You are on page 1of 8

Cognition 146 (2016) 431438

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Cognition
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT

Believe it or not: Moving non-biological stimuli believed to have human


origin can be represented as human movement
E. Gowen a,, E. Bolton a, E. Poliakoff b
a
Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Manchester, UK
b
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Does our brain treat non-biological movements (e.g. moving abstract shapes or robots) in the same way
Received 9 December 2014 as human movements? The current work tested whether the movement of a non-biological rectangular
Revised 28 July 2015 object, believed to be based on a human action is represented within the observers motor system. A
Accepted 13 October 2015
novel visuomotor priming task was designed to pit true imitative compatibility, due to human action
Available online 9 November 2015
representation against more general stimulus response compatibility that has confounded previous belief
experiments. Stimulus response compatibility effects were found for the object. However, imitative
Keywords:
compatibility was found when participants repeated the object task with the belief that the object was
Visuomotor priming
Automatic imitation
based on a human finger movement, and when they performed the task viewing a real human hand.
Mirror neuron system These results provide the first demonstration that non-biological stimuli can be represented as a human
Stimulus response compatibility movement if they are believed to have human agency and have implications for interactions with
Humanrobot interaction technology and robots.
Crown Copyright 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction such as in healthcare, education and entertainment (Andrade et al.,


2014; Chaminade & Cheng, 2009; Dautenhahn, 2007; Tapus,
It is well known that observation of a human action can influ- Mataric, & Scassellati, 2007). More controversially, representing
ence the observers own motor system. For example, observing the action of a non-human agent may suggest the attribution of
another persons action activates brain areas involved in execution characteristics associated with humans such as mental states to
of that action (Gazzola & Keysers, 2009; Kilner, Neal, Weiskopf, non-human stimuli (Chaminade & Cheng, 2009). In this work, we
Friston, & Frith, 2009) and can interfere with or facilitate move- address whether belief that a non-biological stimulus is based
ment production (Brass, Bekkering, & Prinz, 2001; Sturmer, on a human action produces action representation, using a
Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2000). These effects are thought to be due behavioural visuomotor priming task.
to the mirror neuron system (MNS) present within the premotor In visuomotor priming, also termed Automatic Imitation,
cortex and inferior parietal lobe that responds during both obser- observing and performing a compatible action (e.g. lifting ones
vation and execution of an action (Buccino et al., 2001; Rizzolatti index finger while observing another index finger move upwards)
& Craighero, 2004; Van Overwalle & Baetens, 2009). Recently, there facilitates reaction times, whereas reaction times are slowed when
has been increasing interest in whether non-biological stimuli (e.g. observing a movement incompatible with a performed action (e.g.
abstract shapes or robots) are processed in a similar way to human lifting ones index finger while observing a finger press; Brass
actions, leading to non-biological movements being represented et al., 2001). As visuomotor priming is likely to result from activa-
within the observers motor system (Gowen & Poliakoff, 2012; tion of the MNS (Catmur, Walsh, & Heyes, 2009; Heyes, 2011), it
Press, 2011). Measuring whether non-biological movements are provides a behavioural measure of whether an action is repre-
represented in a similar way to human actions could indicate the sented within the observers motor system. Although previous
success of humanrobot interaction which is particularly relevant studies have compared visuomotor priming for human and non-
as humanoid robots are likely to increasingly play a role in society, biological movements (Gowen, Bradshaw, Galpin, Lawrence, &
Poliakoff, 2010; Jansson, Wilson, Williams, & Mon-Williams,
Corresponding author at: Carys Bannister Building, Dover Street, Manchester 2007; Press, Bird, Flach, & Heyes, 2005) these are confounded by
M13 9PL, UK. Stimulus Response Compatibility effects, whereby responses are
E-mail address: emma.gowen@manchester.ac.uk (E. Gowen). faster to spatially or directionally aligned stimuli (Cho & Proctor,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.10.010
0010-0277/Crown Copyright 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
432 E. Gowen et al. / Cognition 146 (2016) 431438

2003). Consequently, visuomotor priming to non-biological stimuli downward and must respond when they observe a go signal in
could result purely from stimulus response compatibility effects in the form of a yellow flash (Fig. 1). Participants viewed a right hand
the absence of action representation (Jansson et al., 2007). rotated 90 degrees counter clockwise (from the participants view-
Models of visuomotor priming share the idea that priming point), in a thumb up orientation and were required to make a
occurs along a visuomotor route, transforming visual input into a key press response with their left hand. This stimulus orientation
motor response and that priming produced by stimulus response and response combination separated three stimulus response com-
compatibility and imitative compatibility are dissociated with patibility effects from imitative compatibility (Fig. 2). Directional
the latter involving the MNS (Gowen & Poliakoff, 2012; Heyes, stimulus response compatibility effects were removed by rotating
2011; Wang & Hamilton, 2012). This visuomotor route is modu- the hand so that up/down index finger movements now became
lated by top-down factors such as attention, prior knowledge and left/right movements. However, rotating the hand introduces two
social cognitive processes which can exert influence at the early further potential stimulus response compatibility effects (i) left-
sensory input stage or at the later motor, output stage (Gowen & down and up-right stimulus response pairings are faster (orthogo-
Poliakoff, 2012; Heyes, 2011). One top-down influence, termed nal stimulus response compatibility; Weeks & Proctor, 1990); (ii)
belief refers to prior knowledge or assumptions that a person has an advantage when the stimulus and response are on the same side
about the observed stimulus. For example, visuomotor priming is of space (Simon effect; Simon, 1990).
greater if a person believes (having received explicit instruction) By using a thumb up orientation together with a left handed
that the non-biological stimulus is created from a human move- pressing response (Fig. 2) we were able to isolate imitative com-
ment (Liepelt & Brass, 2010; Shen, Kose-Bagci, Saunders, & patibility from both orthogonal stimulus response compatibility
Dautenhahn, 2011; Stanley, Gowen, & Miall, 2007), whereas belief and the Simon effect. Thus, when the finger moves leftward across
that a hand is virtual can reduce priming (Longo & Bertenthal, the screen, this is compatible in terms of the Simon effect (left
2009). A more spontaneous or implicit form of belief could also advantage due to using left hand to response) and orthogonal stim-
occur for non-biological stimuli that have human characteristics ulus response compatibility (down-left advantage when pressing
(e.g. a robot) or for non-biological stimuli that are presented in a button), but is imitatively incompatible (downward response,
similar context to a previous human stimulus (Stanley et al., observing upward finger movement). However, when the finger
2007). On the one hand, these results could suggest that belief pro- moves rightward across the screen, this is incompatible in terms
duces action representation for non-biological stimuli by activating of the Simon effect and orthogonal stimulus response compatibil-
the MNS at the input stage or enhancing the MNS at the output ity, but is imitatively compatible (downward response, observing
stage. However, it could be that implicit or explicit belief merely downward finger movement).
alters attention to the stimulus movement, which either enhances Participants responded to the go signal under three stimulus
or reduces stimulus response compatibility effects via the input conditions. Firstly, they carried out the task while observing the
route, without activating the MNS (Heyes, 2011; Press, 2011). Con- object (object condition). Next, they responded while observing
sequently, it is still unknown whether a non-biological movement the object following a belief manipulation informing them that
can produce action representation equivalent to a human the object was based on the movement of a human index finger
movement. (belief condition). Lastly, they performed the task with the real
The aim of this work was to resolve these issues by separating human hand (hand condition). We hypothesized that orthogonal
imitative compatibility, due to action representation, from more spatial compatibility/Simon effects would be present in the initial
basic stimulus response compatibility effects. We used a modified object condition and that imitative compatibility would be present
version of the visuomotor priming task where participants observe for the hand stimulus. However, in the belief condition, there were
an index finger or blue rectangular object moving upwards or two possibilities: (1) There would be an increase in orthogonal

Fig. 1. Time course of one trial for the hand (top) and object stimuli (bottom). Trial starts at left of picture in neutral position and shows a downward movement for both
stimulus types. The yellow go signal is presented for 80 ms at 0, 120 or 280 ms following the start frame. A second end frame is presented for the 280 ms go signal. Pictures to
right of dashed line show position of flash on stimuli. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this
article.)
E. Gowen et al. / Cognition 146 (2016) 431438 433

Left Right

Imitation=incompatible Imitation=compatible

Simon effect=compatible Simon effect=incompatible

Orthogonal =compatible Orthogonal =incompatible

Response: press (down) using left hand

Fig. 2. Dissociating imitative and stimulusresponse compatibility. The participants response (downward key press) shares imitative compatibility with the finger when it
moves rightward across the screen (downward finger movement), but is incompatible when the finger moves leftward across the screen (upward finger movement). In
contrast, stimulus response compatibility effects are compatible with a leftward movement: the leftward stimulus movement is compatible with the left hand response
(Simon effect) and the downward response (orthogonal spatial compatibility; right-up left-down).

spatial compatibility/Simon effect if the belief effect produced compatibility (right stimulus movement, representing downward
increased attention to the stimulus or (2) Imitative compatibility movement, imitatively compatible with downward key press)
in the opposite direction, similar to the hand would be present if (Fig. 2).
the belief effect is caused by representation of a human movement. The hand stimuli were created by converting digital .avi files
In this case, as stimulus response compatibility can be stronger into a sequence of 9 still frames consisting of an initial start frame
than imitative compatibility (Longo & Bertenthal, 2009; Jimenez (finger in a neutral position) and 7 movement frames. The initial
et al., 2012) we expected the imitative compatibility effects to be start frame was presented for 1600 ms and was identical for
smaller than previously reported, but importantly in the opposite upward and downward movements, ensuring it was not possible
direction to stimulus response compatibility effects. to predict the movement direction. The 7 movement frames (pre-
sented for 40 ms each) depicted the finger making an upward
(33 pixels or 12.6 mm) or downward (27 pixels or 10.3 mm) move-
2. Methods
ment. The movement of both the hand and object had a biological
profile, accelerating toward the middle and decelerating towards
2.1. Participants
the end. To plot the trajectory in the object movement condition,
a blue rectangle, was positioned over the moving index finger in
20 right-handed healthy individuals were recruited for the
each frame. However, the hand was not visible in the resulting
study, but the data of two participants was excluded due to failure
clips in this condition; instead the rectangle was shown alone over
to comply with instructions or a high percentage (>20%) of
a background constructed to resemble that used in the hand clips.
responses made prior to the go signal. The mean age (SD) of the
The luminance (142 cd/m2) and size (5.5  1.8 cm) of the rectangle
remaining 18 participants (9 female) was 22.94 (4.71). As the task
were matched to the finger of the hand. The yellow flash
was new, a power calculation was not possible. However, an N of
9.8  1.6 cm and was presented for 80 ms. It was positioned over
20 was chosen based on earlier versions of the task that showed
the finger or object so that it covered the full extent of the move-
a significant interaction between stimulus condition (object/
ment (Fig. 1).
hand/belief) and compatibility, with a large effect size of
gp2 = 0.2. The study was approved by the University of Manchester
Research Ethics Committee and carried out in accordance with the 2.3. Procedure
provisions of the World Medical Association Declaration of
Helsinki. The experiment was split into three blocks. Firstly, the object
was presented, followed by a questionnaire asking whether partic-
2.2. Stimuli ipants found that the rectangle made them think of a human finger
movement (Table 2, questions 12). Secondly, participants were
Participants sat 80 cm from a flat screen monitor, upon which told that the object movement was generated from a human finger
sequences of stimuli were displayed using Presentation Software movement. This was demonstrated using the experimenters own
(Neurobehavioral Systems). Stimuli consisted of either a right hand hand, as well as using a printed picture depicting how the hand
rotated 90 degrees counter clockwise, in a thumb up orientation had been rotated. Therefore, the way the hand was orientated
or a blue rectangular object in an equivalent position (Fig. 1). Par- and the direction of movement (upwards = rightwards; down-
ticipants observed the object or the index finger of the hand mov- wards = leftward) was made clear to the participants and the
ing in a leftward or rightward direction across the screen (i.e. object was referred to as the finger, moving upwards/down
up/down movement of the finger) and were required to make a wards. They then repeated the task. Following the belief condition,
press response using their left index finger, when they observed participants completed a second short questionnaire, asking to
a yellow flash appear (Fig. 1). Key presses were recorded on a sep- what extent they believed that the object movement was based
arate keypad that was positioned centrally in front of the computer on a human finger movement (Table 2, questions 35). Thirdly,
screen, so that the participants left hand was aligned with the cen- participants observed the hand stimulus.
tre of the image on screen. This stimulus orientation and response Each trial consisted of the 9 frames detailed above depicting the
combination separated the Simon effect (left advantage due to moving finger or object (Fig. 1). The yellow flash go signal appeared
using left hand to response) and orthogonal spatial compatibility at one of three different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs)
effects (down-left advantage when pressing button) from imitative following the start frame (0, 120, 280 ms). The three SOAs were
434 E. Gowen et al. / Cognition 146 (2016) 431438

chosen to assess the strength of priming at the start, middle and Table 1
end of the movement. As the 280 ms SOA occurred after the end Reaction times for the different conditions.

frame, a second end frame was presented. In the case of no-go tri- Condition SOA Reaction time (ms) [95% Compatibility effect (ms)
als, the flash did not appear. For baseline trials, no movement (ms) confidence intervals] [95% confidence intervals]
frames were presented. Instead, the flash appeared after the start Object 0 355.03 [334.7, 375.4] 5.25 [ 16.89, 6.38]
frame, followed again by the start frame for 40 ms. Both the no- 349.78 [328.9, 370.6]
go and baseline trials decreased predictability of the flash go signal. 120 331.19 [308.2, 354.2] 13.48 [ 24.31, 2.64]
317.77 [298.6, 336.8]
Trials were terminated if the participant made a response or if no 280 322.98 [307.5, 338.5] 11.34 [ 24.86, 2.06]
response occurred within 2000 ms of the appearance of the flash. 311.58 [294.4, 328.8]
Between each trial a blank screen was displayed for 2000 ms. Belief 0 389.64 [353.0, 426.3] 0.99 [ 16.69, 18.67]
Compatibility was determined by whether the direction (press) 390.63 [346.3, 435.0]
was compatible or incompatible with the finger stimulus move- 120 334.33 [301.8, 366.9] 12.3 [ 0.12, 24.72]
ment (upward/downward). As detailed in Fig. 2, upward finger 346.63 [305.7, 387.6]
280 332.21 [305.1, 359.3] 5.32 [ 9.8, 20.46]
movements correspond to leftward movements across the screen
337.53 [303.5, 371.5]
for all three stimuli, while downward movements correspond to
Hand 0 378.24 [343.7, 412.8] 4.48 [ 20.18, 11.22]
rightward movements. If Reaction Times (RTs) are faster when
373.76 [345.4, 402.1]
the stimulus was moving in the rightward (compatible) compared 120 319.93 [299.2, 340.6] 7.6 [ 11.1, 26.3]
to leftward (incompatible) direction, this will result in positive 327.53 [292.4, 339.6]
compatibility effects (IncompatibleCompatible RTs). In this case, 280 316.01 [292.4, 339.6] 9.15 [ 9.18, 27.48]
movements have been facilitated by representation of an upward 325.16 [301.5, 348.8]

moving finger movement and indicate the presence of imitative Note: Bold = compatible responses.
compatibility. However, if RTs are faster when the stimulus was
moving in the leftward (incompatible) compared to rightward
(compatible) direction, this will result in negative compatibility Table 2
Questionnaire results.
effects (IncompatibleCompatible RTs). Here, movements have
been facilitated by the direction/position of the stimulus and indi- Question Mean One
cate the presence of stimulus response compatibility effects. score [95% sample t
CI] test
Therefore, positive compatibility effects represent imitative com-
patibility, whereas negative compatibility effects represent stimu- 1 During the first half of the experiment, did you 0.12 t = 1.46;
lus response compatibility (the left/right movement). at any time think that the moving rectangle [ 0.1, 0.3] p = .16
might represent a human finger movement?
The experimental conditions were stimulus condition (object, 2 To what extent, if at all did the movement of 0.43 t = 1.58;
belief, hand)  compatibility (compatible, incompatible)  SOA the rectangle remind you of a human finger [ 0.1, 1.0] p = .13
(0, 120, 280 ms). For each block, the 6 compatibility  SOA condi- movement?
tions were presented 12 times, together with 12 baseline and 12 3 Did you think of the rectangle as more of a 7.59 [6.5, t = 15.4;
block representation of a moving finger, 8.6] p < .001
no go trials giving a total of 96 trials. These trials were split into
compared to when you had seen the rectangle
2 mini-blocks of 48 trials and a short break was given after each for the first time?
mini-block. A longer break was given after each block. For the 4 To what extent did you think of the moving 7.0 [5.8, t = 12.5;
entire experiment there were 216 experimental trials, 36 baseline rectangle as being a human finger compared to 8.2] p < .001
trials and 36 no go trials, totalling 288 trials. when you had seen the block for the first time?
5 When you were told the rectangle represented 7.43 [6.3, t = 13.4;
a finger movement, how much did you believe 8.6] p < .001
that this was true?
3. Results
Note: Questions 12 were asked following the object condition, questions 35 fol-
lowing the belief condition. For questions 1, 2, 4, 5, a score of 0 represents not at
RTs for trials were removed if the participant made an incorrect all and a score of 10 represents very much. For question 3, a score of 0 represents
response, did not respond, if the response was longer than 1000 ms disagree and 10 represents agree.
or shorter than 150 ms or lay outside of 2.365 standard deviations
of the participants mean RT (Van Selst & Jolicoeur, 1994). This
resulted in a loss of 0.39% of trials. compatible and incompatible reaction times (across SOA) revealed
A repeated-measures ANOVA with factors of stimulus condition that a significant negative compatibility effect was present for the
(object, belief, hand), SOA and compatibility was conducted on object condition (t(53) = 3.077, p = 0.003; d = 0.24), but that com-
mean RTs (Table 1). There was a main effect of SOA (F(2,34) patibility effects were not significant for the belief or hand condi-
= 51.026, p < .0005, gp2 = .75), indicating that while RTs were sig- tion (t(53) < 1.5, p > 0.14; d < 0.08) (Bonferroni correction = 0.02).
nificantly slower at 0 ms compared to 120 ms (t(17) = 11.202, These results show that while negative compatibility effects, repre-
p < .0005) and 280 ms (t(17) = 5.193, p < .0005), there was no sig- senting stimulus response compatibility and Simon effects were
nificant difference between RTs at 120 ms and 280 ms (t(17) observed for the object, positive (non significant) compatibility
= 1.017, p = .323) (Bonferroni correction = 0.02). Importantly, there effects, representing imitation of a human action were observed
was a significant interaction between stimulus condition and com- for both the belief and hand condition.
patibility (F(2,34) = 5.461, p = .009, gp2 = .24) (Fig. 3). Paired t-tests Finally, there was a significant interaction between stimulus
conducted between mean compatibility effects in each stimulus condition and SOA (F(4,72) = 2.850, p = .049, gp2 = 0.13). Paired t
condition (averaged across SOA) indicated that compatibility tests comparing differences in RTs between the stimulus pairings
effects were significantly smaller for the object ( 10.04 ms) than at each SOA indicated that the difference between belief and hand
both the belief (6.20 ms) (t(17) = 3.024, p = .008; d = 0.6) and (14.4 ms) was significantly smaller than the difference between
the hand condition (4.09 ms) (t(17) = 2.535, p = .021; d = 0.48), the object and hand conditions (23.6 ms) for the 0 ms SOA only
while there was no significant difference between the belief and (t(35) = 3.044, p = 0.004, Bonferroni correction = 0.005; d = 0.71).
hand conditions (t(17) = .416, p = .628; d = 0.06) (Bonferroni Simple main effects comparisons at 0 ms SOA also revealed signif-
correction = 0.02). Simple main effects (paired t tests) comparing icantly faster responses for the object (352.4 ms) than both the
E. Gowen et al. / Cognition 146 (2016) 431438 435

Fig. 3. Compatibility effects for each of the three stimulus conditions. Positive compatibility effects indicate imitative compatibility, whereas negative compatibility effects
indicate stimulus response compatibility. Standard error bars are shown (Morey, 2008).

belief (390.14 ms; t(35) = 3.044, p = .004; d = 0.62) and the hand ment to pit orthogonal spatial compatibility and Simon effects
(376.0 ms; t(35) = 2.98, p = .005; d = 0.46), but no difference against imitative compatibility. To test this we presented a new
between the belief and hand condition (t(35) = 1.46, p = .15; group of 19 participants (9 female, mean age (SD): 22.1 (3.01)
d = 0.2) (Bonferroni correction = 0.02). There were no significant with identical hand stimuli to previously described except that
interactions between compatibility and SOA (F(2,34) = 0.37, the hand was a left rather than right hand in thumb down position
p = .69, gp2 = .02) and condition, compatibility and SOA (F(4,68) (Fig. 4a). Participants responded to the appearance of the yellow
= 1.02, p = .40, gp2 = .0.06). flash by pressing down their right index finger. Using this setup,
Prior to the belief instruction, participants did not associate the when the finger moves downward (rightwards), the participants
object with a finger movement (Table 2). However, following the pressing response has both imitative and stimulus response
belief instruction, scores were significantly different from 0, indi- (Simon effect) compatibility, whereas when the finger moves
cating that participants considered that the object represented a upward (leftward), the pressing response is incompatible for both
human movement. imitation and the Simon effect. As imitative and stimulus response
compatibility effects are combined, we would expect larger, signif-
3.1. Control conditions icant positive compatibility effects. In line with this, a compatibil-
ity  SOA ANOVA revealed a main effect of compatibility (F(1,18)
3.1.1. Control Experiment 1: Order effects = 11.87, p = .0003, gp2 = 0.4) and an interaction between compati-
To test for possible practice/order effects when the object was bility and SOA (F(2,36) = 4.08, p = .03, gp2 = 0.19) (Fig. 4b). Simple
viewed for a second time in the belief condition, compatibility main effects (paired t tests) revealed significant positive compati-
effects for the first and last six trials of the object condition were bility effects for the 280 ms SOA only (t = 4.6, p < 0.001;
calculated and averaged across participants. Compatibility effects d = 0.67, Bonferroni correction = 0.02). This compatibility effect
were negative for both first (0 ms = 2.65, 120 ms = 10.7, was twice as large as the compatibility effects for the finger in
280 ms = 8.52) and last (0 ms = 8.42, 120 ms = 16.8, our main experiment (Fig. 3), highlighting that imitative and stim-
280 ms = 17.7) six trials. This suggests that the positive compat- ulus response compatibility effects are likely to be combined,
ibility found in the belief condition was not due to a practice effect. resulting in strong compatibility effects when in the same direction
This was further confirmed by presenting two separate blocks of (Control Experiment 2), but weaker compatibility effects when in
the object condition to a separate group of naive participants opposite directions (Main Experiment).
(n = 22, 15 females, mean age (SD) = 21.7 3.8 years). Compatibil-
ity effects for the second object block (mean across 4. Discussion
SOA = 4.59 ms, standard error = 2.89) were significantly lower
than the belief condition in the main experiment (mean = 6.2 ms, Our results provide the first behavioural demonstration that
standard error = 4.13; t = 2.8, p = 0.04). observation of a non-biological stimulus, believed to have human
agency, leads to imitative compatibility as opposed to stimulus
3.1.2. Control Experiment 2: Weaker positive compatibility effects response compatibility. More generally, by separating imitative
Although the negative compatibility effects for the object were compatibility from stimulus response compatibility, this is the first
significantly smaller than the positive compatibility effects found evidence to show that priming caused by non-biological stimuli
for the belief and hand conditions, these positive compatibility can be generated by imitative compatibility. When participants
effects were non-significant. This is likely to be due to the require- observed the object, prior to the belief manipulation, negative
436 E. Gowen et al. / Cognition 146 (2016) 431438

(a) Le Right (b)

Imitaon=incompable Imitaon=compable

Simon eect=incompable Simon eect=compable

Orthogonal =compable Orthogonal =incompable

Response: press (down) using right hand

Fig. 4. (a) Left hand in thumb down position, showing co-occurrence of imitation and Simon effect compatibility when responding with right finger press. (b) Compatibility
effects across the three different SOAs, showing large compatibility effects at the 280 ms SOA. Standard error bars are shown.

compatibility effects were present due to stimulus response com- rotation on every trial. As the belief condition closely follows the
patibility produced by the direction and position of the stimulus. pattern of the hand condition, this supports the notion that the
However, following the belief manipulation, compatibility effects action is represented in the belief condition. Secondly, the finding
became numerically positive and of a similar magnitude to the of similar compatibility effects for the first and second half of the
subsequently viewed hand stimulus. Importantly, compatibility trials in the object condition argues against practice effects or
effects for both the belief and hand condition were numerically decreased attention to spatial/directional (stimulus response com-
positive and significantly different to the object condition. If the patibility) properties leading to a reduction in stimulus response
belief effect was simply caused by increased attention to the stim- compatibility effects in the belief condition. This conclusion is fur-
ulus enhancing bottom-up stimulus response compatibility effects, ther supported by significantly different compatibility effects
the belief condition should have produced larger negative compat- between the belief condition and the second object block of Con-
ibility effects, similar to the object condition. Consequently, our trol Experiment 1. Previous belief experiments that have not sepa-
results indicate that belief that a non-biological stimulus is rated stimulus response compatibility and imitative compatibility
human-generated causes action representation, validating previ- (Liepelt & Brass, 2010; Shen et al., 2011; Stanley et al., 2007) also
ous belief studies unable to conclusively rule out the influence of indicate that this possibility is unlikely. These studies show
attention (Liepelt & Brass, 2010; Shen et al., 2011; Stanley et al., increased compatibility effects for the belief condition, whereas a
2007). More generally, our hand condition adds to accumulating reduction in the influence of stimulus response compatibility
data that imitative compatibility cannot be explained by stimulus effects would have resulted in smaller compatibility effects.
response compatibility effects (Catmur & Heyes, 2011; Jimenez Our results advance existing models detailing how priming is
et al., 2012). influenced by top-down factors (Gowen & Poliakoff, 2012; Heyes,
While significant negative compatibility effects were observed 2011; Wang & Hamilton, 2012). These models share the idea that
for the object, it should be noted that the positive compatibility priming occurs along a visuomotor route, involving brain areas
effects for the belief and hand condition were non-significant. such as the Superior Temporal Sulcus (STS), parietal and premotor
However, it is unsurprising that we obtained smaller imitative areas such as the IFG. The visuomotor route transforms visual
compatibility effects since imitative compatibility was pitted input into a motor response and priming produced by stimulus
against a number of stronger stimulus response compatibility response compatibility and imitative compatibility are dissociated
effects (Longo & Bertenthal, 2009; Jimenez et al., 2012). Indeed, (Heyes, 2011), with the latter involving the MNS. Priming within
when the direction of stimulus response compatibility and imita- the visuomotor route is modulated by top-down factors such as
tive compatibility were complementary as in our Control Experi- attention, prior knowledge and social cognitive processes, which
ment 2, positive compatibility effects were significant and more can exert influence at the early sensory input stage or at the later
than twice as large as the finger stimulus in the main experiment. motor, output stage (Heyes, 2011). There is increasing evidence
As with previous work, this highlights that stimulus response com- that one brain area, the Medial Prefrontal Cortex (MPFC) is likely
patibility can mask imitative compatibility when pitted against to modulate priming (Cross, Torrisi, Losin, & Iacoboni, 2013;
one another (Longo & Bertenthal, 2009; Jimenez et al., 2012) and Spengler, von Cramon, & Brass, 2009; Wang, Ramsey, &
that significant positive compatibility effects are more likely when Hamilton, 2011, 2010), including during belief manipulations
imitative compatibility is confounded with stimulus response (Stanley, Gowen, & Miall, 2010). As priming changed from stimulus
compatibility. Furthermore, the overall pattern of our results sup- response compatibility during the object condition to imitative
ports the theory that belief causes action representation as compatibility for the belief condition, mechanisms modulating
opposed to increased attention to stimulus properties. Firstly, the the level of motor output are unlikely to be responsible for the
direction of the compatibility effects was positive for both the effect of belief on priming. Instead, it is more likely that belief
belief and hand condition and differed significantly from the neg- causes the visual input to be processed by the MNS pathway,
ative compatibility effects for the object. In addition, reaction times potentially through connections between the MPFC and STS, simi-
were similarly slower in the belief and hand conditions compared lar to a gating mechanism (Liepelt & Brass, 2010). In contrast, when
to the object conditions, raising the possibility that although they belief is drawn to inanimacy (Longo & Bertenthal, 2009) priming
were not instructed to do so participants were mentally rotating may be channelled along non-mirror, stimulus response compati-
the belief and hand stimuli to match their own posture. Previous bility routes only.
work indicates that making explicit 90 mental hand rotations pro- Our behavioural results complement fMRI studies showing that
duces longer reaction times (Parsons, 1994) than the difference non-biological stimuli activate brain areas associated with the
between the belief and hand compared to object conditions. How- MNS (Cross, Hamilton, Kraemer, Kelley, & Grafton, 2009; Engel,
ever, these smaller differences may result from the rotation not Burke, Fiehler, Bien, & Rosler, 2008; Gazzola, Rizzolatti, Wicker, &
being necessary for the task, as well as viewing a consistent hand Keysers, 2007; Press et al., 2012). However, these studies are
E. Gowen et al. / Cognition 146 (2016) 431438 437

unable to identify whether non-biological movement activates the Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2011). Time course analyses confirm independence of
imitative and spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human
MNS as they have either not separated stimulus response compat-
Perception and Performance, 37(2), 409421.
ibility from imitative compatibility or do not use both observation Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: The role of
and execution conditions to identify the MNS. Furthermore, some experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philosophical
have used training paradigms to associate non-biological stimuli Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 23692380.
Chaminade, T., & Cheng, G. (2009). Social cognitive neuroscience and humanoid
with actions, whereas our study shows that action representation robotics. Journal of Physiology Paris, 103(35), 286295.
of non-biological movement can occur without prior training. Chartrand, T. L., & Lakin, J. L. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human
Combining our task with fMRI could be used to separate brain behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(64), 285308.
Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Stimulus and response representations
activity relating to MNS and stimulus response compatibility. underlying orthogonal stimulusresponse compatibility effects. Psychonomic
As imitation can relate to social affiliation (Chartrand & Lakin, Bulletin & Review, 10(1), 4573.
2013), it is possible that imitation could be used to measure the Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., Kraemer, D. J., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009).
Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human
success of humanrobot interaction, assessing the effect of modifi- action observation network. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7),
cations in robot design aimed at optimising these interactions 13831392.
(Hofree, Ruvolo, Bartlett, & Winkielman, 2014). Our findings indi- Cross, K. A., Torrisi, S., Losin, E. A. R., & Iacoboni, M. (2013). Controlling automatic
imitative tendencies: Interactions between mirror neuron and cognitive control
cate that providing stimulus response compatibility factors are systems. Neuroimage, 83, 493504.
removed, measuring the level of imitative compatibility would be Dautenhahn, K. (2007). Socially intelligent robots: Dimensions of humanrobot
a useful and possible marker for whether a robot elicits action rep- interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B,
Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 679704.
resentation. In addition, instructions and prior knowledge may be a
Engel, A., Burke, M., Fiehler, K., Bien, S., & Rosler, F. (2008). Motor learning affects
useful strategy for encouraging representation of robot actions and visual movement perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9),
facilitating human acceptance of robots (Chaminade & Cheng, 22942302.
2009). Supporting this, visuomotor priming has been found during Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2009). The observation and execution of actions share
motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: Single-subject analyses
observation of a robot movement only if participants are informed of unsmoothed fMRI data. Cerebral Cortex, 19(6), 12391255.
that the robot is observing their actions and consequently is en- Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The anthropomorphic
gaged in the interaction (Shen et al., 2011). Importantly, the pre- brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions.
Neuroimage, 35(4), 16741684.
sent results suggest that this belief effect is likely to be due to Gowen, E., Bradshaw, C., Galpin, A., Lawrence, A., & Poliakoff, E. (2010). Exploring
representation of the robot as a human-like agent as opposed to visuomotor priming following biological and non-biological stimuli. Brain and
increased attention to the robot. In our experiment, as the object Cognition, 74(3), 288297.
Gowen, E., & Poliakoff, E. (2012). How does visuomotor priming differ for biological
moved using a human trajectory, it is unclear whether belief allows and non-biological stimuli? A review of the evidence. Psychological Research, 76
access of this movement to the MNS or whether the object is pro- (4), 407420.
cessed as human. Although this question requires testing using a Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 463483.
Hofree, G., Ruvolo, P., Bartlett, M. S., & Winkielman, P. (2014). Bridging the
non-human trajectory, the results of Shen et al. (2011) support mechanical and the human mind: Spontaneous mimicry of a physically present
the latter possibility as their robot stimulus moved with a non- android. PLoS ONE, 9(7), e99934.
human trajectory. Together, these results suggest that a robot Jansson, E., Wilson, A. D., Williams, J. H., & Mon-Williams, M. (2007).
Methodological problems undermine tests of the ideo-motor conjecture.
can trigger human associated imitative behaviours that could lead
Experimental Brain Research, 182(4), 549558.
to higher level social representations (Chaminade & Cheng, 2009). Jimenez, L., Recio, S., Mendez, A., Lorda, M. J., Permuy, B., & Mendez, C. (2012).
These implications would also apply to the situation where a robot Automatic imitation and spatial compatibility in a key-pressing task. Acta
triggers implicit belief that they are human-like. A crucial next step Psychologica, 141(1), 96103.
Kilner, J. M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2009). Evidence of
is to assess whether those humanrobot interactions that produce mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(32),
imitation lead to better humanrobot acceptance. Furthermore, as 1015310159.
our object stimulus moved with a human trajectory, it will be Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010). Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief
about animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 221227.
important to understand whether the belief effect is apparent for Longo, M. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2009). Attention modulates the specificity of
non-biological stimuli moving with a non-human trajectory. automatic imitation to human actors. Experimental Brain Research, 192(4),
In summary, the current study has demonstrated that abstract, 739744.
Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to
non-biological stimuli can elicit imitative compatibility effects Cousineau (2005). Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4(2), 6164.
equivalent to those produced for a human movement provided Parsons, L. M. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior
that the non-biological movement is believed to be human- reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 709.
generated. These findings imply that non-human movement can Press, C. (2011). Action observation and robotic agents: Learning and
be processed by the MNS, as if it were a human movement. This anthropomorphism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(6), 14101418.
has implications for understanding the control of imitation, as well Press, C., Bird, G., Flach, R., & Heyes, C. (2005). Robotic movement elicits automatic
imitation. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 632640.
as developing successful humanrobot interaction.
Press, C., Catmur, C., Cook, R., Widmann, H., Heyes, C., & Bird, G. (2012). FMRI
evidence of mirror responses to geometric shapes. PLoS ONE, 7(12).
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of
Acknowledgements Neuroscience, 27, 169192.
Shen, Q. M., Kose-Bagci, H., Saunders, J., & Dautenhahn, K. (2011). The impact of
participants beliefs on motor interference and motor coordination in human
The authors would like to thank Shannon Atkinson and Robyn
humanoid interactions. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 3
Dowlen for their help with data collection. (1), 616.
Simon, J. R., & Proctor, R. W. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on
human information processing. Stimulusresponse compatibility: An
References integrated perspective. In R. W. Proctor, T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Advances in
psychology (Vol. 65, pp. 3186). Amsterdam, North-Holland.
Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2009). Control of shared representations
Andrade, A. O., Pereira, A. A., Walter, S., Almeida, R., Loureiro, R., Compagna, D., et al.
relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution. Human Brain
(2014). Bridging the gap between robotic technology and health care.
Mapping, 30(11), 37043718.
Biomedical Signal Processing and Control, 10, 6578.
Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2010). Resisting motor mimicry: Control
Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects
of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with
movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 106
frontal and temporo-parietal lesions. Social Neuroscience, 5(4), 401416.
(12), 322.
Stanley, J., Gowen, E., & Miall, R. C. (2007). Effects of agency on movement
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001).
interference during observation of a moving dot stimulus. Journal of
Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 915926.
manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(2), 400404.
438 E. Gowen et al. / Cognition 146 (2016) 431438

Stanley, J., Gowen, E., & Miall, R. C. (2010). How instructions modify perception: An Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier
fMRI study investigating brain areas involved in attributing human agency. elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human
Neuroimage, 52(1), 389400. Experimental Psychology, 47A(3), 631650.
Sturmer, B., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects with Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. D. (2012). Social top-down response modulation
manual gestures and postures: A study of imitation. Journal of Experimental (STORM): A model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(6), 17461759. Human Neuroscience, 6.
Tapus, A., Mataric, M. J., & Scassellati, B. (2007). Socially assistive robotics The Wang, Y., Ramsey, R., & Hamilton, A. F. D. (2011). The control of mimicry by eye
grand challenges in helping humans through social interaction. IEEE Robotics & contact is mediated by medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(33),
Automation Magazine, 14(1), 3542. 1200112010.
Van Overwalle, F., & Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others actions and goals Weeks, D. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1990). Salient-features coding in the translation
by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. Neuroimage, 48(3), between orthogonal stimulus and response dimensions. Journal of Experimental
564584. Psychology-General, 119(4), 355366.

You might also like