329^340. signicant change. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 11, 690^699.
Davis, T. L., Weissko, R. M., Kwong, K. K., Boxerman, J. L. Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Liddle, P. F. & Frackowiak, R. S.
& Rosen, B. R. 1994 Temporal aspects of fMRI task activa- 1993 Functional connectivity: the principal-component
tion: dynamic modeling of oxygen delivery. Society for analysis of large (PET) data sets. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab.
Magnetic Resonance, 2nd Annual Meeting, August 6^12 13, 4^14.
1994, San Francisco, CA, USA, p. 69. Friston, K. J., Jezzard, P. & Turner, R. 1994a Analysis of func-
Descombes, X., Kruggel, F. & von Cramon, D. Y. 1998 fMRI tional MRI time-series. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1, 210^220.
signal restoration using a spatiotemporal Markov random Friston, K. J., Worsely, K. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Mazziotta,
eld preserving transitions. NeuroImage 8, 340^349. J. C. & Evans, A. C. 1994b Assessing the signicance of focal

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1258 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

activations using their spatial extent. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1, Holmes, A. P., Josephs, O., Buchel, C. & Friston, K. J. 1997
214^220. Statistical modelling of low-frequency confounds in fMRI.
Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Poline, J. B., Grasby, P. J., NeuroImage 5, S480.
Williams, S. C. R., Frackowiak, R. S. J. & Turner, R. 1995 Holmes, A. P., Watson, J. D. G. & Nichols, T. E. 1998 Holmes
Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited. NeuroImage 2, and Watson on `Sherlock'. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 18, 697.
45^53. Horgan, T. & Tienson, J. 1996 Connectionism and the philosophy of
Friston, K. J., Holmes, A., Poline, J.-B., Price, C. J. & Frith, C. D. psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
1996a Detecting activations in PET and fMRI: levels of infer- Horwitz, B. 1998 Using functional brain imaging to understand
ence and power. NeuroImage 4, 223^235. human cognition. Complexity 3, 39^52.
Friston, K. J., Poline, J.-P., Holmes, A. P., Frith, C. D. & Horwitz, B. & Rapoport, S. I. 1988 Partial correlation coe-
Frackowiak, R. S. J. 1996b A multivariate analysis of PET cients approximate the real intrasubject correlation pattern in
activation studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 4, 140^151. the analysis of interregional relations of cerebral metabolic
Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., Rolls, E. & activity. J. Nucl. Med. 29, 392^399.
Dolan, R. J. 1997 Psychophysiological and modulatory inter- Horwitz, B., Duara, R. & Rapoport, S. I. 1984 Intercorrelations
actions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage 6, 218^229. of glucose metabolic rates between brain regions: application
Friston, K. J., Josephs, O., Rees, G. & Turner, R. 1998 to healthy males in a state of reduced sensory input. J. Cerebr.
Nonlinear event-related responses in fMRI. Magn. Reson. Med. Blood-Flow Metab. 4, 484^499.
39, 41^52. Horwitz, B., Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G.,
Geman, S. & Geman, D. 1984 Stochastic relaxation, Gibbs Schapiro, M. B., Mishkin, M. & Rapoport, S. I. 1992a
distribution, and Bayesian restoration of images. IEEE Trans. Functional associations among human posterior extrastriate
Patt. Anal. Mach. Intell. 6, 721^741. brain regions during object and spatial vision. J. Cogn.
Genovese, C. R. 1997 Statistical inference in functional magnetic reso- Neurosci. 4, 311^322.
nance imaging, technical report 674. Pittsburgh: Department of Horwitz, B., Soncrant, J. V. & Haxby, J. V. 1992b Covariance
Statistics, Carnegie Mellon University. analysis of functional interactions in the brain using metabolic
Genovese, C. R. & Sweeney, J. S. 1998 Functional connectivity and blood ow data. In Advances in metabolic mapping techniques
in the cortical regions subserving eye movements (with for brain imaging of behavioral and learning functions (ed. F.
discussion). In Case studies in Bayesian statistics (ed. R. B. Kass, Gonzalez-Lima, T. Finkenstaedt & H. Scheich), pp. 189^217.
B. P. Carlin, A. L. Carriquiry, C. Catsonis, A. Gelman, Dordrecht: Kluwer.
I. Verdinelli & M. West), pp. 59^132. New York: Springer. Horwitz, B., McIntosh, A. R., Haxby, J. V. & Grady, C. L. 1995
Gonzalez-Lima, F. & McIntosh, A. R. 1994 Neural network Network analysis of brain cognitive function using metabolic
interactions related to auditory learning analyzed with struc- and blood ow data. Behav. Brain Res. 66, 187^193.
tural equation modeling. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 23^44. Horwitz, B., Tagamets, M.-A. & McIntosh, A. R. 1999 Neural
Good, P. 1994 Permutation tests. New York: Springer. modeling, functional brain imaging, and cognition. Trends
Grabowski, T. J., Frank, R. J., Brown, C. K., Damasio, H., Cogn. Sci. 3, 91^98.
Boles Ponto, L. L., Watkins, G. L. & Hichwa, R. D. 1996 Hu, X., Le, T. H., Parrish, T. & Erhard, P. 1995 Retrospective
Reliability of PETactivation across statistical methods, subject estimation and correction of physiological uctuation in func-
groups and sample sizes. Hum. Brain Mapp. 4, 23^46. tional MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 201^212.
Grady, C. L., McIntosh, A. R., Rajah, M. N. & Craik, F. I. M. Hu, X., Le, T. H. & Ugurbil, K. 1997 Evaluation of the early
1998 Neural correlates of the episodic encoding of pictures response in fMRI in individual subjects using short stimulus
and words. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 2703^2708. duration. Magn. Reson. Med. 37, 877^884.
Halber, M., Herholz, K., Wienhard, K., Pawlik, G. & Heiss, Huber, P. J. 1985 Projection pursuit. Ann. Statist. 13, 435^475.
W.-D. 1997 Performance of a randomization test for single- Jain, A. K. 1989 Fundamentals of digital image processing.
subject O15 -water PET activation studies. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Englewood Clis, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Metab. 17, 1033^1039. Jaynes, E. T. 1968 Prior probabilities. IEEE Trans. Syst. Sci.
Hayduk, L. A. 1987 Structural equation modeling with LISREL: Cybernetics SSC 4, 227^241.
essentials and advances. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Jollie, I. T. 1986 Principal component analysis. New York: Springer.
Press. Jreskog, K. & Srbom, D. 1996 LISREL 8 user's reference guide.
Herholz, K., Kessler, J., Slansky, I., Mielke, R. & Heiss, W. D. Chicago, IL: Scientic Software International.
1993 A model for separation of regional from global meta- Jreskog, K. G. & Wold, H. 1982 The ML and PLS techniques
bolic activation during continuous visual recognition in for modeling with latent variables: historical and comparative
Alzheimer's disease. In Quantication of brain function: tracer aspects. In Systems under indirect observation: causality, structure,
kinetics and image analysis in brain PET (ed. K. Uemura, N. A. prediction (ed. H. Wold & K. G. Jreskog), pp. 263^270.
Lassen, T. Jones & I. Kanno), pp. 555^560. Amsterdam: Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Excerpta Medica. Kanno, I., Hatazawa, J., Shimosegawa, E., Ishii, K. & Fujita,
Hertz, J., Krogh, A. & Palmer, R. G. 1991 Introduction to the H. 1996 Proportionality of reaction CBF to baseline CBF
theory of neural computation. San Diego, CA: Addison-Wesley. with neural activation and deactivation. In Quantication of
Holmes, A. P. 1994 Statistical issues in functional brain brain function using PET (ed. R. Myers, V. Cunningham, D.
mapping. PhD thesis, University of Glasgow. Bailey & T. Jones), pp. 362^362. San Diego, CA: Academic
Holmes, A. P. & Ford, I. 1993 A Bayesian approach to signi- Press.
cance testing for statistic images from PET. In Quantication of Kenny, D. A. & Judd, C. M. 1984 Estimating the nonlinear
brain function: tracer kinetics and image analysis in brain PET (ed. and interactive eects of latent variables. Psychol. Bull. 96,
K. Uemura, N. Lassen, T. Jones & I. Kanno), pp. 521^531. 201^210.
Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica. Kirkpatrick, S., Gelatt, C. D. & Vecchi, M. P. 1983
Holmes, A. P. & Friston, K. J. 1998 Generalizability, random Optimization by simulated annealing. Science 220, 671^680.
eects and population inference. NeuroImage 7, S754. Koch, C. & Davis, J. L. 1994 Large-scale neuronal theories of the
Holmes, A. P., Blair, R. C., Watson, J. D. G. & Ford, I. 1996 brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Non-parametric analysis of statistic images from functional Laird, N. M. & Ware, J. H. 1982 Random-eects models for
mapping experiments. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 16, 7^22. longitudinal data. Biometrics 38, 963^974.

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

Non-inferential methods and statistical models K. M. Petersson and others 1259

Lange, N. 1997 Empirical and substantive models, the Bayesian Oppenheim, A. V. & Schafer, R. W. 1989 Discrete-time signal
paradigm, and meta-analysis in functional brain imaging. processing. Englewood Clis, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 259^263. Paradis, A.-L., Van de Morrtele, P.-F., Le Bihan, D. & Poline,
Lange, N. & Zeger, S. L. 1997 Non-linear Fourier time- J.-B. 1998 Do high temporal frequencies of the event-related
series analysis for human brain mapping by functional fMRI response have a more specic spatial localization?
magnetic resonance imaging. J. R. Statist. Soc. Appl. Statist. NeuroImage 7, S617.
46, 1^29. Petersson, K. M. 1998 Comments on a Monte Carlo approach
Lee, P. M. 1997 Bayesian statistics: an introduction, 2nd edn. New to the analysis of functional neuroimaging data. NeuroImage 8,
York: Wiley. 108^112.
Locascio, J. J., Jennings, P. J., Moore, C. I. & Corkin, S. 1997 Poline, J.-B., Vandenberghe, R., Holmes, A. P., Friston, K. J. &
Time series analysis in the time domain and resampling Frackowiak, R. S. J. 1996 Reproducibility of PET activation
methods for studies of functional magnetic resonance brain studies: lessons from a multi-center European experiment.
imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 168^193. NeuroImage 4, 34^54.
McColl, J. H., Holmes, A. P. & Ford, I. 1994 Statistical Priestley, M. B. 1988 Non-linear and non-stationary time series
methods in neuroimaging with particular application to emis- analysis. New York: Academic Press.
sion tomography. Statist. Meth. Med. Res. 3, 63^86. Purdon, P. L. & Weissko, R. M. 1998 Eect of temporal auto-
McCrory, S. J. & Ford, I. 1991 Multivariate analysis of SPECT correlation due to physiological noise stimulus paradigm on
images with illustrations in Alzheimer's disease. Statist. Med. voxel-level false-positive rates in fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6,
10, 1711^1718. 239^249.
McCullagh, P. & Nelder, J. A. 1989 Generalized linear models, 2nd Purdon, P. L., Solo, V., Brown, E., Buckner, R., Rotte, M. &
edn. London: Chapman & Hall. Weissko, R. M. 1998 fMRI noise variability across subjects
Macdonald, C. & Macdonald, G. (eds) 1995 Connectionism: and trials: insights for noise estimation methods. NeuroImage 7,
debates on psychological explanation. Oxford: Blackwell. S617.
McIntosh, A. R. & Gonzalez-Lima, F. 1994 Structural equation Rajapakse, J. C., Kruggel, F., Maisog, J. M. & von Cramon,
modeling and its application to network analysis in functional D. Y. 1998 Modeling hemodynamic response for analysis
brain imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 2^22. of functional MRI time-series. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6,
McIntosh, A. R., Bookstein, F. L., Haxby, J. V. & Grady, C. L. 283^300.
1996a Spatial pattern analysis of functional brain images Ramsay, S. C., Murphy, K., Shea, S. A., Friston, K. J.,
using partial least squares. NeuroImage 3, 143^157. Lammertsma, A. A., Clark, J. C., Adams, L., Guz, A. &
McIntosh, A. R., Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Maisog, J. M., Frackowiak, R. S. 1993 Changes in global cerebral blood ow
Horwitz, B. & Clark, C. M. 1996b Within-subject transforma- in humans: eect on regional cerebral blood ow during a
tion of PET regional cerebral blood ow data: ANCOVA, neural activation task. J. Physiol. 471, 521^534.
ratio, and Z-score adjustment on empirical data. Hum. Brain Roland, P. E. & Gulyas, B. 1996 Assumptions and validations of
Mapp. 4, 93^102. statistical tests for functional neuroimaging. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8,
McIntosh, A. R., Cabeza, R. & Lobaugh, N. J. 1998a 2232^2235.
Explaining the activation of occipital cortex by an auditory Rosen, B. R., Buckner, R. L. & Dale, A. M. 1998 Event-related
stimulus through analysis of neural interactions. J. functional MRI: past, present, and future. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci.
Neurophysiol. 80, 2790^2796. USA 95, 773^780.
McIntosh, A. R., Lobaugh, N. J., Cabeza, R., Bookstein, F. L. Rosenfeld, A. & Kak, A. C. (eds) 1982 Digital picture processing.
& Houle, S. 1998b Convergence of neural systems processing Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
stimulus associations and coordinating motor responses. Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. 1986 Parallel distributed
Cerebr. Cortex 8, 648^659. processing: explorations in the microstructures of cognition, vols 1 and
McKeown, M. J., Makeig, S., Brown, G. B., Jung, T.-P., 2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kindermann, S. S., Bell, A. J. & Sejnowski, T. J. 1998 Ruttimann, U. E., Rio, D., Rawlings, R. R., Andreasen, P. &
Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent Hommer, D. M. 1998 PET analysis using a variance stabi-
spatial components. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6, 160^188. lizing transform. In Quantitative functional brain imaging with
Mesulam, M. M. 1990 Large-scale neurocognitive networks and positron emission tomography (ed. R. E. Carson, M. E. Daube-
distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Witherspoon & P. Herscovitch), pp. 217^222. San Diego, CA:
Ann. Neurol. 28, 597^613. Academic Press.
Mesulam, M. M. 1998 From sensation to cognition. Brain 121, Schuster, H. G. 1991 Nonlinear dynamics and neuronal networks. New
1013^1052. York: VHC Publishers Inc.
Moeller, J. R. & Strother, S. C. 1991 A regional covariance Senda, M., Ishii, K., Oda, K., Sadato, N., Kawashima, R.,
approach to the analysis of functional patterns in positron Sugiura, M., Kanno, I., Ardekani, B., Minoshima, S. &
emission tomographic data. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 11, Tatsumi, I. 1998 Inuence of ANOVA design and anatomical
A121^A135. standardization on statistical mapping for PET activation.
Moeller, J. R., Strother, S. C., Sidtis, J. J. & Rottenberg, D. A. NeuroImage 8, 283^301.
1987 Scaled subprole model: a statistical approach to the Snedecor, G. W. & Cochran, W. G. 1967 Statistical methods, 6th
analysis of functional patterns in positron emission tomo- edn. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press.
graphic data. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 7, 649^658. Squire, L. R. 1992 Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis
Ogawa, S., Menon, R. S., Kim, S.-G. & Ugurbil, K. 1998 On from ndings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol. Rev.
the characteristics of functional magnetic resonance imaging 99, 195^231.
of the brain. A. Rev. Biophys. Biomol. Struct. 27, 447^474. Strother, S. C., Anderson, J. R., Schaper, K. A., Siditis, J. J.,
Ojemann, J. G., Buckner, R. L., Akbudak, E., Snyder, A. Z., Liow, J.-S., Woods, R. P. & Rottenberg, D. A. 1995a Principal
Ollinger, J. M., McKinstry, R. C., Rosen, B. R., Petersen, S. E., component analysis and the scaled subprole model
Raichle, M. E. & Conturo, T. E. 1998 Functional MRI compared to intersubject averaging and statistical parametric
studies of word-stem completion: reliability across labora- mapping. I. Functional connectivity of the human motor
tories and comparison to blood ow imaging with PET. Hum. system studied with [15O]water PET. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow
Brain Mapp. 6, 203^215. Metab. 15, 738^753.

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)


Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org

1260 K. M. Petersson and others Non-inferential methods and statistical models

Strother, S. C., Kanno, I. & Rottenberg, D. A. 1995b Worsley, K. J. 1994 Local maxima and the expected Euler char-
Commentary and opinion. I. Principal component analysis, acteristic of excursion sets of 2, F and t elds. Adv. Appl. Prob.
variance partitioning, and `functional connectivity'. J. Cerebr. 26, 13^42.
Blood-Flow Metab. 15, 353^360. Worsley, K. J. 1997 An overview and some new developments in
Strother, S. C., Siditis, J. J., Anderson, J. R., Hansen, L. K., the statistical analysis of PET and fMRI data. Hum. Brain
Schaper, K. & Rottenberg, D. A. 1996 [15O]water PET: more Mapp. 5, 254^258.
noise than signal ? In Quantication of brain function using PET Worsley, K. J. 1999 Testing for signals with unknown location
(ed. R. Myers, V. Cunningham, D. Bailey & T. Jones), and scale in a 2 random eld, with an application to fMRI.
pp. 378^383. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Adv. Appl. Prob. (In the press.)
Strother, S. C., Lange, N., Anderson, J. R., Schaper, K. A., Rehm, Worsley, K. J. & Friston, K. J. 1995 Analysis of fMRI time-
K., Hansen, L. K. & Rottenberg, D. A. 1997 Activation pattern series revisited again. NeuroImage 2, 173^181.
reproducibility: measuring the eect of group size and data Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., Marrett, S. & Neelin, P. 1993
analysis models. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 312^316. Letter to the editor: authors reply. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab.
Taylor, S. F., Minoshima, S. & Koeppe, R. A. 1993 Letter to 13, 1041^1042.
the editor: instability of localization of cerebral blood ow Worsley, K. J., Poline, J. B., Vandal, A. C. & Friston, K. J. 1995
activation foci with parametric maps. J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Tests for distributed nonfocal brain activations. NeuroImage 2,
Metab. 13, 1040^1041. 183^194.
Van Horn, J. D., McIntosh, A. R. & Maisog, J. M. 1995 Letter Worsley, K. J., Marrett, S., Neelin, P.,Vandal, A. C., Friston, K. J.
to the editor: complications in the use of the SPM 2 statistic. & Evans, A. C. 1996 A unied statistical approach for deter-
J. Cerebr. Blood-Flow Metab. 15, 895^896. mining signicant signals in images of cerebral activation.
Van Horn, J. D., Ellmore, T. M., Esposito, G. & Berman, K. F. Hum. Brain Mapp. 4, 58^73.
1998 Mapping voxel-based statistical power on parametric Worsley, K. J., Wolforth, M. & Evans, A. C. 1997a Scale space
images. NeuroImage 7, 97^107. searches for a periodic signal in fMRI data with spatially
Vazquez, A. L. & Noll, D. C. 1998 Nonlinear aspects of the varying hemodynamic response. (Submitted.)
BOLD response in functional MRI. NeuroImage 7, 108^118. Worsley, K. J., Poline, J.-B., Friston, K. J. & Evans, A. C. 1997b
Wei, W. W. S. 1990 Time series analysis: univariate and multivariate Characterizing the response of PET and fMRI data using
methods. Redwood City, CA: Addison-Wesley. multivariate linear models. NeuroImage 6, 305^319.
Weissko, R. M., Baker, J., Belliveau, J., Davis, T. L., Kwong, Worsley, K. J., Cao, J., Paus, T., Petrides, M. & Evans, A. C.
K. K., Cohen, M. S. & Rosen, B. R. 1993 Power spectrum 1998 Applications of random eld theory to functional
analysis of functionally weighted MR data: what's in the connectivity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6, 364^367.
noise ? Proc. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 1, 7. Xiong, J., Gao, J.-H., Lancaster, J. L. & Fox, P. T. 1996
Winer, B. J., Brown, D. R. & Michels, K. M. 1991 Statistical Assessment and optimization of functional MRI analyses.
principles in experimental design, 3rd edn. New York: McGraw- Hum. Brain Mapp. 4, 153^167.
Hill. Zarahn, E., Aguirre, G. K. & D'Esposito, M. 1997 Empirical
Wold, H. 1985 Partial least squares. In Encyclopedia of statistical analyses of BOLD fMRI statistics. I. Spatially unsmoothed
sciences (ed. S. Kotz & N. L. Johnson), pp. 581^591. New York: data collected under null hypothesis conditions. NeuroImage 5,
Wiley. 179^197.
Woods, R. P. 1996 Modeling for intergroup comparisons of Zola-Morgan, S. & Squire, L. R. 1993 Neuroanatomy of
imaging data. NeuroImage 4, S84^S94. memory. A. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 547^563.

Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (1999)

You might also like