You are on page 1of 26

Journal of Small Business Management 2016 54(S1), pp.

7–32
doi: 10.1111/jsbm.12296

The Dark Triad and Nascent Entrepreneurship: An


Examination of Unproductive versus Productive
Entrepreneurial Motives
by Keith M. Hmieleski and Daniel A. Lerner

This study examined relationships of the dark triad personality characteristics (i.e., narcissism,
psychopathy, and Machiavellianism) with entrepreneurial intentions and motives. Results from
samples of business undergraduates (N 5 508) and MBA students (N 5 234) found narcissism to
be positively related to entrepreneurial intentions. In addition, results from subgroups of business
undergraduates and MBA students high in entrepreneurial intentions (i.e., early-stage nascent
entrepreneurs) indicated differences in motives for engaging in the startup process. Specifically,
we found all facets of the dark triad to be positively associated with unproductive entrepreneurial
motives, and observed differential associations of the dark triad characteristics with productive
entrepreneurial motives.

Introduction Artz 1995) as drivers of entry into entrepreneur-


New venture creation is typically thought ship within developed economies.
to be a positive or even heroic endeavor Only recently has there begun to build a con-
(Anderson and Warren 2011; Malach-Pines et al. certed movement toward exploring the darker
2005). This view is logical when considering sides of entrepreneurship (Baron, Zhao, and
that startups often provide employment, innova- Miao 2015; DeNisi 2015; Klotz and Neubaum
tive products/services, and tax revenues that 2016; Shepherd, Patzelt, and Baron 2013;
help to stabilize, grow, and revitalize economies Webb et al. 2009). The current study builds on
(Van Praag and Versloot 2007). Perhaps partly this movement by asking: “Are there dark psy-
for these reasons, the literature considering why chological characteristics and unproductive
individuals intend to become entrepreneurs has motives (e.g., a desire to appropriate value)
focused primarily on the “good in people”— that drive persons’ intentions to enter into
such as positive psychological characteristics entrepreneurship?” This is an important ques-
(e.g., optimism, dispositional positive affect, tion because organizations founded on the
and generalized self-efficacy; Zhao, Seibert, and basis of such drivers are likely to produce
Lumpkin 2010) and virtuous motives (e.g., a counterproductive workplace dynamics and
desire to engage in meaningful work and negative long-term societal and economic effects
deliver products/services that address important (Kets de Vries 1985; Lewin and Stephens 1994).
individual and societal problems; Cooper and Thus, with respect to theory, policy, and prac-
tice, it is arguably as important to identify

Keith M. Hmieleski is Associate Professor of Entrepreneurship at the Neeley School of Business, Texas Chris-
tian University.
Daniel A. Lerner is Assistant Professor at Deusto Business School, University of Deusto, Bilbao, Spain, and
Visiting Professor at Universidad de Desarrollo, Chile.
Address correspondence to: K. M. Hmieleski, Department of Management, Entrepreneurship and Leader-
ship, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX 76129. E-mail: k.hmieleski@tcu.edu.

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 7


individual characteristics relating to unproduc- suggested and speculated about (e.g., Kets de
tive entrepreneurship (which appropriates eco- Vries 1985), little empirical research has been
nomic and societal value) as it is to uncover conducted to investigate such dispositions among
antecedents of productive entrepreneurship those with entrepreneurial inclinations. This type
(which generates economic and societal value).1 of research is needed in order to capture a more
Our research question is examined using a complete and balanced view of would-be entre-
two-step process. First, we consider the relation- preneurs’ personality characteristics and how
ship of the dark triad (i.e., a collection of three they relate to both unproductive and productive
malevolent and ego-centric personality charac- entrepreneurial motives.
teristics: narcissism, psychopathy, and Machia- Second, responding to recent calls for the
vellianism; Jonason and Webster 2010) with study of motives underlying nascent venturing
individuals’ entrepreneurial intentions. Next, (e.g., Carsrud and Br€annback 2011), the results
within our primary samples, we then focus on are expected to reveal meaningful differences
those individuals high in entrepreneurial inten- regarding why individuals engage in the startup
tions (i.e., persons who are actively preparing to process. The literature on individual difference
launch a new venture, and thereby qualifying as characteristics relating to entry into entrepre-
early-stage nascent entrepreneurs; Reynolds and neurship has largely assumed positive underly-
White 1997) in order to examine the relation- ing motives (Zhao, Seibert, and Lumpkin 2010),
ship of the dark triad with individuals’ motives and that the creation of new ventures is inher-
for starting a new business. In other words, for ently beneficial to society (Baron 2015). Not-
those who have begun the initial stages of start- withstanding Baumol’s (1990) theoretical
ing a new business, we consider “why” they are consideration of institutional factors that
doing so—what are their underling motives for encourage unproductive versus productive
engaging in the entrepreneurial process? entrepreneurship, research has yet to examine
The findings are intended to make two pri- differences in individual-level factors leading to
unproductive (i.e., value-appropriating) versus
mary contributions. First, the results are expected
productive (i.e., value-creating) orientations to
to shed light on the dark triad as a previously
entrepreneurship. Thus, a large gap exists in the
under-examined set of personality characteristics
literature regarding why within a particular
within the entrepreneurship literature—ones that
institutional context (especially one conducive
are not only relevant for vocational and organiza-
to productive entrepreneurship), some entrepre-
tional behavior of existing firms (O’Boyle et al.
neurial individuals would be motivated to pur-
2012), but that also may have effects on the
sue unproductive versus productive venture
intentions of individuals to start new ventures
activity (Shepherd 2015). This issue is of funda-
(Kets de Vries 1996). The dark triad has become
mental importance to the field, as value creation
the leading paradigm for the study of dark per- has become an elemental component in most
sonality traits within the fields of psychology conceptualizations of entrepreneurship (Bruyat
(Adrian, Richards, and Paulhus 2013) and busi- and Julien 2001).
ness management research (Smith, Wallace, and
Jordan 2016). Moreover, entrepreneurship schol-
ars have recently pointed to the need for Theoretical Development
research on dark personality characteristics and and Hypotheses
how they relate to entrepreneurial entry and per- Theoretical Underpinnings of the Dark
formance (DeNisi 2015; Klotz and Neubaum Triad
2016; Shepherd 2015). Even though a dark side The dark triad is comprised of three malevo-
to the personality of entrepreneurs has long been lent and ego-centric personality characteristics:

1
Consistent with general economics, our discussion of value appropriation versus value creation is based on
whether value is extracted or generated. An example of value appropriation can be seen with patent trolls (i.e., individ-
uals or companies that pursue opportunistic litigation to capture existing value, rather than generating new value. Our
arguments do not suggest that profit-motive is unproductive; on the contrary, unprofitable firms fail, thereby failing to
create or appropriate value. In line with this point, our argument is not inconsistent with the potential of self-interest to
benefit other parties at the heart of neoclassical economics. In essence, we simply suggest that the higher an individual
is in the dark triad (i.e., psychological characteristics characterized by malevolent self-interest), the more the individu-
al’s motives for venturing will be to appropriate, rather than generate, value.

8 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism.2 somewhat more predictable, a dedicated-life
These characteristics are distinguishable, but strategy in which individuals focus on long-term
overlap with respect to an underlying propen- needs and relationships is generally most effec-
sity for self-serving, callous, and exploitative tive. Individuals high in the dark triad are
behavior (Jonason and Webster 2010). Persons known to adopt a fast-life strategy toward rela-
high in the dark triad tend to be achievement- tionships (Carter et al. 2015), and, more gener-
oriented and skilled at accumulating power and ally, a short-term view of the world that
extracting resources from their environment influences how they regulate their behavior and
(Jonason, Li, and Teicher 2010; Jones and Fig- allocate resources (Jonason et al. 2012).
ueredo 2013). They generally have a competi- The adoption of a fast-life approach is associ-
tive nature and lack altruistic or prosocial ideals. ated with low behavioral inhibition and a lack of
The dark triad has been referred to as the concern for long-term consequences (Sherman,
“James Bond” personality-type because individ- Figueredo, and Funder 2013). To the extent per-
uals high in its facets tend to be more confident, sons high in the dark triad personality character-
extraverted, and agentic than others when oper- istics regulate their behavior through a fast-life
ating in challenging and uncertain situations approach, they may take on risky endeavors
(Jonason et al. 2009). For these reasons, it is not such as new venture creation and do so without
surprising that research has found individuals requisite experience, connections, or resources
high in dark triad characteristics to be com- (Jonason, Koenig, and Tost 2010). Their confi-
monly found, and arguably overrepresented, in dence, lack of fear, preference for agentic
leadership positions, such as among politicians endeavors, disdain for authority and the status
and CEOs (Chatterjee and Hambrick 2007; Del- quo, and general comfort operating in unstruc-
uga 2001; Furtner, Rauthmann, and Sachse tured and dynamic environments would presum-
2011; Lewin and Stephens 1994; O’Reilly et al. ably make entrepreneurship an attractive
2014). It similarly reasons that individuals enter- occupational choice for such persons (Jonason,
ing entrepreneurship—a vocation that requires Li, and Teicher 2010). Moreover, it should be
high levels of self-confidence, leadership and pointed out that even though a fast-life approach
agentic behavior, and involves a high degree of is generally viewed as a counterproductive mode
uncertainty—may also be disproportionately of self-regulation in modern society, it may
high in the dark triad (Mathieu and St-Jean prove to be an adaptive and even superior strat-
2013). egy in certain situations, particularly in the short-
To evaluate the extent to which dark triad term (Jonason, Koenig, and Tost 2010).
personality characteristics relate to entry into A fast-life strategy has important implications
entrepreneurship and the potential underlying for social exchange, in terms of individuals’ per-
motives for engaging in this activity, we draw ception of exchange relationships and social
from two complementary theories: life history exchange behavior. Social exchange theory sug-
theory and social exchange theory. Life history gests that the value of a relationship is equal to
theory is a middle-range theory proposing that its benefits minus the costs (Cook and Rice
individuals select behavioral strategies in 2003). Social exchange is particularly relevant in
accordance with the demands of their environ- domains where markets do not exist or are at
ment, so as to maximize fitness and the likeli- best incomplete, such as in interpersonal rela-
hood of survival (Buss 2009; Roff 2001). tionships and in nascent organizing. Social
According to this theory, when environmental exchange differs from economic exchange in
conditions are brutish and the future is highly that contracts are not explicit or subject to rule
uncertain, a fast-life strategy in which individu- of law, instead it rests upon the expectation of
als focus on immediate needs and short-term reciprocity over time. According to social
relationships is the most effective behavioral ori- exchange theory, individuals foster relationships
entation. In contrast, under less harsh environ- and engage in reciprocal transactions with per-
mental conditions and when the future is sons whom they perceive as offering value.

2
Consistent with the personality literature and with other organizational research involving normal (non-
clinical) populations (e.g., Chatterjee and Hambrick 2007; Jonason and Webster 2010), no suggestion or assump-
tion is made as to whether individuals high in the continuous personality constructs of narcissism, psychopathy,
or Machiavellianism would be so high as to qualify for clinical disorders/diagnoses.

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 9


Thus, in general, social exchange theory pre- In the following section we elaborate on this
dicts that people are more likely to help those logic, explicated through life history theory, to
who they expect to receive reciprocal benefit develop hypotheses regarding the relationship
from in the future. While on the surface such of the dark triad with intentions to enter into
reasoning may appear consistent with the ego- entrepreneurship. We then make linkages with
centricity associated with the dark triad, it typi- social exchange theory to hypothesize about
cally is not. Reciprocal exchange is not antici- why the motives underlying such intentions are
pated by persons high in the dark triad; instead, likely to be based on relatively unproductive,
such individuals view relationship value as rather than productive, orientations toward
being maximized through interactions of taking entrepreneurship.
without giving (O’Boyle et al. 2012). Moreover,
since individuals high in the dark triad will shirk The Dark Triad and Entrepreneurial
reciprocity if given the opportunity, such per- Entry
sons expect others to do the same. Thus, indi- The adoption of a fast-life approach implies a
viduals high in the dark triad do not nurture short-term orientation in which individuals
long-term relationships, instead they develop choose to take on bold tasks without full consid-
many superficial short-term relationships— eration of the effort, experience, and resources
extracting resources from one relationship and needed to produce lasting success (Sherman,
quickly moving on once it has served them.3 In Figueredo, and Funder 2013). Since persons
so doing, such individuals tend to advance who are high in the dark triad tend to regulate
through life largely based on the efforts and their behavior through a fast-life approach, they
resources of others (Jones 2014). may choose to take on risky endeavors such as
Even though a fast-life approach is consistent new venture creation (Jonason, Koenig, and
with each of the dark triad personality charac- Tost 2010). Drawing from this perspective and
teristics, having similar implications for social based on life history theory, we now consider in
exchange, there are important differences with detail how each facet of the dark triad is likely
respect to how self-regulatory behavior is mani- to be associated with entrepreneurial intentions.
fest through each individual characteristic. Per- Narcissism. Individuals who are high in nar-
sons high in narcissism possess an excessive cissism are self-centered and continually seek
need for adoration that manifests in egotism, the attention and admiration of others (Twenge
vanity, pride, and selfishness (Twenge et al. et al. 2008). They also tend to be charismatic,
2008). Their sense of superiority and entitle- and are skilled at acquiring resources and get-
ment leads them to believe that others should ting others to adopt their plans (O’Reilly et al.
give to them without expectation of receiving 2014). In addition, they are known to engage in
anything in return (Hotchkiss 2003). Individuals grandiose thinking and expect others to adopt
high in psychopathy are emotionally callous their world view (Hotchkiss 2003). In relation to
(Decuyper et al. 2009). This allows them to nascent entrepreneurship, by launching a ven-
coldly, and sometimes aggressively, take with- ture, individuals high in narcissism can live the
out giving in return (Ermer and Kiehl 2010). fast-life by immediately becoming leader and
People high in Machiavellianism are character- CEO—bypassing the need to start below others
ized by duplicity—believing that the ends justify and to patiently climb the corporate ladder.
the means, and placing a particularly high prior- Moreover, entrepreneurship has become an
ity on power, money, and competition (Zettler admired and even sexy vocational choice
and Solga 2013). Taking from others without (Magister 2013)—befitting the self-presumed
reciprocation is viewed by such persons as greatness of narcissistic individuals. Finally,
acceptable (and expected) when so doing fur- research has found persons high in narcissism
thers progress toward the achievement of their to have a bias toward risk-taking, being prone
goals (Gunnthorsdottir, McCabe, and Smith to gambling (Jones 2013) and making risky
2002). financial investments (Foster et al. 2011)—

3
This does not preclude individuals high in Machiavellianism from superficially investing in relationships or
deferring immediate value extraction, so as to further their manipulation of others (e.g., as pawns and patrons).
Similarly, those high in narcissism and psychopathy may superficially maintain a relationship while it serves their
ego-centric interests of admiration, dominance, or parasitic lifestyle.

10 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


characteristics associated with persons engaged (Humphrey 2013). Finally, their lack of fear and
in nascent venturing and with following a fast- relative insensitivity to loss/punishment stands
life strategy (Buss 2009). For these reasons, it is to reduce or eliminate any venturing inhibition
not surprising that narcissism has been identi- related to high startup mortality or general fear
fied in recent research as being positively of failure (Morgan and Sisak 2016); thus,
related to possessing entrepreneurial intentions psychopathy may facilitate entry into entrepre-
(Mathieu and St-Jean 2013). neurship—even for those without sufficient
Psychopathy. Persons high in psychopathy preparation or experience.
lack normal levels of emotional arousal.4 Given Machiavellianism. People who are high in
their relatively limited ability to experience all Machiavellianism are driven by the perspective
but the most superficial and primal emotions, that the ends justify the means and have a strong
combined with cynicism, they are incapable of need for money, power and competition (Zettler
experiencing affective empathy. This, however, and Solga 2013). They are known for their desire
does not preclude them from understanding to win at the expense of others (Buckels, Jones,
what drives people (i.e., cognitive empathy), and Paulhus 2013). Moreover, they have no
and makes them well suited to take advantage compunction about engaging in socially deviant
of others (Jonason and Krause 2013). Individu- behaviors (e.g., lying, stealing, and cheating) in
als who are high in psychopathy excel in highly order to win, and are experts at masking their
stressful and uncertain situations, being able to true objectives (C^ote et al. 2011). Such persons
focus and perform well under conditions in also discount future consequences and have a
which others persons typically breakdown (Dut- need for immediate gratification—characteristics
ton 2012). Moreover, people high in psychopa- closely linked with the fast-life (Jonason and
thy are often attracted toward sensation-seeking Tost 2010). In relation to nascent entrepreneur-
activities and get bored easily (Hunt et al. 2005), ship, for such persons venturing may serve as a
thus fueling a fast-life behavioral orientation vehicle to aggressively compete, and if success-
(Jonason, Koenig, and Tost 2010; Jonason, Li, ful, quickly acquire inordinate amounts of
and Teicher 2010). In addition, they abhor wealth and power. Whereas excessive tenacity
social norms and enjoy going against the status and competitiveness are often viewed as coun-
quo (Mathieu et al. 2013). In relation to nascent terproductive among workers within traditional
entrepreneurship, by starting a business, per- organizational setting (Lu, Tjosvold, and Shi
sons high in psychopathy can avoid having to 2010), such characteristics are more commonly
report to others and needing to adhere to the revered among entrepreneurs and considered
social norms that exist within a corporate setting necessary in order to overcome high mortality
(Rindova, Barry, and Ketchen 2009). They can odds and to win business away from incumbent
instead shape the culture and norms for their firms (Robinson 2014). Further, the brutish con-
startup without undergoing the scrutiny and ditions that startups often face and the potential
monitoring faced by those entering an estab- benefits that new ventures provide (e.g., jobs,
lished organization as an employee (Staw 1991). tax revenues) can be used by Machiavellian
Entrepreneurship also offers the opportunity to entrepreneurs to obfuscate or justify question-
hit it big, aligning with their “all or nothing” able competitive tactics and behaviors that char-
sensation-seeking desires that characterize the acterize a short-term view and fast-life approach.
fast-life. Moreover, the wide range of activities In sum, consistent with general suggestions
in which entrepreneurs must take on would regarding the presence of pathological charac-
likely prevent persons who are high in psychop- teristics and tendencies among entrepreneurs
athy from getting bored (cf. Nicolaou et al. (Kets de Vries 1985, 1996; Lerner 2016; Verheul
2011). In addition, their ability to read people et al. 2016), we suggest that each facet of the
could help them to identify opportunities for dark triad may yield an attraction toward
new products/services or stakeholders to exploit launching a new venture, driven at least in part

4
We refer to psychopathy as per the dark triad and personality literature (e.g., Jonason and Webster 2010;
Paulhus and Williams 2002). This largely overlaps with psychopathy as well as psychopaths in the forensic and
clinical literatures (e.g., Hare 1991; Hare and Neumann 2006). Yet as psychopathy within the dark triad of person-
ality is more parsimonious and germane to non-clinical populations, use of the term here should not be conflated
with clinical models of psychopathy.

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 11


by a desire to live the fast-life. We therefore would be unfazed in doing whatever it takes to
offer our first hypothesis: be successful (e.g., so that they receive what
they are, in their own mind, entitled to). More-
H1. Individuals’ levels of narcissism (H1a), psy- over, their charisma and social adeptness facili-
chopathy (H1b), and Machiavellianism (H1c) tate shirking from reciprocation in social
will be positively associated with their inten- exchange relationships, enhancing their poten-
tions to start a new venture. tial to engage in rent-seeking and personal value
appropriation via their firm.
Psychopathy. Persons high in psychopathy
The Dark Triad and Unproductive versus are callous, insensitive to the needs of others,
Productive Entrepreneurial Motives and lack remorse (Jonason and Krause 2013).
In many respects entrepreneurship can be These individuals consider life from the perspec-
seen as a series of social and economic tive of every man for himself, viewing people as
exchanges between parties who mutually expect either prey or fellow predators (Babiak and
to benefit as result of transacting with one Hare 2006). They therefore do not have any
another (e.g., Goss 2008). For example, founders problem engaging in rent-seeking behavior and
hire employees and offer financial and other ben- externalizing all costs. Such behavior would in
efits in exchange for expertise and labor. In addi- fact be considered the norm for persons high in
tion, they sell products/services to customers psychopathy (Dutton 2012). In their minds, only
who expect to receive value from interactions fools care about the needs of others; dominance
and transactions that are made in good faith. Pro- and coercion are the way to achieve cooperation
ductive relationships exist when exchange and for an individual to maximize his/her gains.
increases value for those involved, whereas Not surprisingly, studies of individuals high in
unproductive relationships exist when one party psychopathy have found that such persons
appropriates value from others (Cook and Rice choose to gamble with others’ money if the
2003). Many entrepreneurs begin new ventures opportunity presents itself (Jones 2013). More-
for productive reasons, desiring to deliver prod- over, the ability of persons who are high in psy-
ucts and services that generate value beyond just chopathy to pick out individuals who are
for the entrepreneur (Cooper and Artz 1995). Yet vulnerable or easily dominated would allow
other entrepreneurs launch new ventures to them to identify stakeholders whom they can
engage in rent-seeking or value-extracting behav- take advantage of (e.g., employees, customers,
ior—appropriating existing value, externalizing suppliers) while offering little in return (Wilson,
costs, and potentially reducing others’ overall Demetrioff, and Porter 2008). In sum, we expect
welfare in the process (Baumol 1990). Building that the entrepreneurial motives of individuals
from social exchange theory, we now argue why high in psychopathy will be positively oriented
the dark triad personality characteristics are likely toward appropriating value for themselves, and
to be positively linked with unproductive entre- negatively oriented toward creating value more
preneurial motives and negatively associated broadly for others.
with productive entrepreneurial motives. Machiavellianism. People who are high in
Narcissism. Individuals who are high in nar- Machiavellianism tend to be cynical and do not
cissism feel that they are superior and too believe that individuals or societies care about
important to be bothered by others’ needs their well-being (Zettler and Solga 2013). As a
(Twenge et al. 2008). Their self-presumed great- result, they tend to have little concern for the
ness leads them to believe that people should well-being of others. In addition, they fail to
admire and serve them without expectation of reciprocate in social exchanges once the other
reciprocation. As such, they see no moral party has been used and is no longer convenient
dilemma in engaging in rent-seeking behavior to their own goals, and are motivated to win at
that involves taking without giving in return all costs (C^ ote et al. 2011). Judgment and
(i.e., value appropriation) (Brown et al. 2010). decision-making for individuals high in Machia-
Narcissistic entrepreneurs, much like narcissistic vellianism is weighted in favor of maximizing
CEOs, are apt to view their business as a direct personal gain and short-term profits, with little
reflection of their own greatness (Chatterjee and thought given to broader and/or long-term
Hambrick 2007). To this end, they are unlikely repercussions (Sherman, Figueredo, and Funder
to want their venture to be viewed by others as 2013). Therefore, in the startup context, faced
being unscrupulous, but behind closed doors with entrepreneurial uncertainty, such persons

12 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


are unlikely to be motivated to invest resources not be logical to sample full-time entrepreneurs.
in productive entrepreneurial strategies that Instead, a business-oriented population with
might only generate long-term value; they are variance in entrepreneurial intentions is needed.
instead likely to be motivated to pursue unpro- Accordingly, college business students were
ductive entrepreneurial strategies that appropri- selected as the sample population for the cur-
ate value in the present or near future. As long- rent research. Given concerns about the use of
term resource investments are uncertain, individ- students as proxies for other populations, it is
uals high in Machiavellianism can benefit with important to underscore the following points.
greater certainty by focusing on short-term wins First, business students as a whole do not
(Jonason, Koenig, and Tost 2010). This is likely broadly proxy nascent entrepreneurs; rather
to result in an appropriative rent-seeking orienta- they offer a general business-oriented popula-
tion in which relationships involve taking as tion from which to attempt to distinguish
much as possible while minimizing reciproca- between those who are high versus low in
tion. Such self-interest with guile could manifest entrepreneurial intentions. Thus, for research
in various ways, including: taking shortcuts on questions seeking to predict entrepreneurial
product quality, overcharging for services, taking intentions, sampling business students is appro-
advantage of employees, or use of a litigatory priate (Krueger 1993). It is also worth noting
competitive strategy. We therefore anticipate that that unlike more heterogeneous full-time work-
Machiavellian nascent entrepreneurs will be high ers with existing careers (e.g., persons who
in unproductive entrepreneurial motives and low have already become employees or entrepre-
in productive entrepreneurial motives. neurs), business students offer a population less
Taken together we have argued that, driven subject to omitted variable bias and endogeneity
by a fast-life approach, individuals high in dark threats. Additionally, many individuals begin the
triad personality characteristics who intend to entrepreneurial process while a student, and
become firm founders, will be motivated to use nearly all business students are on the verge of
venturing as a pathway to by-in-large appropri- entering the full-time workforce as (co)found-
ate value rather than create it (i.e., engage in ers, employees of a startup firm, or employees
exchange relationships with a focus on taking of existing organizations. Finally, to address
and externalizing). In addition, we have sug- concerns relating to the generalizability of
gested that there may be subtle differences undergraduate business student populations to
regarding how each facet of the dark triad older and more experienced adult populations,
relates to motives for venturing. Overall, we we used samples of both business undergradu-
posit that the dark triad personality characteris- ates and MBA students (Hmieleski and Corbett
tics are each positively associated with unpro- 2006).
ductive entrepreneurial motives and negatively Two primary data collections (N 5 508 and
associated with productive motives. Specifically, N 5 234) were conducted for the current
research. In addition, a supplementary data col-
we offer the following hypotheses:
lection (N 5 130) was performed in order to
examine the validity and temporal stability of a
H2. Individuals’ levels of narcissism (H2a), psy-
number of the study’s measures. Participants in
chopathy (H2b), and Machiavellianism (H2c)
both primary data collections completed a sur-
will be positively associated with their level of
vey instrument containing the measures
unproductive entrepreneurial motives.
described below, as well as several demo-
graphic items. The first primary data collection
(Sample 1) was with undergraduate business
H3. Individuals’ levels of narcissism (H3a), psy-
students, obtained from subject pools at two
chopathy (H3b), and Machiavellianism (H3c)
business schools in the United States. Subjects
will be negatively associated with their level of
were not graded on their participation and had
productive entrepreneurial motives.
no other incentive to respond in any particular
way. These subjects simply received one partici-
pation point for completing the survey, with all
Method responses being anonymous. The sample was
Data Collection Procedures and Sample comprised of 508 business undergraduates, with
In order to assess the relationship of the dark an average age of 21.5 (Standard Deviation
triad with entrepreneurial intentions, it would [S.D.] 5 3.22) and slightly more males (n 5 296)

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 13


than females (n 5 212). Eighty-one percent of Machiavellianism (example item: “I tend to
participants identified as Caucasian (n 5 411), manipulate others to get my way”). Scores were
14 percent had started a venture (n 5 71), 30 averaged for each construct, with higher scores
percent had worked for a startup (n 5 150), and reflecting greater levels of narcissism, psychopa-
36 percent had taken a course in entrepreneur- thy and Machiavellianism, respectively.
ship (n 5 181). In alignment with theory and previous empir-
The second primary data collection (Sample ical evidence of each facet being relatively stable
2) consisted of MBA students recruited through across time and situation (i.e., trait-like), Jonason
courses they were attending at U.S. business and Webster (2010) found the test–retest correla-
schools. The MBA sample did not receive partic- tion over a three-week period to range from
ipation points or other incentive for completing 0.76 to 0.87 for narcissism, psychopathy, and
the survey. It was composed of 234 MBA stu- Machiavellianism. Results from our supplemen-
dents, with an average age of 29.6 (S.D. 5 6.61) tary sample found the measure to produce simi-
and approximately twice as many males lar temporal stability, with test-retest correlations
(n 5 160) than females (n 5 74). Seventy-four over a four-week period ranging from 0.69 to
percent of participants identified as Caucasian 0.84 for narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavel-
(n 5 173), 23 percent had started a venture lianism. The internal consistency of the measure
(n 5 53), 24 percent had worked for a startup was found to be acceptable in both of our pri-
(n 5 55), and 23 percent had taken a course in mary data collections, with Cronbach’s coeffi-
entrepreneurship (n 5 53). cient alpha (a) scores of 0.76 and 0.73 for
A supplemental data collection was con- narcissism, 0.71 and 0.73 for psychopathy, and
ducted in order to evaluate validity for the meas- 0.82 and 0.79 for Machiavellianism for the pri-
ures of entrepreneurial motives, including the mary samples of business undergraduates and
test-retest reliabilities (temporal stability) of the MBAs, respectively.
focal study measures. The protocol used for this Entrepreneurial Intentions. This construct
sample was similar to the others. However, was examined using a 5-item measure developed
these participants were surveyed twice, at time by Chen, Greene, and Crick (1998). Example
1 (t1) and then four weeks later at time 2 (t2). In items include “I have begun planning for starting
order to link a particular subject’s test-retest my own business,” and “I am going to set up my
responses, a four-digit identification number own business in the near future.” Responses to
was used (matching t1 to t2). This supplemental items were averaged, such that higher scores
sample was composed of 130 business under- represent greater levels of entrepreneurial inten-
graduates at t1, with 118 of those participants tions. Results from the supplementary sample
also completing t2. These participants had an found the measure to produce good temporal
average age of 21.0 (S.D. 5 1.85), and included stability, with a test–retest correlation of 0.88
slightly more males (nt1 5 80; nt2 5 72) than over a four-week period. The measure produced
females (nt1 5 50; nt2 5 46). The majority of par- high levels of internal consistency, with
ticipants identified as Caucasian (nt1 5 110; Cronbach’s coefficient alpha (a) scores of 0.92
nt2 5 100), 13 percent had started a venture and 0.93 for the primary samples of business
(nt1 5 17; nt2 5 15), 29 percent had worked for undergraduates and MBAs, respectively.
a startup (nt1 5 38; nt2 5 34), and approximately Entrepreneurial Motives. Measures were
30 percent had taken a course in entrepreneur- developed by the authors based on the general
ship (nt1 5 42; nt2 5 34). theoretical consideration of unproductive and
productive entrepreneurship made by Baumol
Measures (1990) and input from other entrepreneurship
Unless otherwise noted, all measures were scholars. Consistent with prior individual-level
rated on a five-point Likert-type response scale, research and recent calls to examine individual
with responses ranging from 1 (strongly dis- entrepreneurial motives (Carsrud and
agree) to 5 (strongly agree). Br€annback 2011), we focused on individuals’
Dark Triad. The dark triad was examined motivation to pursue both unproductive (i.e.
using a 12-item measure developed and validated value-appropriating) and productive (i.e., value-
by Jonason and Webster (2010). It includes 4 creating) entrepreneurial activities. The meas-
items each for narcissism (example item: “I tend ures consist of five items for unproductive
to want others to admire me”), psychopathy entrepreneurial motives and five items for pro-
(example item: “I tend to lack remorse”) and ductive entrepreneurial motives. The items and

14 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


results of principal components confirmatory consider starting a new business (Kautonen, Gel-
factor analysis using Varimax rotation are deren and Fink 2015). Males have historically
included in Appendix. Item responses were been found to be more likely to start businesses
averaged, such that higher scores represent than females (Gupta, Turban, and Bhawe 2008;
greater levels of unproductive and productive Zhao, Seibert, and Hills 2005). Ethnic minorities
entrepreneurial motives, respectively. Results tend to have a high rate of startup intentions
from our supplementary sample found the due to perceptions of having a decreased oppor-
measures to produce reasonable temporal stabil- tunity to find employment and make career
ity, with test-retest correlations over a four-week advancements through traditional employment
period of 0.75 for each type of entrepreneurial routes (Fairlie 2004). Individuals who are mar-
motive. The measures also showed reasonable ried and who have children are likely to have
levels of internal consistency, with Cronbach’s lower entrepreneurial intentions due to the risk
coefficient alpha (a) scores of 0.76 and 0.75 for involved in the startup process (Schiller and
unproductive entrepreneurial motives, and 0.79 Crewson 1997). Those who have taken an entre-
and 0.84 for productive entrepreneurial motives, preneurship course are more likely to start a
in the primary samples of business undergradu- new business due to their enhanced knowledge
ates and MBAs, respectively. The measures also and motivation for partaking in entrepreneurial
appeared to show reasonable convergent and activities (Piperopoulos and Dimov 2015).
discriminate validity.5 Finally, students from private schools may, on
Control Variables. Several demographic con- average, have greater access to capital than stu-
trol variables were used in the current research. dents from public schools, thus enhancing the
These included the age, and a series of dummy perceived feasibility of starting a business.
variables (yes 5 1) indicating if a subject was
male, Caucasian, married, had children, had pre- Statistical Procedures
viously taken an entrepreneurship course, and Regression analysis was used to examine all
was enrolled at a private university. Age was hypotheses (Cohen et al. 2003). Two models are
included because older students generally have presented for each hypothesis. In each case,
more work experience and broader connections Model 1 corresponds to Sample 1 (business
that could afford them a better position to undergraduates) and Model 2 corresponds to

5
Convergent validity for the measures of entrepreneurial motives was first examined by considering the
degree to which items within their respective factors were highly correlated across the combined primary sam-
ples (i.e., as a general rule factor loadings should be greater than 0.4; Tabachnick and Fidell 2001). As shown in
Appendix, factor loadings ranged from 0.61 to 0.79 for unproductive entrepreneurial motives (Mean factor
loading 5 0.70) and from 0.61 to 0.86 for productive entrepreneurial motives (Mean factor loading 5 0.73). These
results support the convergent validity of the measures. Second, convergent validity was more broadly investi-
gated by comparing the association of the measures with a number of scales included in the supplementary data
collection that were expected to be theoretically related to unproductive and productive entrepreneurial motives.
Specifically, the measures used for comparison included the negative and positive reciprocity scales developed
by Perugini et al. (2003) to examine personal norms of reciprocity, as well as the profit-seeking and fairness
scales developed by Leybman, Zuroff, and Fournier (2011) to examine social exchange styles. In support of con-
vergent validity: the measure of unproductive entrepreneurial motives was positively correlated with negative
reciprocity (r 5 0.26, p < 0.01) and with profit-seeking in social exchange (r 5 0.27, p < 0.01); additionally, the
measure of productive entrepreneurial motives was positive correlated with positive reciprocity (r 5 0.31,
p < 0.01) and with fairness in social exchange (r 5 0.21, p < 0.05). These results provide additional support for
the convergent validity of the measures.
Discriminant validity was evaluated in two ways. First, we considered the between-factor loading of items for
the unproductive and productive measures, which should differ by 0.2 or more from within-factor loadings (Hair
et al. 2010). The differences in the absolute values of the between-factor loadings ranged from 0.35 to 0.65 for
unproductive entrepreneurial motives (mean difference in between-factor loadings 5 0.56) and from 0.42 to 0.75
for productive entrepreneurial motives (mean difference in between-factor loadings 5 0.61). Second, we eval-
uated the correlations between the items for the two measures (no correlations should be greater than 0.7, since
this level of association would indicate a majority of shared variance). The absolute values of the correlations
ranged from 0.04 to 0.32, with an average correlation of 0.15. Thus, the measures appear to demonstrate reasona-
ble discriminant validity.

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 15


Sample 2 (MBAs). Due to recent arguments that were anonymous, thus eliminating any reason to
control variables can distort relationships misrepresent one’s motives and reducing the
between focal variables of interest (Becker likelihood of social desirability bias.
2005; Lance and Vandenberg 2009; Schjoedt The above points notwithstanding, two steps
and Bird 2014; Spector and Brannick 2011), the were taken to evaluate the threat of common
hypotheses were evaluated both with and with- method variance and social desirability bias.
out controls. Since there was no change in the First, we assessed the amount of variance
statistical significance or pattern of results accounted for by loading all of the items from
according to whether the controls were the study’s focal variables onto a single factor.
included, the full models including the controls This accounted for 20.44 percent of the variance
are presented and used to test the study hypoth- for the undergraduate sample and 21.42 percent
eses. All independent variables were mean- of the variance for the MBA sample, which in
centered in order to reduce the potential for each case is well below the 50 percent threshold
multicollinearity. The change in R-squared is that is needed to be a distinct construct. Second,
presented for each model in terms of additional as part of our supplemental data collection
variance accounted for by the main effects (n 5 130), we examined the relationship of our
above and beyond the control variables. dependent variables with a 5-item measure of
The first hypothesis, regarding entrepreneur- social desirability (a 5 0.75) developed by Hays,
ial intentions was examined using the full sam- Hayashi, and Stewart (1989). To do so, we
ples of 508 business undergraduates and 234 regressed entrepreneurial intentions (B 5 20.06,
MBAs. The remaining hypotheses, regarding p > .10), unproductive entrepreneurial motives
unproductive and productive entrepreneurial (B 5 0.13, p > .10), and productive entrepreneur-
motives, were examined using the respective ial motives (B 5 20.17, p > .10) onto the measure
subsets of 281 business undergraduates and 145 of social desirability. Each of the regression coef-
MBAs high in entrepreneurial intentions (i.e., ficients was nonsignificant, indicating that none
those scoring an average of 3 or higher; that is of the focal dependent variables (i.e., entrepre-
endorsing the statements, and thus effectively neurial intentions, unproductive entrepreneurial
qualifying as early-stage nascent entrepreneurs; motives, productive entrepreneurial motives) are
Reynolds and White 1997). This approach was significant predictors of social desirability. Taken
used because it was considered most appropri- together, the above noted evidence suggests that
ate to examine the entrepreneurial motives of neither common method variance nor social
only those persons who showed behavioral desirability biases threatened our ability to test
intentions toward starting a new venture (i.e., the study’s hypotheses.
those involved in the nascent stages of planning
to launch a business). Correlation tables for the
full samples and the entrepreneurial subsamples Results
are presented as Tables 1 and 3. Tables 2, 4, Prior to our hypothesis testing, we first con-
and 5 present the regression results for H1–H3. sider the potential threat of multicollinearity.
The possibility of multicollinearity was exam-
Common Method Variance Analyses and ined using variance inflation factor (VIF) and
Social Desirability Bias conditional index (CI) scores for models used to
Multiple steps were taken as part of the test each of the hypotheses. The largest VIF
research design to reduce the threat of common score across both samples was 1.42, which is
method variance and social desirability. First, the well below the cutoff value of 10 that is consid-
dependent variable of entrepreneurial intentions ered to be a problematic (Neter et al. 1996). In
involved nonsubjective behavior—specifically, addition, the highest CI score across both sam-
whether a participant had begun the entrepre- ples was 1.86, which is well below the value of
neurial process (e.g., has started business plan- 30 that is seen as an indicator of multicollinear-
ning). The dependent variables regarding an ity (Tabachnick and Fidell 2001). Accordingly,
individual’s motives for starting the entrepreneur- multicollinearity does not appear to be a threat
ial process, are necessarily within the purview of to our hypothesis testing. We now turn to the
the individual—that is, an individual’s motives formal tests of our hypotheses.
are an intraindividual phenomenon. As a result, H1 predicted that individuals’ levels of narcis-
the actors in question are necessarily informants sism (H1a), psychopathy (H1b), Machiavellian-
of this variable. Second, participant responses ism (H1c) would be positively associated with

16 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Variable Intercorrelations for Full Samples
Undergraduates (N 5 508)

Variable Mean S.D.a 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Age 21.52 3.22


Sex 0.58 0.49 0.00
Caucasian 0.81 0.39 0.01 0.02
Married 0.05 0.22 0.51** 0.03 0.04
Children 0.03 0.16 0.47** 20.03 0.02 0.46**
Entrepreneurship 0.36 0.48 0.15** 0.08 20.08 20.02 0.03
course
Private school 0.70 0.46 0.27** 20.10* 20.01 0.11* 0.08 0.40**
Narcissism 3.50 0.76 2.09* 0.07 0.03 20.01 20.05 0.02 20.06
Psychopathy 2.14 0.75 0.05 0.28** 20.03 20.01 0.02 20.03 20.15** 0.16**
Machiavellianism 2.42 0.88 20.07 0.28** 0.02 20.08 20.10* 20.01 20.14** .029** 0.50**
Entrepreneurial 3.11 1.23 0.12** 0.19** 20.16** 0.08 0.03 0.27** 0.09* 0.09* 20.02 20.01
intentions

MBAs (N 5 234)

Variable Mean S.D.a 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

HMIELESKI AND LERNER


Age 29.62 6.61
Sex 0.68 0.47 20.03
Caucasian 0.74 0.44 20.11 0.08
Married 0.42 0.49 0.34** 0.00 0.01
Children 0.24 0.43 0.57** 2.03 0.06 0.50**
Entrepreneurship 0.23 0.42 20.10 20.01 20.03 20.17** 20.09
course
Private school 0.79 0.41 0.25** 0.16* 20.00 0.15* 0.15* 20.17*
Narcissism 3.09 0.75 20.14* 0.20** 0.08 20.04 20.17** 20.05 0.09
Psychopathy 2.10 0.81 20.20** 0.24** 0.17** 20.13* 20.15* 0.02 20.04 0.18**
Machiavellianism 2.30 0.87 20.14* 0.17* 0.17* 20.16* 20.12 0.07 20.07 0.32** 0.48**
Entrepreneurial 3.31 1.21 0.05 0.18** 20.03 0.05 0.07 0.11 20.02 0.16** 0.11 0.07
intentions

a
S.D., standard deviation.

17
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
Table 2
Regression Models of Entrepreneurial Intentions for Full Samples
Entrepreneurial Intentions

Undergraduates MBAs
(N 5 508) (N 5 234)

Model 1 Model 2
Variable B (SEa) B (SE)

Control variables
Age 0.03 (0.02) 0.01 (0.02)
Sex 0.49** (0.11) 0.41* (0.18)
Caucasian 20.46** (0.13) 20.16 (0.18)
Married 0.29 (0.29) 0.06 (0.19)
Children 20.20 (0.37) 0.25 (0.25)
Entrepreneurship course 0.60** (0.12) 0.35 (0.19)
Private school 20.05 (0.13) 20.19 (0.20)

Main effects
Narcissism 0.17* (0.07) 0.26* (.11)
Psychopathy 20.11 (0.08) 0.13 (.11)
Machiavellianism 20.07 (0.07) 20.05 (.11)

F-Ratio 8.45** 2.10*


R2 0.15 0.09
F-Change 2.95* 2.24
DR2 0.02 0.03

a
S.E., standard error.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.

their intentions to start a new venture. As shown (BMBA 5 0.00, p > .10); as predicted, psychopa-
in Models 1 and 2 of Table 2, in both samples, thy (Bugrad 5 0.25, p < .01; BMBA 5 0.17, p < .05)
narcissism was found to have a significant posi- and Machiavellianism (Bugrad 5 0.19, p < .01;
tive relationship with entrepreneurial intentions BMBA 5 0.23, p < .01) were significantly linked
(Bugrad 5 0.17, p < .05; BMBA 5 0.26, p < .05), to higher unproductive entrepreneurial motives
whereas psychopathy (Bugrad 5 20.11, p > .10; across both samples. Overall, these results offer
BMBA 5 0.13, p > .10) and Machiavellianism partial support for H2a, and full support for
(Bugrad 5 20.07, p > .10; BMBA 5 20.05, p > .10) H2b and H2c.
were not significantly related to entrepreneurial H3 predicted that individuals’ levels of narcis-
intentions. Overall, these results support H1a, sism (H3a), psychopathy (H3b), and Machiavel-
and do not support H1b or H1c. lianism (H3c) would be negatively associated
H2 predicted that individuals’ levels of narcis- with their level of productive entrepreneurial
sism (H2a), psychopathy (H2b), and Machiavel- motives. As shown in Models 1 and 2 of Table
lianism (H2c) would be positively associated 5: narcissism was significant but positive in the
with their level of unproductive entrepreneurial undergraduate sample (Bugrad 5 0.10, p < .05)
motives. As shown in Models 1 and 2 of Table and nonsignificant in the MBA sample (BMBA 5
4, narcissism was indeed positive and significant 0.03, p > .10); psychopathy, as expected, had a
in the undergraduate sample (Bugrad 5 0.14, significant negative relationship with productive
p < .05), and nonsignificant in the MBA sample entrepreneurial motives in both samples

18 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


Table 3
Descriptive Statistics and Variable Intercorrelations for Subsamples of Early-Stage Nascent
Entrepreneurs
Undergraduates (n 5 281)

Variable Mean S.D.a 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Age 21.75 3.43


2. Sex 0.66 0.48 20.04
3. Caucasian 0.77 0.43 0.01 0.10
4. Married 0.01 0.24 0.51** 20.01 0.07
5. Children 0.03 0.18 0.42** 20.04 0.01 0.46**
6. Entrepreneurship course 0.44 0.50 0.13* 0.08 20.12 20.02 0.04
20.05

HMIELESKI AND LERNER


7. Private school 0.72 0.45 0.28** 20.11 0.09 0.07 0.46**
8. Narcissism 3.57 0.74 20.10 0.03 0.02 0.03 20.09 20.01 20.05
9. Psychopathy 2.16 0.77 0.01 0.24** 0.01 20.01 0.02 20.06 20.18** 0.09
10. Machiavellianism 2.44 0.80 20.03 0.25** 0.02 20.08 20.07 20.10 20.23** 0.18** 0.41**
11. Unproductive 2.55 0.75 20.05 0.14* 20.12* 20.08 20.10 20.09 20.02 0.20** 0.34** 0.33**
entrepreneurial motives
12. Productive 4.37 0.56 20.20** 20.15* 20.12* 20.16** 0.01 0.12 0.04 0.12 20.26** 20.06 20.18**
entrepreneurial motives

19
20
Table 3
Continued

MBAs (n 5 145)

Variable Mean S.D.a 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Age 29.75 6.66


2. Sex 0.75 0.43 20.05
3. Caucasian 0.75 0.43 20.15 0.11
4. Married 0.43 0.50 0.28** 20.02 0.05
5. Children 0.26 0.44 0.56** 20.03 0.08 0.49**
6. Entrepreneurship course 0.26 0.44 20.09 0.04 20.10 20.22** 20.13
7. Private school 0.79 0.41 0.28** 0.02 0.02 0.20* 0.14 20.13
8. Narcissism 3.18 0.76 20.17* 0.14 0.10 0.02 20.11 20.07 0.07
9. Psychopathy 2.17 0.83 20.21** 0.15 0.20* 20.11 20.14 0.02 20.10 0.22**
10. Machiavellianism 2.34 0.90 220.17* 0.15 0.16* 20.16* 20.09 0.06 20.16 0.31** 0.50**
11. Unproductive 2.36 0.75 20.12 0.19* 0.04 20.01 20.03 20.12 0.05 0.17* 0.33** 0.36**
entrepreneurial motives
12. Productive 4.12 0.73 0.00 20.22** 20.25** 20.02 20.11 0.18* 20.15 20.10 20.32** 20.23** 20.47**
entrepreneurial motives

JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


a
S.D., standard deviation.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
Table 4
Regression Models of Unproductive Entrepreneurial Motives for
Subsamples of Early-Stage Nascent Entrepreneurs
Unproductive Entrepreneurial Motives

Undergraduates MBAs
(n 5 281) (n 5 145)

Model 1 Model 2
Variable B (SE a) B (SE)

Control variables
Age 20.00 (0.02) 20.02 (0.01)
Sex 0.11 (0.09) 0.22 (0.14)
Caucasian 20.25** (0.10) 20.17 (0.14)
Married 20.11 (0.21) 0.03 (0.14)
Children 20.24 (0.27) 0.11 (0.18)
Entrepreneurship course 20.21* (0.09) 20.24 (0.14)
Private school 0.27* (0.11) 0.22 (0.15)

Main effects
Narcissism 0.14* (0.06) 0.00 (0.08)
Psychopathy 0.25** (0.06) 0.17* (0.08)
Machiavellianism 0.19** (0.05) 0.23** (0.08)

F-Ratio 7.73** 3.80**


R2 0.22 0.20
F-Change 18.23** 8.44**
DR2 0.16 0.15

a
S.E., standard error.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.

(Bugrad 520.19, p < .01; BMBA 5 2.22, p < .01); intentions. With regard to participants high in
and Machiavellianism showed a nonsignificant rela- entrepreneurial intentions, those at the begin-
tionship with productive entrepreneurial motives ning of the entrepreneurial process and thus
in both samples (Bugrad 5 0.04, p > .10; BMBA 5 qualifying as early-stage nascent entrepreneurs,
20.08, p > .10). Accordingly, the results fully sup- the results indicate a general pattern in which
port H3b, and do not support H3a and H3c. psychopathy and Machiavellianism were posi-
tively associated with unproductive entrepre-
Discussion neurial motives, and psychopathy was
This study sought to examine dark psycho- negatively related to productive entrepreneurial
logical predictors of entrepreneurial intentions motives. We now discuss the results in greater
and associated motives for engaging in nascent detail, followed by implications, limitations and
entrepreneurial activity. Overall, the findings future directions, and concluding thoughts.
demonstrate a consistent pattern in which nar-
cissism was positively related to entrepreneurial The Dark Triad and Entrepreneurial
intentions, and psychopathy and Machiavellian- Intentions
ism being neither over- nor under-represented Drawing from life history theory, we antici-
characteristics in those with entrepreneurial pated that the propensity for individuals high

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 21


Table 5
Regression Models of Productive Entrepreneurial Motives for
Subsamples of Early-Stage Nascent Entrepreneurs
Productive Entrepreneurial Motives

Undergraduates MBAs
(n 5 281) (n 5 145)

Model 1 Model 2
Variable B (SE a) B (SE)

Control variables
Age 20.03** (0.01) 0.00 (0.01)
Sex 20.13 (0.07) 20.27* (0.13)
Caucasian 20.12 (0.07) 20.22 (0.13)
Married 20.28 (0.16) 0.11 (0.13)
Children 0.52* (0.20) 20.26 (0.17)
Entrepreneurship course 0.14* (0.07) 0.27* (0.13)
Private school 20.01 (0.08) 20.30* (0.15)

Main effects
Narcissism 0.10* (0.04) 0.03 (.08)
Psychopathy 20.19** (0.05) 20.22** (0.08)
Machiavellianism 0.04 (0.04) 20.08 (0.07)

F-Ratio 6.06** 4.29**


R2 0.18 0.24
F-Change 7.44** 5.09**
DR2 0.07 0.09

a
S.E., standard error.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.

in the dark triad to follow a “fast life” behav- variance in entrepreneurial intentions above-
ioral strategy would result in such persons and-beyond (controlling for) psychopathy and
having higher intentions to become entrepre- Machiavellianism.
neurs. However, our results did not find each As noted, psychopathy and Machiavellianism
facet of the dark triad to individually predict were not found to be significant individual pre-
entrepreneurial intentions. Narcissism was the dictors of entrepreneurial intentions. The lack of
only individual facet of the dark triad that had a relationship for psychopathy and Machiavel-
a significant relationship with entrepreneurial lianism with entrepreneurial intentions could
intentions across both samples. This finding theoretically be explained by subtle differences
replicates other research demonstrating a pos- between the dark triad characteristics. Whereas
itive linkage of narcissism with entrepreneur- narcissism inflates self-perceptions of ability and
ial intentions (Mathieu and St-Jean 2013). Our makes attention and admiration an ends in
results extend previous findings by providing itself, this is not the case for psychopathy or
evidence of this relationship within the con- Machiavellianism. Absent narcissistic beliefs or
text of the full dark triad; that is, we find that delusions of grandeur, it takes considerable
narcissism uniquely explains significant work to start a venture—and entrepreneurial

22 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


success is inherently uncertain. Thus, it may be with unproductive entrepreneurial motives in the
at least as preferable for individuals high in psy- MBA sample: r 5 0.17, p < .05; the relationship
chopathy or Machiavellianism to join existing was nonsignificant only after controlling for psy-
ventures or organizations, where there are chopathy and Machiavellianism). Thus, consistent
already resources and rents to appropriate, and with a value appropriating approach to relation-
other individuals (e.g., coworkers) to be used ships and broadly toward life, the dark triad—
for unreciprocated social exchange. The under- and particularly the darkest traits in the triad (i.e.,
lying (over)confidence and attention-seeking psychopathy and Machiavellianism; Rauthmann
characteristics associated with narcissism may and Kolar 2012)—were strongly linked with an
partly explain why it was related to entrepre- unproductive approach toward entrepreneurship.
neurial intentions while psychopathy and In terms of productive entrepreneurial
Machiavellianism were not. Although psychop- motives, psychopathy was the only consistent
athy’s emotional callousness and Machiavellian- predictor across both samples—operating in
ism’s capacity for manipulation may be useful to alignment with our general expectations in
an individual once entering entrepreneurship, it terms of the dark triad having a negative rela-
appears they do not significantly relate to entre- tionship with such motives. Considering that a
preneurial intentions. In addition, it should be callous lack of emotion and wanton disregard
underscored that neither psychopathy nor for others are the most defining markers of psy-
Machiavellianism had a significant negative rela- chopathy (Dutton 2012), it is not surprising that
tionship with entrepreneurial intentions. This this facet of the dark triad stood apart as being
suggests that these characteristics are neither the characteristic that was most negatively
over- nor under-represented in those with entre- related to productive entrepreneurial motives.
preneurial intentions. In other words, early- This is consistent with the general callous nature
stage nascent entrepreneurs may, on average, of such persons and research indicating a nega-
be a bit more narcissistic than their peers, but tive relationship of psychopathy with social-
are not distinguishably higher or lower in Mach- oriented entrepreneurship (Akhtar, Ahmetoglu,
iavellian and psychopathy than business- and Chamorro-Premuzic 2013).
oriented peers. In contrast, within the undergraduate sample,
narcissism was found to have a positive relation-
The Dark Triad and Unproductive versus ship with productive entrepreneurial motives.
Productive Entrepreneurial Motives This might be explained by the narcissistic
For early-stage nascent entrepreneurs (i.e., desire for attention and admiration, considering
those endorsing high entrepreneurial intentions, the social appeal of productive value creation—
including having begun business planning), we particularly among idealistic undergraduates
applied social exchange theory to predict that who are currently part of a generation that has
the dark triad would be positively related to grown up with models of prosocial-oriented
unproductive entrepreneurial motives and nega- startups being viewed as cool by their peers
tively related to productive entrepreneurial (Mycoskie 2012).
motives. In so doing, we suggested that the In terms of the other nonsignificant finding,
entrepreneurial motives of those higher in the individuals who are high in Machiavellianism
various facets of the dark triad would be charac- may not be motivated by value creation for any-
terized by value appropriation rather than value one other than themselves, as admiration is not
creation. a valued end for such persons; yet being per-
The findings yielded results generally consist- ceived as a socially oriented entrepreneur might
ent with predictions for unproductive entre- facilitate their duplicitous theater, offering an
preneurial motives, and were mixed for pro- explanation for why such individuals do not
ductive entrepreneurial motives. Psychopathy and demonstrate negative inclinations toward pro-
Machiavellianism were found to each have a posi- ductive orientations.
tive relationship with unproductive entrepreneur-
ial motives across both samples, whereas the Implications for Entrepreneurship
relationship of narcissism with unproductive Theory
entrepreneurial motives was positive and signifi- The entrepreneurship literature on individual
cant for the undergraduate sample but nonsignifi- difference characteristics has been dominated
cant for the MBA sample (narcissism did, by positive traits, particularly with respect to
however, have a significant positive correlation those linked with entrepreneurial intentions and

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 23


motives (Antoncic et al. 2015; Zhao, Seibert, and creation, let alone survival and performance,
Lumpkin 2010). The current research set out to typically requires a long-term view and focused
alternatively consider potential negative or dark persistence (Baron, Franklin, and Hmieleski
individual difference characteristics. Our motiva- 2016; Hmieleski, Corbett, and Baron 2013)—
tion was to examine potential unproductive which may be at odds with key aspects of the
(i.e., value-appropriating) and productive (i.e., dark triad. Thus, similar to other double-edged
value-creating) drivers of individuals’ interest characteristics such as optimism and positive
starting a new venture. The reason being that it affect (Baron, Hmieleski, and Henry 2012;
is arguably as important to identify unproduc- Baron, Tang and Hmieleski 2011; Hmieleski
tive drivers of entrepreneurship as it is to iden- and Baron 2009) or behavioral disinhibition
tify productive drivers, so that we not only (Lerner 2016), it is possible that aspects of the
know what to promote, but also what should be dark triad could be positively related to interest
re-channeled or discouraged (Shepherd 2015). and entry into entrepreneurship, while—at high
While the vast majority of existing theory on levels—negatively related to sustained perform-
entrepreneurial entry and associated policy pre- ance in developing and leading new ventures.
sumes value-creating entrepreneurship, our Future research is needed to explicitly test these
results suggest that some degree of caution and other questions relating to the double-
should be taken. For example, individuals high edged nature of personality characteristics
in psychopathy and/or Machiavellianism were (DeNisi 2015; Klotz and Neubaum 2016).
neither more nor less likely to possess entrepre-
neurial intentions than others; however, among Implications for Education, Policy, and
such persons high in psychopathy and/or Practice
Machiavellianism with entrepreneurial inten- Our findings offer several potential implica-
tions, when entering this vocation, they are tions for education, policy, and practice. In
likely to do so with unproductive motives aimed terms of education, it appears that business
at appropriating value rather than creating it. school faculty may need to become increasingly
The findings also surface a potentially accustom to interacting with students high on
counter-intuitive upside of narcissism among the dark triad personality characteristics—as
undergraduates, an individual characteristic on these dispositions are on the rise among this
the rise within this population (Twenge et al.
population (Bergman, Westerman, and Daly
2008). The significant positive relationship of
2010; Webster and Harmon 2002). At first
narcissism with entrepreneurial intentions, and
thought, this fact may seem unfortunate and
moreover with value-creating entrepreneurial
even daunting; yet in terms of entrepreneurship
motives, suggests that this otherwise egocentric
there may be a silver lining. Such students may
characteristic holds productive potential and
have less fear than typical students in terms of
could be channeled for good. Considering
increasing levels of narcissism in society and launching ventures and may be naturally skilled
among undergraduate students in particular at acquiring resources and bootstrapping
(Twenge and Campbell 2010), if properly (Jonason, Koenig, and Tost 2010; Jonason, Li,
directed, it may be a psychological resource and Teicher 2010). The self-centered nature of
instrumental for entry into productive entrepre- individuals who are high on facets of the dark
neurship in the face of long odds. Future triad, particularly those who are high on narcis-
research is necessary to further examine “how sism, however, creates the need to move stu-
much” narcissism may be beneficial, as well as dents’ mindsets from “I” and “me” (which has
how it can be most constructively channeled. become the norm for students due to the rise in
Similarly, future research is needed to deter- social media; Twenge and Campbell 2010) to
mine the interplay and effects of the dark triad “we” and “us.” This point is of crucial impor-
on new venture survival and performance. On tance considering the need for entrepreneurs to
one hand, it reasons that having “just enough” build teams and lead employees (Hmieleski,
of the dark triad characteristics may be helpful Cole, and Baron 2012; Klotz et al. 2014). The
(or adaptive) with respect to certain aspects of development of self-regulation skills will be
entrepreneurship such as resource mobilization, especially important for business school stu-
resistance to stress, and even innovation (Jona- dents high in the dark triad, such that they may
son, Koenig, and Tost 2010; Jonason, Li, and be able to capitalize on its adaptive aspects
Teicher 2010). Yet on the other hand, venture while avoiding its socially counter-productive

24 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


downside that could diminish the chances of nonpecuniary awards (e.g., free enrollment in
achieving long-term success. training programs and mentorships, aversive to
In terms of practice, it might be important the narcissist) and distribute prize money over a
for investors who are betting on mid- to long- period of time; and press-releases and ceremo-
term returns from startups to be weary of those nies could present the award to the startup
that are led by entrepreneurs who possess psy- team (versus the entrepreneur) ideally alongside
chopathic, and to a lesser extent Machiavellian, the customers/stakeholders that the venture
characteristics. Such individuals appear to be seeks to serve.
motivated by appropriative strategies that might
be advantageous for producing short-term Limitations and Future Directions
results, but could become less viable in the We now address a few limitations of the cur-
long-run if their self-interested behavior eventu- rent research, and offer some suggestions for
ally tarnishes their reputation. Thus, problems future directions. First, it could be questioned as
of agency are likely to be heightened when to whether our findings generalize to older or
dealing with such persons–requiring tightly more experienced business professionals. For
aligning interests, appropriate organizational research examining predictors of entrepreneur-
policies and procedures, and independent exter- ial intentions, business undergraduate and MBA
nal oversight. In contrast, those high in narcis- samples are seemingly at least as appropriate as
sism may be partly driven by a productive other populations such as full-time employees.
orientation that could be effective at producing Many individuals begin the entrepreneurial pro-
long-term results if properly channeled. Impor- cess while in college, and most business under-
tantly, consideration of the dark triad (and asso- graduates and MBA students are on the verge of
ciated motives) by investors when evaluating making vocational choices—to become
aspiring entrepreneurs should not preclude (co)founders, employees of a startup firm, or
evaluation of other factors that have been previ- employees of existing organizations. Addition-
ously identified as predictors of individuals’ ally, the increase of dark triad characteristics
potential for creating profitable new ventures. among college students made this an interesting
Rather, we suggest that traditional factors (e.g., population for examining the study’s research
entrepreneurial experience) should be consid- question. It is also important to note that a num-
ered hand-in-hand with the underlying disposi- ber of participants in our study did have some
tions and motives of nascent entrepreneurs so degree of entrepreneurial experience. For exam-
as to maximize both financial performance and ple, many had been employed by a startup
overall value creation. (nugrad 5 150, nMBA 5 55) and several had previ-
From a policy perspective, it may prove use- ously launched a new venture (nugrad 5 71,
ful for grant programs intending to foster entre- nMBA 5 53). It would, however, be interesting
preneurship to be strategic in terms of focusing for future research to examine similar research
toward the communal aspects of venture crea- questions using samples of participants that rep-
tion and away from a more celebrity-oriented resent a broader range of life stages.
view of entrepreneurship. Government agencies Another potential limitation is that our
and universities that award grants and prize research used a cross-sectional design. Our
money to startup ventures should be careful to approach in this regard seemed warranted con-
avoid “fast-life” entrepreneurs high in the dark sidering that there is considerable evidence that
triad (particularly, psychopathy and Machiavel- intentions are a strong predictor of future
lianism), because such persons may be likely to actions (Ajzen 1991), entrepreneurship is an
recklessly run through resources quickly with- agentic activity that cannot begin without inten-
out providing any long-term societal benefits. In tions (Krueger, Reilly, and Carsrud 2000), and
practice, very simple adjustments to the design because we examined entrepreneurial intentions
of competitions and awards could potentially from a behavioral perspective that evaluated the
help. For example: independent third-party eval- extent to which actual steps were taken toward
uations (e.g., customers) of the entrepreneur/ the launch of a new venture (e.g., whether the
startup could be included as part of judging cri- participant had considered the type of business
teria as well as interviews with startup subordi- to be started, whether the participant had begun
nates (looking for evidence of abusive or other planning to start a business). Nonetheless, it
nondevelopmental interactions); awards could would be useful for future studies to track the
deemphasize cash prizes and instead focus on extent to which participants’ dark triad

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 25


characteristics are associated with the actual Lumpkin 2010) leaves open the related question
launching of new ventures. of whether such characteristics might be more
Considering that the study’s measures were prevalent among later-stage entrepreneurs. There
gathered from a single source, there is a possibil- are a few plausible reasons why the dark triad
ity that our results could be partly influenced by may indeed be higher among later-stage entre-
common method variance and social desirability preneurs than incipient nascent entrepreneurs.
bias. Our findings do, however, appear robust— First, the uncertain and challenging nature of
based on the fact that the they held even after the entrepreneurial environment may activate
including a range of controls, were generally dark triad traits within individuals, thus
consistent across two different samples, and that enhancing or further developing these charac-
the results did not appear to be biased by social teristics within individuals who possess such
desirability (according to our supplemental data dispositions (much like individuals born with
collection and analysis). Despite these facts, the genetic characteristics for athleticism become
opportunity remains for future research to use fitter more quickly and to higher levels when
other designs and sources of data. exercising than persons not possessing such
The fact that we used a relatively short mea- characteristics). Another possibility is that dark
sure of the dark triad (the Dirty Dozen; Jonason triad characteristics may be advantageous in
and Webster 2010) could be viewed as another helping to survive the brutish conditions and
limitation of the current research. We selected challenges associated with early-stage ventur-
this measure for use because it is concise, has ing. In such case, the proportion of entrepre-
good psychometric properties, and contains neurs who are high in the dark triad might
items that are worded in such a way as to be rela- increase among those in the later stages of the
tively inoffensive as compared to other estab- nascent venturing process, as individuals low
lished measures of the dark triad. These factors in the dark triad may (initially) be dispropor-
are even more important when considering tionally selected out (or exit from) the popula-
measures for studies involving full-time founders, tion. In the long-run, however, such a trend
as other longer and more intrusive measures may would likely reverse, as the short-term (fast-
be less practical for use among such populations. life) orientation of those high in the dark triad
With these advantages stated, it should also be is likely to be counter-productive for the long-
noted that a downside of using Dirty Dozen is term growth and sustainability of firms. These
that its brevity tends to limit its predictive power conjectures represent potentially interesting
as compared to other lengthier measures of the opportunities for future research.
dark triad (Miller et al. 2012). As an alternative to We should also clarify that life history theory
the Dirty Dozen, the 27-item Short Dark Triad and social exchange theory were applied in the
(SD3) measure recently developed by Jones and current research due to the natural linkages
Paulhus (2014) is another option for entrepre- between these theories, their appropriateness
neurship researchers to consider. with examining entrepreneurial intentions and
It is also important to underscore that our motives, and because these theories have argu-
development of measures for unproductive and ably been the most commonly used frameworks
productive entrepreneurial intentions is only applied for the study of the dark triad (Jonason,
intended to be a first step at assessing these con- Koenig, and Tost 2010; O’Boyle et al. 2012).
structs. As such, we encourage researchers to With this said, other theories may also be appro-
develop more in-depth scales to measure these, priate for examining the dark triad within the
as well as other types, of entrepreneurial context of new venture creation and develop-
motives. The development of such measures ment. For example, person-job fit theory and
will be critical to build a more extensive base of contingency theories might be particularly useful
knowledge regarding why individuals intend to frameworks for examining whether the dark
engage in entrepreneurial activity (Carsrud and triad is an adaptive set of personality characteris-
Br€annback 2011; Shepherd 2015). tics for entrepreneurs in general (Kristof-Brown,
Next, the fact we did not observe the dark Zimmerman, and Johnson 2005; Markman and
triad to explain a great deal of variance in entre- Baron 2003) and within certain entrepreneurial
preneurial intentions within the current study contexts in particular (Hmieleski and Baron
(albeit not much less than other dispositional 2008; Hmieleski, Carr, and Baron 2015). More
predictors of entrepreneurial intentions reported generally, future research involving the dark
within the literature; e.g., Zhao, Seibert, and triad could bridge relatively dissonant or cross-

26 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


level theoretical perspectives on organizing. For Akhtar, R., G. Ahmetoglu, and T. Chamorro-
example: whether, when, or which actors resem- Premuzic (2013). “Greed Is Good? Assessing
ble Schumpeterian opportunity entrepreneurs, the Relationship between Entrepreneurship
versus self-interest with guile opportunists com- and Subclinical Psychopathy,” Personality
mon to strategy and economics. and Individual Differences 54, 420–425.
Finally, our consideration of entrepreneurial Anderson, A. R., and L. Warren (2011). “The
motives and intent presumed formal economy Entrepreneur as Hero and Jester: Enacting
venturing. Our theory and findings suggest the the Entrepreneurial Discourse,” Interna-
opportunity for future research illuminating the tional Small Business Journal 29, 589–609.
role of the dark triad in more shadowy types of Antoncic, B., T. Bratkovic Kregar, G. Singh,
entrepreneurial activity such as that which takes and A. F. DeNoble (2015). “The Big Five
place in the underground economy (Webb et al. Personality-Entrepreneurship Relationship:
2009). Evidence from Slovenia,” Journal of Small
Business Management 53, 819–841.
Conclusions Babiak, P., and R. D. Hare (2006). Snakes in
Moving beyond the largely taken-for-granted Suits: When Psychopaths Go to Work. New
view of entrepreneurship as source of value cre- York: HarperCollins Publishing.
ation, the current research considered Baron, R. A. (2015). Essentials of Entrepre-
individual-level factors relating to unproductive neurship: Evidence and Practice. North-
versus productive venturing. Our findings sug- ampton, MA: Edward Elgar.
gest that, even within an institutional context Baron, R. A., R. J. Franklin, and K. M.
suitable for productive entrepreneurship, indi- Hmieleski (2016). “Why Entrepreneurs
vidual differences may motivate actors to pursue Often Experience Low, Not High, Levels of
unproductive motives for venturing. Previous Stress: The Joint Effects of Selection and
research on entrepreneurial intentions has typi- Psychological Capital,” Journal of Manage-
cally assumed that the more new ventures ment 42(3), 742–768.
started, the better—thus, attempting to discover Baron, R. A., K. M. Hmieleski, and R. A. Henry
levers (e.g., entrepreneurial self-efficacy) that (2012). “Entrepreneurs’ Dispositional Posi-
might be used to increase entrepreneurial inten- tive Affect: The Potential Benefits—and
tions of the general population. Our nuanced Potential Costs—of Being ‘Up’,” Journal of
view suggests that not all such levers are likely Business Venturing 27, 310–324.
to be equal (e.g., some may attract/enable indi- Baron, R. A., J. Tang, and K. M. Hmieleski
viduals high in the dark triad, inadvertently (2011). “The Downside of Being ‘Up’: Entre-
increasing the entry of individuals with unpro- preneurs’ Dispositional Affect and Firm Per-
ductive motives). For these reasons, policy mak- formance,” Strategic Entrepreneurship
ers and potential new venture stakeholders Journal 5(2), 101–119.
ought to be vigilant in terms of the design of Baron, R. A., H. Zhao, and Q. Miao (2015).
incentives and in managing their relationships “Personal Motives, Moral Disengagement,
with individuals potentially high in the dark and Unethical Decisions by Entrepreneurs:
triad. While individuals high in the dark triad Cognitive Mechanisms on the ‘Slippery
may not be greatly over-represented in the pool Slope’,” Journal of Business Ethics 128(1),
of aspiring entrepreneurs, those who do possess 107–118.
entrepreneurial intentions may stand to use Baumol, W. (1990). “Entrepreneurship: Pro-
entrepreneurship as a vehicle for appropriating ductive, Unproductive, and Destructive,”
value rather than creating it. Journal of Political Economy 98, 893–921.
Becker, T. E. (2005). “Potential Problems in
References the Statistical Control of Variables in Organ-
Adrian, R., S. C. Richards, and D. L. Paulhus izational Research: A Qualitative Analysis
(2013). “The Dark Triad of Personality: A with Recommendations,” Organizational
10 Year Review,” Social and Personality Research Methods 8, 274–289.
Psychology Compass 7, 199–216. Bergman, J. Z., J. W. Westerman, and J. P.
Ajzen, I. (1991). “The Theory of Planned Daly (2010). “Narcissism in Management
Behavior,” Organizational Behavior and Education,” Academy of Management
Human Decision Processes 50, 179–211. Learning and Education 9, 119–131.

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 27


Brown, T., J. Sautter, L. Littvay, A. Sautter, and Behavior,” Psychological Science 22, 1073–
B. Bearnes (2010). “Ethics and Personality: 1080.
Empathy and Narcissism as Moderators of Decuyper, M., S. De Pauw, F. De Fruyt, M. De
Ethical Decision Making in Business Bolle, and B. J. De Clercq (2009). “A Meta-
Students,” Journal of Education for Busi- Analysis of Psychopathy, Antisocial PD, and
ness 85, 203–208. FFM Associations,” European Journal of
Bruyat, C., and P. A. Julien (2001). “Defining Personality 23, 531–565.
the Field of Research in Entrepreneurship,” Deluga, R. J. (2001). “American Presidential
Journal of Business Venturing 16, 165–180. Machiavellianism: Implications for Charismatic
Buckels, E. E., D. N. Jones, and D. L. Paulhus Leadership and Rated Performance,” The
(2013). “Behavioral Confirmation of Every- Leadership Quarterly 12, 339–363.
day Sadism,” Psychological Science 24, DeNisi, A. S. (2015). “Some Further Thoughts
2201–2209. on the Entrepreneurial Personality,” Entre-
Buss, D. M. (2009). “How Can Evolutionary preneurship Theory and Practice 39,
Psychology Successfully Explain Personality 997–1003.
and Individual Differences?,” Perspectives Dutton, K. (2012). The Wisdom of Psychopaths:
on Psychological Science 4, 359–366. What Saints, Spies, and Serial Killers Can
Carsrud, A., and M. Br€annback (2011). Teach Us about Success. New York: Farrar,
“Entrepreneurial Motivations: What Do We Straus and Giroux.
Still Need to Know?,” Journal of Small Busi- Ermer, E., and K. A. Kiehl (2010).
ness Management 49, 9–26. “Psychopaths Are Impaired in Social
Carter, G. L., A. C. Campbell, S. Muncer, and Exchange and Precautionary Reasoning,”
K. A. Carter (2015). “A Mokken Analysis of Psychological Science 21, 1399–1405.
the Dark Triad ‘Dirty Dozen’: Sex and Age Fairlie, R. W. (2004). “Recent Trends in Ethnic
Differences in Scale Structure and Issues
and Racial Business Ownership,” Small
with Individual Items,” Personality and
Business Economics 23, 203–218.
Individual Differences 83, 185–191.
Foster, J. D., D. E. Reidy, T. A. Misra, and J. S.
Chatterjee, A., and D. C. Hambrick (2007). “It’s
Goff (2011). “Narcissism and Stock Market
All About Me: Narcissistic Chief Executive
Investing: Correlates and Consequences of
Officers and Their Effects on Company
Cocksure Investing,” Personality and Indi-
Strategy and Performance,” Administrative
Science Quarterly 52, 351–386. vidual Differences 50, 816–821.
Chen, C. C., P. G. Greene, and A. Crick (1998). Furtner, M. R., J. F. Rauthmann, and P. Sachse
“Does Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy Distin- (2011). “The Self-Loving Self-Leader: An
guish Entrepreneurs from Managers?,” Jour- Examination of the Relationship between
nal of Business Venturing 13, 295–316. Self-Leadership and the Dark Triad,” Social
Cohen, J., P. Cohen, S. G. West, and L. S. Behavior and Personality 39, 369–380.
Aiken (2003). Applied Multiple Regression/ Goss, D. (2008). “Enterprise Ritual: A Theory of
Correlation Analysis for the Behavior Scien- Entrepreneurial Emotion and Exchange,”
ces, 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum British Journal of Management 19, 120–137.
Associates. Gunnthorsdottir, A., K. McCabe, and V. Smith
Cook, K. S., and E. Rice (2003). “Social (2002). “Using the Machiavellianism Instru-
Exchange Theory,” in The Handbook of ment to Predict Trustworthiness in a Bar-
Social Psychology. Ed. J. DeLamater. New gaining Game,” Journal of Economic
York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, Psychology 23, 49–66.
53–76. Gupta, V. K., D. B. Turban, and N. M. Bhawe
Cooper, A. C., and K. W. Artz (1995). (2008). “The Effect of Gender Stereotype Acti-
“Determinants of Satisfaction for Entrepre- vation on Entrepreneurial Intentions,” Journal
neurs,” Journal of Business Venturing 10, of Applied Psychology 93, 1053–1061.
439–457. Hair, J., W. Black, B. Babin, and R. Anderson
C^
ote, S., K. A. DeCelles, J. M. McCarthy, G. A. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th ed.
Van Kleef, and I. Hideg (2011). “The Jekyll Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
and Hyde of Emotional Intelligence: Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy
Emotion-Regulation Knowledge Facilitates Checklist-Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health
both Prosocial and Interpersonally Deviant Systems.

28 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


Hare, R. D., and C. S. Neumann (2006). “The Jonason, P. K., B. L. Koenig, and J. Tost
PCL-R Assessment of Psychopathy: Develop- (2010). “Living a Fast Life: The Dark Triad
ment, Structural Properties, and New and Life History Theory,” Human Nature
Directions,” in Handbook of Psychopathy. 21, 428–442.
Ed. C. Patrick. New York: Guilford, 58–88. Jonason, P. K., and L. Krause (2013). “The
Hays, R. D., T. Hayashi, and A. L. Stewart Emotional Deficits Associated with the Dark
(1989). “A Five-Item Measure of Socially Triad Traits: Cognitive Empathy, Affective
Desirable Response Set,” Educational and Empathy, and Alexithymia,” Personality
Psychological Measurement 49, 629–636. and Individual Differences 55, 532–537.
Hotchkiss, S. (2003). Why Is It Always about Jonason, P. K., N. P. Li, and E. A. Teicher
You?: The Seven Deadly Sins of Narcissism. (2010). “Who Is James Bond? The Dark
New York: Free Press. Triad as an Agentic Social Style,” Individual
Hunt, M. K., D. R. Hopko, R. Bare, C. W. Differences Research 8, 111–120.
Lejuez, and E. V. Robinson (2005). Jonason, P. K., N. P. Li, G. D. Webster, and D.
“Construct Validity of the Balloon Analog P. Schmitt (2009). “The Dark Triad: Facilitat-
Risk Task (BART): Associations with Psy- ing a Short-Term Mating Strategy in Men,”
chopathy and Impulsivity,” Assessment 12, European Journal of Personality 23, 5–18.
416–428. Jonason, P. K., and J. Tost (2010). “I Just Can-
Hmieleski, K. M., and R. A. Baron (2008). not Control Myself: The Dark Triad and
“Regulatory Focus and New Venture Per- Self-Control,” Personality and Individual
formance: A Study of Entrepreneurial Differences 49(6), 611–615.
Opportunity Exploitation Under Conditions Jonason, P. K., and G. D. Webster (2010). “The
of Risk versus Uncertainty,” Strategic Entre- Dirty Dozen: A Concise Measure of the Dark
preneurship Journal 2(4), 285–299. Triad,” Psychological Assessment 22, 420–432.
——— (2009). “Entrepreneurs’ Optimism and Jonason, P. K., G. D. Webster, D. P. Schmitt,
New Venture Performance: A Social Cogni- N. P. Li, and L. Crysel (2012). “The Antihero
tive Perspective,” Academy of Management in Popular Culture: Life History Theory and
Journal 52, 473–488. the Dark Triad Personality Traits,” Review
Hmieleski, K. M., J. C. Carr, and R. A. Baron of General Psychology 16, 192–199.
(2015). “Integrating Discovery and Creation Jones, D. N. (2013). “What’s Mine Is Mine and
Perspectives of Entrepreneurial Action: The What’s Yours Is Mine: The Dark Triad and
Relative Roles of Founding CEO Human Gambling with Your Neighbor’s Money,” Jour-
Capital, Social Capital, and Psychological nal of Research in Personality 47, 563–571.
Capital in Contexts of Risk versus ——— (2014). “Risk in the Face of Retribution:
Uncertainty,” Strategic Entrepreneurship Psychopathic Individuals Persist in Financial
Journal 9(4), 289–312. Misbehavior Among the Dark Triad,” Per-
Hmieleski, K. M., M. S. Cole, and R. A. Baron sonality and Individual Differences 67,
(2012). “Shared Authentic Leadership and 109–113.
New Venture Performance,” Journal of Jones, D. N., and A. J. Figueredo (2013). “The
Management 38(5), 1476–1499. Core of Darkness: Uncovering the Heart of
Hmieleski, K. M., and A. C. Corbett (2006). the Dark Triad,” European Journal of Per-
“Proclivity for Improvisation as a Predictor sonality 27, 521–531.
of Entrepreneurial Intentions,” Journal of Jones, D. N., and D. L. Paulhus (2014).
Small Business Management 44(1), 45–63. “Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A
Hmieleski, K. M., A. C. Corbett, and R. A. Brief Measure of Dark Personality Traits,”
Baron (2013). “Entrepreneurs’ Improvisa- Assessment 21, 28–41.
tional Behavior and Firm Performance: A Kautonen, T., M. Gelderen, and M. Fink
Study of Dispositional and Environmental (2015). “Robustness of the Theory of
Moderators,” Strategic Entrepreneurship Planned Behavior in Predicting Entrepre-
Journal 7(2), 138–150. neurial Intentions and Actions,” Entrepre-
Humphrey, R. H. (2013). “The Benefits of neurship Theory and Practice 39, 655–674.
Emotional Intelligence and Empathy to Kets de Vries, M. F. R. (1985). “The Dark Side
Entrepreneurship,” Entrepreneurship Research of Entrepreneurship,” Harvard Business
Journal 3, 287–294. Review 63, 160–167.

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 29


——— (1996). “The Anatomy of the Entrepre- Malach-Pines, A., H. Levy, A. Utasi, and T. L.
neur: Clinical Observations,” Human Rela- Hill (2005). “Entrepreneurs as Cultural Her-
tions 49, 853–883. oes: A Cross-Cultural, Interdisciplinary
Klotz, A. C., K. M. Hmieleski, B. H. Bradley, Perspective,” Journal of Managerial Psy-
and L. W. Busenitz (2014). “New Venture chology 20, 541–555.
Teams: A Review of the Literature and Markman, G. D., and R. A. Baron (2003).
Roadmap for Future Research,” Journal of “Person–Entrepreneurship Fit: Why Some
Management 40, 226–255. People Are More Successful as Entrepre-
Klotz, A. C., and D. O. Neubaum (2016). neurs than Others,” Human Resource Man-
“Research on the Dark Side of Personality agement Review 13, 281–301.
Traits in Entrepreneurship: Observations Mathieu, C., R. D. Hare, D. N. Jones, P.
from an Organizational Behavior Babiak, and C. S. Neumann (2013). “Factor
Perspective,” Entrepreneurship Theory and Structure of the B-Scan 360: A Measure of
Practice 40, 7–17. Corporate Psychopathy,” Psychological
Kristof-Brown, A. L., R. D. Zimmerman, and E. Assessment 25, 288–293.
C. Johnson (2005). “Consequences of Indi- Mathieu, C., and E  . St-Jean (2013).
vidual’s Fit at Work: A Meta-analysis of “Entrepreneurial Personality: The Role of
Person-Job, Person-Organization, Person- Narcissism,” Personality and Individual
Group, and Person-Supervisor Fit,” Person- Differences 55, 527–531.
nel Psychology 58, 281–342. Miller, J. D., L. R. Few, L. A. Seibert, A. Watts, A.
Krueger, N. F. (1993). “The Impact of Prior Entre- Zeichner, and D. R. Lynam (2012). “An Exami-
preneurial Exposure on Perceptions of New nation of the Dirty Dozen Measure: A Caution-
Venture Feasibility and Desirability,” Entrepre- ary Tale About the Costs of Brief Measures,”
neurship Theory and Practice 15, 5–21. Psychological Assessment 24, 1048–1053.
Krueger, N. F., M. D. Jr., Reilly, and A. L. Morgan, J., and D. Sisak (2016). “Aspiring to
Carsrud (2000). “Competing Models of Succeed: A Model of Entrepreneurship and
Entrepreneurial Intentions,” Journal of Fear of Failure,” Journal of Business Ven-
Business Venturing 15, 411–432. turing 31, 1–21.
Lance, C. E., and R. J. Vandenberg (2009). Sta- Mycoskie, B. (2012). Start Something That
tistical and Methodological Myths and Matters. New York: Random House.
Urban Legends: Doctrine, Verity, and Fable Neter, J., M. H. Kutner, W. Wasserman, and C. J.
in the Organizational and Social Sciences. Nachtscheim (1996). Applied Linear Regres-
New York: Routledge. sion Models. New York: McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
Lerner, D. (2016). “Behavioral Disinhibition Nicolaou, N., S. Shane, G. Adi, M. Mangino,
and Nascent Venturing: Relevance and Ini- and J. Harris (2011). “A Polymorphism
tial Effects on Potential Resource Providers,” Associated with Entrepreneurship: Evidence
Journal of Business Venturing 31, 234–252. from Dopamine Receptor Candidate Genes,”
Lewin, A. Y., and C. U. Stephens (1994). “CEO Small Business Economics 36(2), 151–155.
Attitudes as Determinants of Organization O’Boyle, E. H., Jr., D. R. Forsyth, G. C. Banks,
Design: An Integrated Model,” Organiza- and M. A. McDaniel (2012). “A Meta-
tion Studies 15, 183–212. Analysis of the Dark Triad and Work Behav-
Leybman, M. J., D. C. Zuroff, and M. A. ior: A Social Exchange Perspective,” Journal
Fournier (2011). “A Five-Dimensional Model of Applied Psychology 97, 557–579.
of Individual Differences in Social Exchange O’Reilly III., C. A., B. Doerr, D. F. Caldwell,
Styles,” Personality and Individual Differ- and J. A. Chatman (2014). “Narcissistic
ences 51, 940–945. CEOs and Executive Compensation,” The
Lu, J. F., D. Tjosvold, and K. Shi (2010). “Team Leadership Quarterly 25(2), 218–231.
Training in China: Testing and Applying the Paulhus, D. L., and K. M. Williams (2002). “The
Theory of Cooperation and Competition,” Jour- Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism,
nal of Applied Social Psychology 40, 101–134. Machiavellianism and Psychopathy,” Journal
Magister, H. (2013). “The 4 People Who Made of Research in Personality 36(6), 556–563.
Entrepreneurship Sexy,” Forbes. Retrieved Perugini, M., M. Gallucci, F. Presaghi, and A.
from http://www.forbes.com/sites/holly- P. Ercolani (2003). “The Personal Norm of
magister/2013/10/01/the-4-people-who- Reciprocity,” European Journal of Personal-
made-entrepreneurship-sexy ity 17, 251–283.

30 JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


Piperopoulos, P., and D. Dimov (2015). “Burst Spector, P. E., and M. T. Brannick (2011).
Bubbles or Build Steam? Entrepreneurship “Methodological Urban Legends: The Misuse
Education, Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy, and of Statistical Control Variables,” Organiza-
Entrepreneurial Intentions,” Journal of tional Research Methods 14, 287–305.
Small Business Management 53, 970–985. Staw, B. M. (1991). “Dressing Up Like an
Rauthmann, J. F., and G. P. Kolar (2012). Organization: When Psychological Theories
“How ‘Dark’ Are the Dark Triad Traits? Can Explain Organizational Action,” Journal
Examining the Perceived Darkness of Nar- of Management 17, 805–819.
cissism, Machiavellianism, and Psy- Tabachnick, B. G., and L. S. Fidell (2001). Using
chopathy,” Personality and Individual Multivariate Statistics. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Differences 53, 884–889. Twenge, J. M., and W. K. Campbell (2010). The
Reynolds, P. D., and S. White (1997). The Narcissism Epidemic: Living in the Age of
Entrepreneurial Process: Economic Growth, Entitlement. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Men, Women, and Minorities. Westport, Twenge, J. M., S. Konrath, J. D. Foster, W. K.
CT: Quorum Books. Campbell, and B. J. Bushman (2008). “Egos
Rindova, V., D. Barry, and D. J. Ketchen (2009). Inflating Over Time: A Cross-Temporal Meta-
“Entrepreneuring as Emancipation,” Acad- Analysis of the Narcissistic Personality
emy of Management Review 34, 477–491. Inventory,” Journal of Personality 76, 875–902.
Robinson, J. (2014). “Do You Have What it Van Praag, C. M., and P. Versloot (2007).
Takes?,” Entrepreneur 42, 46–49. “What Is the Value of Entrepreneurship: A
Roff, D. A. (2001). Life History Evolution. Sun- Review of Recent Research,” Small Business
derland, MA: Sinauer Associates. Economics 29(4), 351–382.
Schiller, B. R., and P. E. Crewson (1997). Verheul, I., J. Block, H. Larsson, I. Franken,
“Entrepreneurial Origins: A Longitudinal and A. R. Thurik (2016). “Attention-Deficit/
Hyperactivity Symptoms and Self-Employ-
Inquiry,” Economic Inquiry 35, 523–531.
Schjoedt, L., and B. Bird (2014). “Control Vari- ment,” European Journal of Epidemiology
31(8), 793–801.
ables: Use, Misuse and Recommended Use,”
Webb, J. W., L. Tihanyi, R. D. Ireland, and D.
in Handbook of Research Methods and
G. Sirmon (2009). “You Say Illegal, I Say
Applications in Entrepreneurship and Small
Legitimate: Entrepreneurship in the Infor-
Business. Eds. M. Br€annback and A. L. Cars-
mal Economy,” Academy of Management
rud. Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Pub-
Review 34, 492–510.
lishing, 136–155.
Webster, R. L., and H. A. Harmon (2002).
Shepherd, D. A. (2015). “Party On! A Call for
“Comparing Levels of Machiavellianism of
Entrepreneurship Research That Is More Today’s College Students with College Stu-
Interactive, Activity Based, Cognitively Hot, dents of the 1960s,” Teaching Business
Compassionate, and Prosocial,” Journal of Ethics 6, 435–445.
Business Venturing 30, 489–507. Wilson, K., S. Demetrioff, and S. Porter (2008).
Shepherd, D. A., H. Patzelt, and R. A. Baron “A Pawn by Any Other Name? Social Infor-
(2013). “I Care About Nature But. . . mation Processing as a Function of Psycho-
Disengaging Sustainable Development Val- pathic Traits,” Journal of Research in
ues to Exploit Opportunities that Harm the Personality 42, 1651–1656.
Natural Environment,” Academy of Manage- Zettler, I., and M. Solga (2013). “Not Enough
ment Journal 56, 1251–1273. of a ‘Dark’ Trait? Linking Machiavellianism
Sherman, R. A., A. J. Figueredo, and D. C. to Job Performance,” European Journal of
Funder (2013). “The Behavioral Correlates Personality 27, 545–554.
of Overall and Distinctive Life History Strat- Zhao, H., S. E. Seibert, and G. E. Hills (2005).
egy,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- “The Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy in the
chology 105, 873–888. Development of Entrepreneurial Intentions,”
Smith, M. B., J. C. Wallace, and P. Jordan Journal of Applied Psychology 90, 1265–1272.
(2016). “When the Dark Ones Become Zhao, H., S. E. Seibert, and G. T. Lumpkin
Darker: How Promotion Focus Moderates (2010). “The Relationship of Personality to
the Effects of the Dark Triad on Supervisor Entrepreneurial Intentions and Performance:
Performance Ratings,” Journal of Organiza- A Meta-Analytic Review,” Journal of Man-
tional Behavior 37(2), 236–254. agement 36, 381–404.

HMIELESKI AND LERNER 31


32
Appendix
Confirmatory Principal Components Matrix of Unproductive and Productive
Entrepreneurial Motives Using Varimax Rotation for Subsamples of Early-Stage
Nascent Entrepreneursa,b,c

Item Unproductive Productive


Entrepreneurial Motives Entrepreneurial Motives
Undergraduates MBAs Undergraduates MBAs
(n 5 281) (n 5 145) (n 5 281) (n 5 145)
If I were to start a new venture, my motivation for the business would
include wanting it to:
1. Achieve financial success, even if it is a little destructive to society 0.79 0.71 20.20 20.36
2. Maximize profits, even at the cost of employees’ well-being 0.79 0.78 20.19 20.23
3. Grow quickly, even if that means sacrificing quality 0.67 0.67 20.06 20.02
4. Earn a financial profit at all costs 0.65 0.69 0.02 20.20
5. Outsource work to reduce costs as much as possible 0.65 0.61 0.07 20.04
6. Generate value for society 20.14 20.28 0.74 0.70
7. Produce products/services that enrich the lives of people 0.03 20.26 0.76 0.71
8. Develop a culture in which its employees value their work 0.01 20.17 0.69 0.74
9. Be admired for the value that it adds to the community 20.04 20.00 0.76 0.61
10. Attract employees who value the mission of the company as though 20.17 20.11 0.73 0.86
it were their own

JOURNAL OF SMALL BUSINESS MANAGEMENT


a
In relation to the items associated with value appropriation, no assertion is being made about profit-driven entrepreneurship being necessarily unproductive. Sim-
ply, with value appropriation characterizing unproductive entrepreneurship and motives, seeking to externalize costs appropriates others’ value. In relation to the
items associated with value creation: no suggestion is made that these are the only possible means to creative value, that the profit-motive cannot be productive, or that
for entrepreneurship to be productive it must achieve all these ends. Simply, it is unambiguous that said items indicate value creation rather than appropriation motives.
Overall, in terms of the central thesis of the paper, new ventures formed to differing degrees on the basis these two sets of motivations, are apt to generate (or appropri-
ate) value to different degrees.
b
The question stem was phrased generally such that the items could be applied to all individuals: those having not begun the entrepreneurial process, those consid-
ering venturing, and those who have begun the entrepreneurial process (independent of whether they have reached associated operational milestones).
c
The items representing unproductive entrepreneurial motives (items 1–5) and productive entrepreneurial motives (items 6–10) are respectively indicated by their
corresponding bold factor loadings.

You might also like