You are on page 1of 12

628 Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior 40(6) December 2010

2010 The American Association of Suicidology

Adolescent Help-Seeking and the Yellow


Ribbon Suicide Prevention Program:
An Evaluation
Stacey Freedenthal, PhD

The Yellow Ribbon Suicide Prevention Program has gained national and
international recognition for its school- and community-based activities. After the
introduction of Yellow Ribbon to a Denver-area high school, staff and adolescents
were surveyed to determine if help-seeking behavior had increased. Using a pre-
post intervention design, staff at an experimental school and comparison school
were surveyed about their experiences with student help-seeking. Additionally,
146 students at the experimental high school were surveyed. Staff did not report
any increase in student help-seeking, and students reports of help-seeking from
11 of 12 different types of helpers did not increase; the exception was help-seeking
from a crisis hotline, which increased from 2.1% to 6.9%. Further research with
larger, more inclusive samples is needed to determine whether Yellow Ribbon is
effective in other locations.

It is well-known that adolescent suicidal be- suicidal ideation are even more common.
havior imposes substantial social and finan- Nationally, 14.5% of high school students se-
cial burdens on individuals and communities riously considered suicide in 2007, and 6.9%
(Corso, Mercy, Simon, Finkelstein, & Miller, made an attempt (Eaton et al., 2008). Along
2007; ODea & Tucker, 2005). Suicide is the with death and injuries, another potential
third leading cause of death for adolescents consequence of youth suicidal behavior is
ages 12 to 19 (Centers for Disease Control contagion, as adolescents appear particularly
and Prevention, 2007). Suicide attempts and susceptible to imitating the suicidal behavior
of their peers (Gould, Jamieson, & Romer,
2003).
Stacey Freedenthal is Associate Profes- Research suggests that most suicidal
sor in the Graduate School of Social Work at the
University of Denver. youth do not receive mental health care (Far-
This study was supported by the State of and, Renaud, & Chagnon, 2004; Freeden-
Colorado Department of Public Health and Envi- thal, 2007) or even tell an adult about their
ronment, Office of Suicide Prevention. The find- suicidal thoughts or behaviors (Freedenthal
ings were originally reported in a paper presented & Gutierrez, 2007; Negron, Piacentini, Graae,
in April 2009 in San Francisco, CA, at the 42nd
American Association of Suicidology Annual Con- Davies, & Shaffer, 1997). Instead of telling
ference. The author would like to thank Jarrod an adult, adolescents more often confide in
Hindman, MS, Program Manager of the Office of friends, who in turn rarely tell an adult when
Suicide Prevention for the State of Colorado, for a friend discloses suicidal thoughts or behav-
his support. ior to them (Dunham, 2004; Kalafat & Elias,
Address correspondence to Stacey Free-
denthal, PhD, Graduate School of Social Work, 1992). Numerous explanations can account
University of Denver, 2148 S. High St., Denver, for the reluctance of high school students to
CO 80208; E-mail: Stacey.Freedenthal@du.edu seek help from an adult. Cigularov, Chen,
Freedenthal 629

and Thurber (2008) surveyed 854 students hotline numbers, instructions to youth to
after the implementation of a suicide preven- give the card to somebody who can help, and
tion program and found that barriers to help- directions to potential helpers on how to
seeking include a strong sense of self-reli- proceed. The overriding messages of the Yel-
ance; fears of psychiatric hospitalization; low Ribbon programming are that youth
uncertainty about how to talk with ones par- should tell an adult if somebody they know is
ents, teachers, or school counselors about suicidal (Be a link) and seek help for them-
problems; and a dearth of adults at school selves when necessary (Its OK to ask for
with whom the student feels close. Instead of help).
confiding in an adult, adolescents at risk for Anecdotally, there are numerous re-
suicide often use harmful coping methods, ports of Yellow Ribbons success. The pro-
such as isolation and drug use (Gould et al., grams Web site states that through the
2004). When it comes to seeking help from efforts of the Yellow Ribbon Suicide Preven-
an adult for a troubled friend, adolescents tion Program more than 2,500 lives have
may fear angering the friend, causing the been saved (retrieved April 14, 2009, from
friend to be hospitalized, or simply misread- www.yellowribbon.org/Lflstats.html). Accord-
ing the situation (Cigularov et al., 2008). ing to their Web site, some aspect of the Yel-
Many adolescents also fear adults reactions, low Ribbon program has been adopted by all
in particular perceived overreactions (Freeden- states in the United States, all provinces in
thal & Breslin, 2008). Canada, and 47 other countries worldwide.
In recent years, an increasing number However, reports about Yellow Rib-
of suicide prevention measures has been tar- bons effectiveness have remained anecdotal.
geted at adolescents in schools. School-based A search of numerous databases, including
prevention programs hold particular promise PsycInfo and Google Scholar, yielded no
because teachers and other school staff can published research reporting a systematic
serve as gatekeepers or gateway provid- study of the programs effectiveness at in-
ers, spotting students who seem to be in tur- creasing adolescents help-seeking behaviors.
moil and referring them to mental health ser- The only reports located about the Yellow
vices (Stiffman, Pescosolido, & Cabassa, 2004; Ribbon program were purely descriptive
U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, (Omni Research and Training, 2003), pro-
2001). Most youth who receive mental health vided only anecdotal information (Leahy,
services first receive them at school (Farmer, 2004), or addressed mainly attitudes toward
Burns, Phillips, Angold, & Costello, 2003), Yellow Ribbon and student help-seeking
reinforcing the importance of school-based (Bennett, 2003).
programs. This pilot study sought to evaluate the
A Colorado-based group, the Yellow effectiveness of the Yellow Ribbon Suicide
Ribbon Suicide Prevention Program (www. Prevention Program at a Denver-area high
yellowribbon.org), has gained national and school. The outcome variablethe action
international stature for its school- and com- that Yellow Ribbon was hypothesized to posi-
munity-based activities. The group was tively affectwas student help-seeking, both
formed by Dale and Dar Emme in 1994, for oneself and for a suicidal friend.
after the suicide of their 17-year-old son
Mike. Yellow Ribbon programming includes
schoolwide assemblies, peer leadership train- METHOD
ing for students, staff training for adult gate-
keepers such as high school teachers, com- We used a pre-post intervention survey
munity presentations, and local chapters that design. In the fall of 2007, before the intro-
provide outreach and education. Yellow Rib- duction of Yellow Ribbon activities, and 6 to
bon programming includes distribution of 8 months later in the spring of 2008, staff and
the Ask4Help card, which contains suicide students at the experimental school were sur-
630 Yellow Ribbon Evaluation

veyed about student help-seeking. Staff at a data collection began, the researchers pro-
comparison school, which received no Yellow vided all study hall teachers with study infor-
Ribbon programming, similarly completed the mation sheets and consent forms to distribute
surveys. The comparison schools principal to students. The students were to return to
would not permit students to be surveyed. the teacher the consent form with a parent
Schools were recruited to participate or guardians signature. The parent could
in the study with no random selection or as- provide or refuse consent on the form; re-
signment involved. Rather, at least 19 schools gardless, the student was asked to return the
in the area that had never used Yellow Rib- form. Consent forms were provided in both
bon programming were invited to take part English and Spanish due to the high enroll-
in the study. Sample selection concluded ment of Hispanic/Latino students, many of
when two school principals agreed to partici- whom were children of Spanish-speaking im-
pate. The participating school district re- migrants. The consent forms included the
quired that the schools names not be pub- national suicide hotline number (1-800-273-
lished, so this paper refers to them as the TALK).
experimental school and control school. Several incentives were provided in an
The experimental school had an en- effort to increase the return of parental con-
rollment of 870 students at the time of the sent forms. The three teachers who turned
study, and the control schools enrollment in the highest proportion of student consent
was about 1,600. The experimental schools forms were each awarded a $100 gift certifi-
demographics were more racially and ethni- cate to a local book store. To motivate stu-
cally diverse than those of the control school. dents, teachers were provided with candy to
Specifically, more than half of the experimen- give to students who returned a signed con-
tal schools student body was Hispanic, whereas sent form, regardless of whether consent was
almost 90% of the control school students granted or denied by the parent or guardian.
were White. The experimental school was Also, drawings for an Ipod (digital music
located in a less affluent and more racially di- player) were held three times in a 2-week pe-
verse area than the control school. According riod before the study began. Only students
to school district statistics, more than half who returned a consent form could be eligi-
(58.4%) of the experimental schools students ble for the drawing; students were entered
received a free or reduced lunch rate, com- regardless of whether their parent refused or
pared to only 9.8% at the control school. provided consent. Each drawing was tele-
Another difference between the exper- vised live over the schools closed-circuit pro-
imental and control schools concerned histo- gramming, which is produced by students,
ries of student suicide. The experimental and students also were provided a flier noti-
schools principal could recall no student sui- fying them of the drawing.
cides in the previous 5 years; the control Despite these incentives, about one
schools principal stated that several suicides third (35.2%) of the schools 870 students re-
had occurred in that time period. An addi- turned a signed and usable consent form. Of
tional student suicide occurred at the control those who returned usable consent forms, 87
school during this study. (28.4%) were not permitted by their parent
Neither the experimental school nor or guardian to participate in the study. Stu-
the control school had ever adopted Yellow dents whose parents consented were pro-
Ribbon programming prior to the study. vided with an assent form to sign immedi-
At each school, staff completed surveys ately before completing the survey in study
anonymously during a regularly scheduled hall.
monthly staff meeting. They signed a con-
Sample
sent form before completing the survey, and
the consent forms and surveys were turned Staff. At the experimental school, 70
in separately. For students, the study secured of the schools 76 staff members completed
active parental consent. Three weeks before the pre-intervention survey, representing a
Freedenthal 631

92.1% participation rate; this number dropped or choosing not to remain in the study. Over-
to 57 (75.0%) for the post-intervention sur- all, the student participation rate in both the
vey. At the control school, 98 (89.1%) of the pre-test and post-test surveys was 16.8%. All
schools 110 staff members participated in analyses were restricted to youth who partici-
the first survey, and 85 (77.3%) completed pated in both the pre- and post-intervention
the second survey. surveys. The demographic characteristics of
Staff characteristics are reported in Ta- these students are reported in Table 2.
ble 1. Overall, teachers comprised three Analyses were undertaken to deter-
quarters of school staff members in both the mine whether students who dropped out of
pre- and post-intervention surveys. The staff the study were different from those who
at each school shared similar characteristics completed both surveys. Students in both
for all demographic and job variables assessed groups were similar in terms of key study
with one exceptionstaff at the control variablesgenerally equal proportions in
school had worked at their current school for each group had endorsed suicidal thoughts,
a longer period of time, on average, than ex- suicide attempts, disclosures of suicidality,
perimental school staff. and the different types of help-seeking in the
Students. Although one quarter (24.1%; pre-intervention survey. There also was no
n = 210) of the experimental schools students statistically significant difference in study re-
completed the pre-intervention survey, only tention based on gender. However, compared
146 of those students also completed the to students who completed both surveys,
post-intervention survey, reflecting a de- larger proportions of students who dropped
crease of 30.4%. The attrition rate was due out of the study were Hispanic, reported
to students leaving the school, being absent, problems with drugs or alcohol, earned lower

TABLE 1
Demographic Characteristics of Staff at Experimental and Control Schools for an Evaluation
of the Yellow Ribbon Suicide Prevention Program
Pre-Intervention Post-Intervention

Experimental Control Experimental Control


(n = 70) (n = 98) (n = 57) (n = 85)
Variable % (n) or Mean (SD) % (n) or Mean (SD)

Age 42.1 (12.2) 42.6 (11.2) 42.1 (10.7) 42.8 (9.9)


Gender
Male 40.0 (28) 52.0 (51) 45.6 (26) 47.1 (40)
Female 60.0 (42) 48.0 (47) 54.4 (31) 52.9 (45)
Race
White 82.9+ (58) 93.5 (92) 84.2 (48) 94.0 (79)
Latino/Hispanic 7.1 (5) 3.1 (3) 7.0 (4) 3.6 (3)
Other 10.0 (7) 3.2 (3) 8.8 (5) 2.4 (2)
Position at current school
Teacher 72.9 (51) 74.2 (72) 79.0 (45) 84.7 (72)
Administrator 8.6 (6) 6.2 (6) 3.5 (2) 5.9 (5)
Other Staff 18.5 (13) 19.6 (19) 17.5 (10) 9.4 (8)
# years at current school 4.5** (4.9) 6.8 (5.2) 4.7* (4.6) 7.4 (5.2)
# years at any school 12.4 (9.2) 14.4 (9.3) 12.5 (8.3) 14.4 (8.7)
# years taught at any school 10.9 (8.4) 12.3 (9.2) 11.1 (7.9) 13.2 (9.1)

Note. All bivariate comparisons are between schools during the same survey administration.
+
p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01.
632 Yellow Ribbon Evaluation

TABLE 2 personal story of their sons suicide, coupled


Demographic Characteristics of Experimental with messages about asking for help when
School Students (N = 146) needed, telling an adult about a suicidal peer,
and not keeping secrets about a friends sui-
% (n) or cide risk. Students who completed the lead-
Variable N Mean (SD)
ership training distributed the Yellow Ribbon
Age 145 15.8 (1.2) Ask4Help cards after the assembly, as stu-
Grade level dents streamed out of the school gymnasium.
9 146 32.2 (47) School counselors continued to distribute the
10 146 32.2 (47) card throughout the school year to students
11 146 19.9 (29) with whom they worked. (A separate paper,
12 146 15.8 (23) currently in process, will report findings
Gender about students use of the card.)
Male 144 41.0 (59)
Female 144 59.0 (85)
Surveys
Race/Ethnicity
Hispanic 143 43.4 (62)
White 143 39.2 (56) The student and staff surveys were de-
Black 143 1.4 (2) veloped in consultation with two suicide pre-
Other 143 16.1 (23) vention experts and two school counselors
Free/reduced lunch 144 47.2 (68) and the principal at the experimental school.
The student survey was pilot tested in spring
Note. Only students who completed both 2007 in a series of focus groups with a total
the pre- and post-intervention surveys are in-
cluded in the study sample. of 27 youth, and modifications were made ac-
cording to youths recommendations.
Staff answered questions specifically
grades, and received a free or reduced price about students seeking help from them per-
lunch. All of these differences were statisti- sonally and about students disclosing their
cally significant (p < .05). own or a friends suicidal thoughts or behav-
ior. The student surveys assessed help-seek-
Intervention ing from a diverse array of helpers, both
inside and outside of school. The pre-inter-
The experimental school received sev- vention surveys assessed help-seeking behav-
eral components of the Yellow Ribbon Suicide iors in the prior year. The surveys after the
Prevention Program in the fall of 2007. These intervention assessed help-seeking in the cur-
included a 60-minute student leadership train- rent academic year.
ing program for about 15 students selected by At the study funders request, the study
school counselors; a 1.5 hour staff training for did not aim to assess attitudes or other sub-
teachers, counselors, administrators, and all jectively oriented variables due to the impor-
other school staff; and a 50-minute schoolwide tance of assessing actual behavioral out-
assembly. These activities were conducted by comes. Attitudes tend to be inconsistent
Dale and Dar Emme, the groups founders, or predictors of future behaviors (Glasman &
their daughter Becca Emme. Albarracn, 2006).
The student leadership and staff train-
ings included a digital slide presentation with Variable Measurement
information about the history of Yellow Rib-
bon; adolescent suicide, including statistics, Staff Surveys. Staff at the experimen-
myths, and warning signs; and the impor- tal and control schools answered survey items
tance of seeking help from an adult for one- related to demographic characteristics, years
self or for a suicidal peer. The schoolwide as- of teaching experiences, and several aspects
sembly presentation centered on the Emmes of student help-seeking. Specifically, staff
Freedenthal 633

were asked to specify the number of students for Children and Adolescents (SACA) (Stiff-
who had come to them in the prior year (pre- man et al., 2000). Personal help-seeking was
intervention) or current academic year (post- operationalized three ways:
intervention) and:
Scores for each help-seeking item
Asked for help with problems re- were summed to create a help-seek-
lated to stress, emotions, behavior, or ing scale score, with a higher score
drug or alcohol use equaling greater help-seeking. Each
Told you they were thinking about option was scored 0 (Never) to 4 (Al-
attempting suicide ways). For the first survey, the reli-
Told you they had attempted sui- ability coefficient for this variable
cide was 0.71; it was .76 for the second
Told you that another student was survey.
thinking about attempting suicide Responses to the different types of
Told you that another student had help-seeking items were dichoto-
attempted suicide mized (Yes/No) so that youth who
noted at least some use (Almost Never
The median number for each variable is re- and higher) were coded as having
ported in the findings. Responses additionally used the helper (Yes), and those who
were categorized as Yes or No to reflect the answered Never were coded as not
proportion of staff who had experienced the having turned to the helper (No).
specific type of student help-seeking or dis- The number of types of helpers con-
closure. The coefficient alpha for these six sulted by a respondent was summed.
help-seeking, dichotomous items was .74 in
the first staff survey and .69 in the second. Youth who endorsed suicidal thoughts
Student Surveys. Student surveys que- or a suicide attempt were asked to indicate
ried youth about demographic characteris- on the survey whether they had told an adult
tics, personal history of suicidality, emotional (yes/no). Adults were specified as parent,
health, and help-seeking. The survey used teacher, school counselor or other school
items from the Youth Risk Behavior Surveil- staff, mental health professional, other physi-
lance Survey (Brener et al., 2004) to assess cian, religious leader, or other adult.
the presence of suicidal thoughts or a suicide
attempt in the prior year or during the cur- Human Subjects Protections
rent academic year. An additional item in-
quired about suicidal thoughts or a suicide To help ensure student safety, the
attempt in the prior week. study team immediately scanned completed
Survey items rated youths self-reported surveys to discern whether students indicated
frequency (Never, Almost never, Sometimes, Al- that they had thought about or attempted
most always, or Always) for discussing personal suicide in the previous week. If yes, then the
problems with the following types of helpers: team matched the survey number with the
(1) friend; (2) parent; (3) brother or sister; (4) corresponding number on an assent form,
another relative; (5) teacher at school; (6) which had the students name. The team no-
school counselor or other adult at school tified the school counseling office of the stu-
whos not a teacher; (7) psychiatrist, psychol- dents recent suicidality, and a counselor met
ogist, social worker, or other type of counse- with the student within an hour. This process
lor, but not at school; (8) another type of occurred for six students before the interven-
doctor; (9) a minister, rabbi, imam, or other tion and for nine after the intervention.
religious leader; (10) crisis hotline; (11) In- For both students and staff, the last
ternet site; and (12) other adult. These items page of the survey contained the number for
were adapted from the Service Assessment the national suicide hotline and space to re-
634 Yellow Ribbon Evaluation

quest a call or e-mail from the study investi- Stata statistical software (Stata Corp., version
gator. For students, the last page of the sur- 9, 2003). All statistical tests were two-tailed.
vey also provided instructions on how to
contact ones school counselor if needed.
About ten students from each survey session RESULTS
left their e-mail address to request contact
from the study investigator, who contacted Staff Surveys
each student and provided referrals as needed.
The current study was approved by the As shown in Table 3, the experimental
Institutional Review Boards of both the par- schools staff reports did not indicate a statis-
ticipating school district and the investiga- tically significant increase, from the pre- to
tors university. post-intervention surveys, in student help-
seeking, disclosures of personal suicidal
Data Analysis thoughts or attempted suicide, or reports of
another students thoughts or attempt. Fur-
For the staff data, data analyses com- ther, the degrees of change from pre- to
pared results for the outcome variables across post-intervention at the experimental school
two categories. First, pre- and post-interven- did not differ from those of the control
tion responses from the same school were school for any variable, except for the student
compared for within-group differences over disclosure to staff of a personal student at-
time. Second, the degree of change in help- tempt; the experimental school experienced a
seeking from the pre-test to post-test was decrease of 7.9% from the pre- to post-inter-
compared between schools; comparing the vention surveys, while the control school
degree of change, rather than the difference experienced an increase of 7.7%, z = 4.6,
in results, took into account any baseline dif- p < .001.
ferences. To test the significance of a differ-
ence in proportions, the z statistic was used Student Surveys
(Twisk, 2003). For continuous data, the
Mann-Whitney (rank sums) test compared The proportion of students who re-
medians because the distributions were highly ported having thought about or attempted
skewed. suicide during the current school year in-
For the student data, the proportions creased slightly from 11.2% (n = 15) in the
of students positively endorsing the respec- year before the intervention to 14.7% (n =
tive help-seeking outcome variables in the 19) in the year of the intervention. This in-
pre- and post-intervention surveys were crease was not statistically significant, z =
compared. To determine the statistical signif- 0.85, p < .40.
icance of pre- and post-intervention differ- One of 12 types of help-seeking in-
ences in proportions, z tests were used with creased to a statistically significant degree
the prtest command in Stata. Where con- after the introduction of Yellow Ribbon pro-
tinuously measured variables were reported gramming (Table 4). The number of youth
(e.g., number of helpers), t tests or the rank reporting that they called a crisis hotline for
sum test of medians assessed statistical signif- help increased from 2.2% in the pre-inter-
icance. vention survey to 6.9% post-intervention,
As noted earlier, analyses of students z(144) = 2.0, p < .05. These proportions re-
responses were restricted to students who flect small numbers of students who called a
completed both the pre- and post-interven- crisis hotline (3 and 10, respectively). Other-
tion surveys. The staff surveys were anony- wise, no other type of help-seeking increased
mous, so it was not possible to limit analyses more than would be expected with random
only to those staff who completed both sur- variation, and two types actually decreased to
veys. Statistical tests were completed using a statistically significant degree: help-seeking
Freedenthal 635

TABLE 3
Staff Reports of Student Help-Seeking Variables Before and After the Introduction of Yellow Ribbon
Suicide Prevention Program Activities at a Denver-Area High School
Experimental School Control School Both Schools

Within- Within- Between-


Pre- Post- school Pre- Post- school schools
Variable Test Test difference1 Test Test difference1 difference2

Student help-seeking3
% Staff (Yes) 60.0 64.9 +4.9 54.2 56.5 +2.3 +2.6
Median number of students4 5 4 1 4 4 0 1
Student disclosed suicidal
thoughts
% Staff (Yes) 16.2 15.8 0.4 16.7 21.2 +4.5 4.9*
Median number of students 2 2 0 1 2 +1 1
Student disclosed suicide attempt
% Staff (Yes) 25.4 17.5 7.9 14.7 22.4 7.7 15.6***
Median number of students 1 1 0 1 1 0 0
Student reported peer with
suicidal thoughts
% Staff (Yes) 24.6 22.8 3.4 19.8 19.1 0.9 2.5
Median number of students 2 1 1 1 2 +1 2
Student reported peer attempted
suicide
% Staff (Yes) 22.6 12.3 12.3+ 13.8 22.6 7.8 21.1
Median number of students 2 1 1 1 1 0 1
Student gave teacher the Yellow
Ribbon Ask-4-Help Card
% Staff (Yes)4 0 1.8 +1.8 1.0 1.2 +0.2 +1.6
1
Within-school difference reflects change in proportion from pre-test to post-test.
2
Between-school difference is the change in proportion, from pre-test to post-test, at the experi-
mental school minus the change in proportion at the control school. A negative number indicates that
the experimental school had a smaller increase or greater decrease in proportion than did the control
school.
3
Student help-seeking for problems related to stress, emotions, behavior, or drug or alcohol use.
4
All medians are limited to respondents who reported at least one student for the respective
variable. No median is reported for the number of students who gave a Yellow Ribbon card to staff,
because no more than one staff member per school endorsed this item.
+
p < .10; *p < .05; ***p < .001

from a friend and help-seeking from a par- consulted did not differ to a statistically sig-
ent. Analyses restricted to students reporting nificant degree before the intervention (M =
suicidality in either survey did not show any 4.3, SD = 0.2) or afterward (M = 4.1, SD =
increase in help-seeking (data not shown). 0.2).
The mean score on the help-seeking scale The proportions of youth with suicidal-
before the intervention (M = 12.0, SD = 0.5) ity in the prior (or current) year who told an
decreased after the intervention (M = 10.8, adult increased from 27.3% to 45.5%. How-
SD = 0.5), indicating that students reported ever, this difference cannot be assumed to rep-
seeking help less frequently than they did be- resent an actual increase, for two important
fore the intervention, t(135) = 2.1, p < .05. reasons. First, the difference in proportions
The mean number of helpers whom students was not statistically significant, z(11) = 0.9,
636 Yellow Ribbon Evaluation

TABLE 4
Student-Reported Help-Seeking Before and After Introduction of Yellow Ribbon
Suicide Prevention Program Activities at a Denver-Area High School
Pre-Intervention Post-Intervention

Type of Helper N % (n) N % (n) z p

ANY 146 94.5 (138) 146 87.7 (128) 2.1 .04


Friend 145 91.0 (132) 142 83.1 (118) 2.0 .04
Parent 145 80.0 (116) 143 69.9 (100) 2.0 .04
Sibling 145 64.1 (93) 144 61.8 (89) 0.4 .68
Other relative 145 63.4 (92) 144 54.9 (79) 1.5 .14
Teacher 144 34.0 (49) 144 36.1 (52) 0.4 .71
School counselor 143 29.4 (42) 144 25.7 (37) 0.7 .49
Mental health professional1 145 15.2 (22) 144 13.9 (20) 0.3 .76
Other doctor 145 15.9 (23) 144 16.7 (24) 0.2 .85
Religious leader 145 18.6 (27) 144 16.0 (23) 0.6 .55
Crisis hotline 145 2.1 (3) 144 6.9 (10) 2.0 .05
Internet site 144 18.1 (26) 144 18.8 (27) 0.2 .88
Other adult2 144 28.5 (41) 144 22.2 (32) 1.2 .22
1
Mental health professional includes a psychiatrist, psychologist, social worker
or other type of counselor but not at school.
2
For this item, the survey specified, Another adult/elder not listed above.

p < .40. Second, 20.8% of participants who The practical significance of the in-
reported suicidality in the pre-intervention crease in crisis hotline calls is questionable.
survey skipped this question, as did 42.1% of On the one hand, the proportion of students
participants who reported suicidality in the using a crisis hotline tripled, from 2.1% to
post-intervention survey. 6.9%. On the other hand, the numbers in
question were very small, with 3 out of 146
students using a crisis hotline before the in-
DISCUSSION tervention and 10 calling one after the inter-
vention. Further, the study survey, consent
Based on both staff and student re- form, and student assent form all contained
ports, we found little indication that help- the national suicide hotline number. It is pos-
seeking by students increased after the intro- sible that those study materials, rather than
duction of the Yellow Ribbon program at a the intervention itself, contributed to the in-
Denver-area high school. School staff re- crease in calls to crisis hotlines. More re-
ported no increase after the intervention in search is needed to determine the potential
students seeking help for themselves or their impact of Yellow Ribbon on calls to crisis
peers, and a comparison with another school hotlines.
also showed no difference between schools. The study had several methodological
Students self-reported help-seeking rates re- limitations that could have obscured other
mained the same for nine types of helpers, intervention effects. Although the staff sur-
including professional, school-based, reli- veys had reasonable participation rates, the
gious, and informal sources. Adolescents student participation rate in the experimental
help-seeking from friends and parents actu- school was extremely low. The consent pro-
ally declined after the intervention. The lone cess likely introduced sample bias into the
type of help-seeking to increase, based on student component of the study because the
student reports, was calls to a crisis hotline. majority of students neglected to turn in a
Freedenthal 637

signed parental consent form, and research pre- and post-intervention surveys. The first
suggests that youth who turn in consent survey covered student help-seeking in the
forms are, relative to those who do not turn entire prior year, while the post-intervention
in a consent form, more likely to be White, surveys addressed a smaller time period from
to earn better grades, and to live in a two- September to early May. It is possible that
parent home (Anderman et al., 1995). Even student help-seeking would have shown an
among those who turned in a form, parents increase in the remaining months of the cal-
often denied consent to participate. The high endar year, although research indicates that
rate of student attrition from the pre- to young people seek help for emotional prob-
post-intervention surveys exacerbated the lems more often in fall and spring than in
potential for sample bias, because students summer (Morken, Sund, & Linaker, 2004).
who dropped out of the study were more Taken alone, the student portion of
likely than those who remained in the study this study contains formidable limitations.
to be Hispanic and to have psychosocial However, in combination with the staff data,
problems. the student data can serve to triangulate find-
It is impossible to know the direction ings. With the exception of a small number
of any sample bias that might indeed exist. of students who used a crisis hotline after the
We know that Hispanic and Black adoles- intervention, both the student and staff data
cents at risk for suicide have lower rates of reflected essentially flat rates of help-seeking
help-seeking than White adolescents (Free- at the experimental school from the times be-
denthal, 2007). This disparity suggests that fore and after the Yellow Ribbon programs
the study results might actually understate introduction. The congruence of staff and
the rates of help-seeking at this school, be- student reports supports the staff reports that
cause the participants were disproportion- student help-seeking from adults did not in-
ately White. However, youth with more psy- crease following Yellow Ribbon activities.
chosocial problems seek help more often This is especially true concerning student
than their counterparts (Zwaanswijk, Ver- help-seeking from school personnel, because
haak, Bensing, Van der Ende, & Verhulst, although only a fraction of students partici-
2003), and those who dropped out of the pated in the study, most staff at both schools
study had higher rates of substance use, took part.
lower grades, and lower family income than Methodological limitations aside, an-
students who completed the study. The pre- other possible explanation for the study re-
sumed higher need for help among study sults is that student help-seeking was already
drop-outs might balance any effect of racial quite common even before Yellow Ribbon
disparities. was introduced. In the pre-intervention sur-
As for other limitations, randomization vey, almost all students indicated that they
was not used to select the participating had received help informally from friends or
schools or students, and the student sample family in the previous year, so there was not
lacked a comparison group. The staff sam- much room for improvement. One quarter to
ples comparison school had markedly differ- one half of youth had discussed emotional or
ent demographic characteristics among its behavioral problems with a school staff mem-
students, and the control school was situated ber, mental health professional, or physician.
in a more affluent area. A student suicide oc- In contrast, a longitudinal epidemiologic
curred at the control school in between the study of 1,420 youth found that only 18% to
pre- and post-intervention surveys, and this 19% received help for mental health prob-
event could have stimulated help-seeking lems from any type of professional (Farmer
among students, limiting even more the et al., 2003).
comparability of the schools. Although this studys findings must be
Another potential problem concerns viewed tentatively, they point to two impor-
the disparate periods of time assessed in the tant avenues for future research. One avenue
638 Yellow Ribbon Evaluation

concerns investigating the reasons why al- of this small sample in this one school, rather
most all forms of help-seeking apparently did than a broad portrayal of how students at
not increase after the Yellow Ribbon pro- other schools might respond to the Yellow
gram, including barriers to help-seeking that Ribbon Suicide Prevention Program. Still,
students may experience. Another avenue of this study provides a small data set where no
research would explore whether a program others previously had been reported. The
evaluation of Yellow Ribbon yields different student data, though limited, largely corrob-
results in a different location, with a higher orate the staff reports that students help-
proportion of students participating in the seeking overall did not increase after the Yel-
surveys. low Ribbon programs introduction, although
more students indicated they turned to a cri-
sis hotline after the intervention. Ultimately,
CONCLUSION this studys limitations and findings can help
inform future investigations of the effective-
Due to the studys limitations, the stu- ness of Yellow Ribbon and other school based
dent findings must be considered a snapshot suicide-prevention programs.

REFERENCES

Anderman, C., Cheadle, A., Curry, S., Farand, L., Renaud, J., & Chagnon, F.
Diehr, P., Shultz, L., & Wagner, E. (1995). Se- (2004). Adolescent suicide in Quebec and prior
lection bias related to parental consent in school- utilization of medical service. Canadian Journal of
based survey research. Evaluation Review, 19, 663 Public Health, 95, 357360.
674. Farmer, E.M.Z., Burns, B. J., Phillips,
Bennett, S. (2003). Yellow Ribbon ambassa- S. D., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2003).
dors survey (Center report series No. 77). Aukland, Pathways into and through mental health services
New Zealand: Injury Prevention Research Centre. for children and adolescents. Psychiatric Services,
Retrieved April 21, 2009, from http://www.fmhs. 54, 6066.
auckland.ac.nz/soph/centres/ipic/_docs/cr77.pdf. Freedenthal, S. (2007). Racial disparities
Brener, N. D., Kann, L., Kinchen, S. A., in mental health service use by adolescents who
Grunbaum, J. A., Whalen, L., Eaton, D., et al. thought about or attempted suicide. Suicide and
(2004). Methodology of the Youth Risk Behavior Life-Threatening Behavior, 37, 2234.
Surveillance system. Morbidity and Mortality Freedenthal, S., & Breslin, L. (2008).
Weekly Report, 53, 113. They just go overboard: Why teens do not tell adults
Centers for Disease Control and Pre- about suicidal peers. Paper presented at the 41st An-
vention. (2007). Web-based injury statistics query nual Conference of the American Association of
and reporting system (WISQARS). Retrieved Janu- Suicidology, Boston, MA.
ary 6, 2007, from www.cdc.gov/ncipc/wisqars. Freedenthal, S., & Gutierrez, P. (2007).
Cigularov, K., Chen, P. Y., & Thurber, Adolescents disclosures of suicidality: Who
B. W. (2008). What prevents adolescents from knows? In J. R. Rogers (Ed.), Suicide 2006: Pro-
seeking help after a suicide education program? ceedings of the 39th annual conference of the American
Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 38, 7486. Association of Suicidology. Washington, DC: Ameri-
Corso, P., Mercy, J., Simon, T., Finkel- can Association of Suicidology.
stein, E., & Miller, T. (2007). Medical costs and Glasman, L. R., & Albarracn, D. (2006).
productivity losses due to interpersonal and self- Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: A
directed violence in the United States. American meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation.
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 32, 474482. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 778822.
Dunham, K. (2004). Young adults support Gould, M., Jamieson, P., & Romer, D.
strategies when peers disclose suicidal intent. Sui- (2003). Media contagion and suicide among the
cide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 34, 5665. young. American Behavioral Scientist, 46, 1269
Eaton, D. K., Kann, L., Kinchen, S., 1284.
Shanklin, S., Ross, J., Hawkins, J., et al. (2008). Gould, M. S., Velting, D. M., Kleinman,
Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance: United States, M., Lucas, C. P., Thomas, J. G., & Chung, M.
2007. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 57, 1 (2004). Teenagers attitudes about coping strate-
131. gies and help-seeking behavior for suicidality.
Freedenthal 639

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado- suicide prevention program. Retrieved on April 21,
lescent Psychiatry, 43, 11241133. 2009, from http://www.omni.org/docs/yellow_rib
Kalafat, J., & Elias, M. (1992). Adoles- bon.pdf.
cents experience with and response to suicidal Stiffman, A. R., Horwitz, S. M., Hoag-
peers. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 22, wood, K., Compton, W., Cottler, L., Bean,
315321. D. L., et al. (2000). The Service Assessment for
Leahy, S. K. (2004). Colorado LINK: Teen Children and Adolescents (SACA): Adult and
suicide prevention initiative: Evaluation report sum- child reports. Journal of the American Academy of
mary. Denver, CO: The Colorado Trust. Re- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 10321039.
trieved April 21, 2009, from http://www.colorado Stiffman, A. R., Pescosolido, B. A., &
trust.org/attachments/0000/2962/ColoradoLINK Cabassa, L. J. (2004). Building a model to under-
SummaryReport.pdf. stand youth service access: The Gateway Provider
Morken, G., Sund, A. M., & Linaker, Model. Mental Health Services Research, 6, 189
O. M. (2004). A help-line for children. Seasonal 198.
variations in issues. Psychiatry Research, 128, 191 Twisk, J.W.R. (2003). Applied longitudinal
197. analysis for epidemiology: A practical guide. New
Negron, R., Piacentini, J., Graae, F., York: Cambridge University Press.
Davies, M., & Shaffer, D. (1997). Microanalysis U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Ser-
of adolescent suicide attempters and ideators dur- vices. (2001). The national strategy for suicide pre-
ing the acute suicidal episode. Journal of the Amer- vention: Goals and objectives for action. Washington,
ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, DC: Author.
15121519. Zwaanswijk, M., Verhaak, P.F.M., Bens-
ODea, D., & Tucker, S. (2005). The cost ing, J. M., Van der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C.
of suicide to society. Wellington, New Zealand: Min- (2003). Help seeking for emotional and behav-
istry of Health. Retrieved on September 5, 2008, ioural problems in children and adolescents: A re-
from http://www.moh.govt.nz/moh.nsf/0/BFA96 view of recent litetature. European Child & Adoles-
AA9486D2C3BCC2570D4008009E6/$File/thecost cent Psychiatry, 12, 153161.
ofsuicidetosociety.pdf.
Omni Research and Training. (2003). A Manuscript Received: January 2, 2010
preliminary evaluation of Colorados Yellow Ribbon Revision Accepted: January 8, 2010

You might also like