You are on page 1of 14

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:


https://www.researchgate.net/publication/8224520

Children with Mathematics Learning


Disabilities in Belgium

Article in Journal of Learning Disabilities · February 2004


DOI: 10.1177/00222194040370010601 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

70 126

3 authors:

Annemie Desoete Herbert Roeyers


Ghent University Ghent University
188 PUBLICATIONS 1,572 CITATIONS 369 PUBLICATIONS 12,632 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Armand De Clercq
Ghent University
36 PUBLICATIONS 1,245 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

intervention studies View project

understanding atypical development View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Annemie Desoete on 15 March 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Children with Mathematics
Learning Disabilities in Belgium
Annemie Desoete, Herbert Roeyers, and Armand De Clercq

Abstract
In Belgium, between 3% and 8% of the children in elementary schools have mathematics learning disabilities (MLD). Many of these chil-
dren have less developed linguistic, procedural, and mental representation skills. Moreover, a majority of the children have been found
to show inaccurate prediction and evaluation skills in Grade 3. MLD often become obvious in elementary school. Whereas some chil-
dren are retained, others are referred to special education. During the last 10 years, the number of children in special education and the
number of children with learning disabilities who are following a special guidance program in general education (inclusive education)
have increased. Children in Belgium with MLD can get therapy for about 2 years. Nevertheless, many problems continue unresolved
even in high school and adulthood.

F
rom 1974 to 1997, only 28 articles Mathematics Education ther by vocational training or by col-
on mathematics learning disabil- in Belgium lege or university.
ities (MLD) were cited in Psyc- All Belgian children have to con-
Info, whereas 747 articles could be General Education tinue their education (and in most
found on reading disabilities (Noel, cases remain at school) at least until
2000). Therefore, we agree with Gins- Belgium is a small country in the they are 18 years old. A small number
burg (1997) and Hanich, Jordan, Kap- heart of Europe with a population of of children are educated at home, be-
lan, and Dick (2001) that children with nearly 11 million. Belgium has French- cause since 1914, there has been com-
mathematics difficulties have been un- speaking and a Flemish-speaking com- pulsory education in Belgium, but not
derstudied, and their problems have munities, along with a small German- compulsory schooling. There are three
been underestimated. We know that speaking community. This article relates types of schools in Belgium: Catholic
MLD can seriously limit a student’s ed- mainly to the Flemish-speaking com- schools, schools organized by the fed-
ucational opportunities (Rivera-Batiz, munity, or Flanders, although some eral government, and schools orga-
1992). Moreover, the disabilities often additional information is provided nized by the cities or provinces. There
persist into the college years (Miller & concerning the French-speaking com- are no major differences between these
Mercer, 1997), and many of these chil- munity. The situation in the French- three types of schools, although each
dren continue to function below the and German-speaking parts is similar operationalizes the obligatory central
arithmetical level of a 13-year-old child to that of the Flemish-speaking part. developmental aims (ontwikkelingsdoe-
(Cawley & Miller, 1989; Miller & Mer- In Belgium, most children start len) of the kindergarten and the out-
cer, 1997). kindergarten when they are 21⁄2 years comes to be achieved at the end of ele-
In this article, first we give a general old. They get 31⁄2 years of kindergarten mentary education (eindtermen) in a
description of mathematics education education. At the age of 6, the children slightly different way with respect to
in Belgium. Second, we describe chil- go to primary school, where they learn the mathematics curriculum (wiskun-
dren with MLD in our country. Third, to read, write, and do mathematics. deleerplan).
we discuss the problems with mathe- Primary classes include roughly 20 to Schools are held accountable for stu-
matical problem solving in children of 30 children in a single classroom. Chil- dent performance. Inspection is orga-
average intelligence, both with and dren spend about 7 hours at school nized at a central level. As in most Eu-
without MLD, in Belgium. Finally, we every weekday, except Wednesday, ropean countries, several systematic,
inventory the instructional interven- which has early dismissal. There is no full inspections of the schools as a
tions and the accommodations imple- school on Wednesday afternoon. After whole are carried out in Belgium an-
mented to help students with MLD in primary school, students enter sec- nually (Standaert, 2001). The CIPO
Belgium. ondary education. This is followed ei- (Context, Input, Process, Output) model

JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES


VOLUME 37, NUMBER 1, JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2004, PAGES 50–61
VOLUME 37, NUMBER 1, JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2004 51

is used for this inspection, and a Korea and Japan. Especially children in given the opportunity to discover ade-
school’s performance is evaluated in families with a high socioeconomic sta- quate strategies for themselves on a va-
four key areas. This evaluation deter- tus did well in this study. Children in riety of tasks presented in meaningful
mines the results of each school with families with a lower socioeconomic and rich contexts (e.g., Milo, 2001; Van
respect to student performance (out- status had results comparable with Luit, 1999; Verschaffel, 1999).
put) and the way in which these results those of their peers in the other coun- This conceptual change has had con-
have been achieved ( process), taking tries. Moreover, students in the French- sequences for the content of mathe-
into consideration the context in which speaking part of Belgium did worse on matics education (Verschaffel, 1995).
the school is operating (e.g., demo- mathematics, reading, and science So-called structuralistic “new mathe-
graphic, juridical, and administrative than their Flemish-speaking peers in matics” (e.g., A∪B) has disappeared
data) and the input data (e.g., informa- the same country (Bormans, 2002; from the curriculum, and some num-
tion about pupils, school staff, and ma- Galle, 2001). ber algorithms, formulas, and other
terial resources). In Flanders, the eval- The mathematics curriculum in Bel- domain-specific data (e.g., prime num-
uation of the quality of teachers is left gium changed in 1998 (Commissie bers, nine control on multiplications,
to the local school or its governing Wiskundeleerplan van het Gemeen- theory of sets) have disappeared with
body, whereas the French-speaking schapsonderwijs, 1998; Vlaams Ver- them. In contrast, the accent is now on
community of Belgium has a central bond van het Katholiek Basisonder- mental mathematics, on approximate
teacher assessment system (Standaert, wijs, 1999). The former mechanistic arithmetic or number sense, on mathe-
2001). approach and even the structuralistic matics using the calculator (from
In the Flanders curriculum, 80% of approach (with “new mathematics”) Grade 4), and on problem solving and
the children must acquire 80% of the became history. Inspired by the exam- strategy use. However, there is still ac-
required minimum skills and knowl- ple of the Netherlands (Freudenthal, knowledgement of the importance of
edge by the end of their primary school 1991; Treffers, 1991), a more construc- older, established aims and contents,
education. Children who do not meet tivistic realistic mathematics education such as perfect mastery of number con-
these minimum aims and who read or (RME) was introducted, which empha- cepts. But in contrast with the tradi-
solve mathematics problems on the sizes the importance of students be- tional conception of mathematics learn-
level of third graders without learning coming active learners who construct ing as the passive and isolated
disabilities (LD) are referred to the their own mathematics (Blöte, Van der absorption of knowledge and proce-
B-classes of secondary education (Hel- Burg, & Klein, 2001). The main para- dures, these concepts should be con-
linckx & Ghesquière, 1999). If their lost digm of RME is that the teaching of structed actively by children, allowing
ground is made up in the first year of mathematics should build on the pre- for individual learning trajectories
secondary education, then these chil- vious knowledge of children derived (Torbeys, Verschaffel, & Ghesquière,
dren can go back to the A-classes, where from their real-life experiences with 2003).
they follow a general course of studies numbers. Dialectical constructivists RME focuses on adaptive expertise
aimed at preparing them for college or stress that the understanding of arith- (Baroody & Dowker, 2003) and on hav-
university. About 20% of the children metic develops through active knowl- ing students create and discuss their
with LD in special primary education edge construction, transformation, and own solution procedures and on using
go to one of the A-classes in secondary discovery within an approach involv- context problems to relate mathemat-
education (Hellinckx & Ghesquière, ing scaffolding and does not merely ics to real-life situations. RME has re-
1999). The other children follow a spe- result from passive information acqui- sulted in a higher level of flexibility
cialized course of vocational training. sition (Cobb, Yackel & Wood, 1992; and more conceptual understanding
General mathematics education in Muthukrishna & Borkowski, 1996). for average students without LD (e.g.,
Flanders seems to be doing quite well They assume that people actively con- Blöte et al., 2001). However, children
for most children. The Program for In- struct their own knowledge by linking with MLD have not done as well (e.g.,
ternational Student Assessment (PISA) prior knowledge to new knowledge. Woodward & Baxter, 1997). Several
recently compared the learning perfor- The emphasis is on a more functional handbooks (e.g., Pluspunt, 2002) and
mance of students in 30 different coun- use of basic mathematics concepts and teaching methods based on the RME
tries all over Europe, Asia, and Amer- skills and on the use of schemata to as- approach that have been used for
ica. This project revealed that the sist abstract thinking. Moreover, math- Flemish children with MLD have re-
15-year-old Flemish students did best ematics has become a tool for solving sulted in confused children who are
on mathematics (DeMeyer, De Vos, & all kinds of problems, for handling all unable to solve math problems. This is
Van de Poele, 2002). Flemish students kinds of tasks, and for communicating presumably because RME places sub-
obtained a second place on reading and working together through a more stantial demands on metacognitive
skills, just after students from Finland, interactive teaching process. In the RME skills, which may not be adequately
and they scored third on science, after approach, children in small groups are developed in children with MLD (De-
52 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

soete, Roeyers, & De Clercq, 2003b; in Types l through 8 altogether repre- schools now have the possibility of co-
Mercer, Jordan, & Miller, 1996). It ap- sent 78.65% of the total number of chil- operating with schools for special edu-
pears that children with LD profit less dren in special primary education cation where students with learning or
from RME than their peers with age- (Mardulier, 2001). However, there other disabilities are being educated.
adequate learning skills. In clinical seems to be a large gender difference in As a result, it is becoming possible for
practice, children with MLD seem to the choice of general versus special ed- some children with disabilities to re-
need a more structured and direct in- ucation. In 2000–2001 in Flanders, boys main in general education for 1 or
structional approach, in which the went more often to special schools than 2 years or, in the case of a severe dis-
teachers teach the conceptual under- girls. ability, for an unlimited time. The gen-
standing and demonstrate and repeat In general, the budget for special eral education school then receives
the rules and procedural skills to be primary and secondary education is pedagogical–didactic or therapeutic
learned. The RME goal of flexible use about 414 million euro, which means assistance from a school for special ed-
of procedures often results in children that 9% of the total education budget is ucation. The school for special educa-
with MLD lacking the necessary struc- spent on the 3.9% of the students, who tion receives an integration allowance
ture and conceptual knowledge to de- are receiving this kind of education to assist the general education school
duce for themselves the procedural (Mardulier, 2001). With this extra bud- in educating children with disabilities.
skills needed to solve mathematical get, special education is able to work Although the percentage of children in
problems. For this reason, there is con- with smaller classes (about 10 children special education has increased, the
siderable debate in Flanders as to the in a class) using specially trained number of GON students has also in-
value of RME for these children (e.g., teachers and a team of other specialists creased in 10 years. In 1994–1995, there
Feys, 1998; Kroesbergen, 2002; Milo, (speech–language therapists, occupa- were 36 children in Type 8 with a GON
2001; Van Luit, 1992, 1994). tional therapists, ergonomic therapists, status, whereas in 2000–2001, there
psychologists, and sometimes psychia- were already 190 children in the GON
trists or other physicians) to assist in education system (Mardulier, 2001;
Special, Inclusive, or Integrated
the education of these children. Wuyts, 1996).
Education?
At the present time in Flanders, there To summarize, general mathematics
The general education system has not is a strong movement in favor of inclu- education in Flanders seems to be
met the needs of some children in Bel- sive education (Broekaert & Van Hove, doing quite well for most children. Al-
gium. Since 1970, separate special 1995; Kennes, 2001; Lebeer, 2003; Van though there is a strong movement in
schools have been created for some cat- Hove & Roets, 2000; Wuyts, 1996). favor of inclusive and integrated edu-
egories of children. For young children Some of its proponents are even insist- cation, separate special schools exist
(7–14 years) with reading, spelling, or ing on the abolition of special educa- for children with LD.
mathematics learning disabilities, Type tion for children with LD. This inclu-
8 special primary education has been sive approach to education is aimed at
set up. For children with LD, no special restructuring the schools in order to MLD in Flanders
education program has as yet been set meet the needs of the students who are
General and Special Education
up either in kindergarten or in sec- at risk and at promoting the value of
ondary schools. In the 1970s, about 3% diversity within the human commu- In Flanders, most learning disabilities
of school-age children went to special nity. The guiding principle is that are not detected until primary school,
schools (Types l through 8). However, schools should accommodate all chil- when reading disabilities often become
by the end of the 1990s, 3.91% were at- dren, regardless of their physical, intel- obvious. Likewise, many children with
tending such special schools. This was lectual, social, emotional, linguistic, or MLD in primary school did not en-
the case for 0.73% of the children in other characteristics. In this spirit, the counter severe difficulties with pre-
kindergarten, 5.92% of the primary care coordinator (zorgcoördinator) has liminary mathematics learning in kin-
school children, and 3.65 % of the sec- an important task in general education dergarten, although in some cases,
ondary school children. For Type 8 from September 2003 on. inefficient counting and not remem-
schools (children with LD), there has An integrated education (Geïnte- bering number names was already
also been a large increase in recent greerd onderwijs; GON) experiment was present as a marker (Grégoire, Van
years. In 1990–1991, there were 6,015 started in 1980 on Flemish children Nieuwenhoven, & Noel, 2003). Never-
children in Type 8 schools, whereas in who had a physical or sensory disabil- theless, most children with MLD in
2000–2001, 10,049 (+ 59.86%) of the ity (Types 4, 6, and 7). In 1983, it was Belgium are detected in Grade 1, when
children in special primary education generalized to all types of special edu- they have to master addition and sub-
in Belgium were being educated in cation, and in 1986, it was put into law. traction, and, in some cases, even later
Type 8 schools. The number of children School boards of general education (in Grade 3), when they have to learn
VOLUME 37, NUMBER 1, JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2004 53

to quickly retrieve the multiplication telligence. Second, the severeness crite- 17 children with MLD were detected.
tables or to select and apply various rion is applied, which is based on the This study indicated that a combined
problem-solving strategies in addition DSM-IV (pp. 46–51). We only speak of assessment—a test of number facts, at
to the basic mathematical operations. MLD when the child’s difficulties with least one test of domain-specific
In secondary education, college, or mathematics, as measured by a valid knowledge, and a test of math word
university, students with MLD en- test, are found to be 2 or more standard problems—was needed to prevent the
counter numerous problems with deviations (SD) below the norm. The choice of test from determining the di-
mathematics and physics and very few third criterion is the resistance criterion, agnosis. The domain-specific test in
teachers who understand their learn- which refers to the teacher’s assess- combination with the test of math
ing problems. This often results in low ment of the fact that the difficulties word problems was able to detect 27
self-concept and even in behavioral remain severe even with the usual re- out of the 37 children. The domain-
problems. About 40% to 80% of stu- mediation at school (remedial teaching specific test combined with the test of
dents with LD develop behavioral or school therapist). Teachers’ assess- number facts was able to identify 34
problems (Thiery, 2001). Parents are ments are used because, although out of the 37 children with MLD. Only
confronted with a very tumultuous pe- some researchers question the trust- the combination of a test of domain
riod in the life of these youngsters, up worthiness of these data, reviews indi- specific knowledge, a test of number
to suicidal behavior in some of them. cate that these judgments can serve as facts, and a test of math word problems
worthy assessments of a student’s was found to be able to detect all 37
achievement-related behavior (Meltzer, children with MLD (Desoete & Roey-
Definition of LD in Flanders Roditi, Houser, & Perlman, 1998; ers, 2000). It might be interesting to
Winne & Perry, 2000). Moreover, we combine the assessment of those as-
Although various authors agree that define mathematics learning problems pects within one test (e.g., Grégoire,
an operational definition of LD is (MLP) as the unexplainable difficulties 1997) and to investigate if such a test
meaningful (e.g., Kavale & Forness, that a child has with mathematics would detect all children with MLD.
2000; Swanson, 2000), most studies are when validated by a test in which the Such studies are currently being pre-
rather vague when it comes to charac- child’s performance is between –2 SD pared (Grégoire et al., 2003).
terizing the children who fit in their and –1 SD below the norm (severeness
category of children with learning dis- criterion; e.g., Ghesquière, Ruijsse-
abilities. Several authors use different naars, Grietens, & Luyckx, 1996). These Prevalence of MLD in Flanders
terms for a deficit in mathematical difficulties must be noticed by the
problem solving, such as mathematics teacher for them to be defined as a In October 2001, a prevalence study
learning difficulties, mathematics learning mathematics learning problem. was conducted among general educa-
problems, mathematics learning disorder, The three criteria (severeness, dis- tion second-, third-, and fourth-grade
mathematics learning disability, mathe- crepancy, and resistance) may seem students in Flanders (N = 3,978; see
matics learning retardation, mathematics very clear parameters for ascertaining Table 1).
learning deficiency, or dyscalculia (e.g., whether an individual child belongs to In second grade, 2.27 % of the chil-
American Psychiatric Association, 1994; the group of children with MLD. How- dren (2.11% boys and 2.42% girls) were
Dumont, 1994; Fletcher & Morris, 1986; ever, in clinical practice, the diagnosis found to have MLD according to the
Hellinckx & Ghesquière, 1999; Rourke also depends on the tests chosen to teacher and a mathematics test battery
& Conway, 1997; Swanson, 2000; measure the severeness criterion. The (–2 SD on one of the tests; see Table 1).
Thiery & Anthonis, 2003; Van Luit, choice of these mathematics tests was About 1.51% of the MLD had to do
1998; World Health Organization, 1997). found to be crucial in a study of 85 chil- with a memory deficit, translated in se-
In our studies, in accordance with dren in Flanders. No single test suc- vere problems to retrieve number facts.
the definition in the Diagnostic and ceeded in identifying all 37 children About 0.91% of the children had se-
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders with MLD out of the 85 third graders vere difficulties dealing with domain-
(DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Asso- according to the discrepancy and resis- specific mathematics knowledge and
ciation, 1994), we use three criteria tance criteria. Using a test of domain- skills. Moreover, in the third grade,
to determine whether children have specific mathematics knowledge (mea- 7.70 % of the children (7.15% boys and
mathematics learning disabilities (MLD). suring number knowledge and mental 8.32% girls) had MLD (see Table 1). In
First, in accordance with the discrep- arithmetic), only 23 children with MLD 4.41% of the cases, these children had
ancy criterion, the child must perform were detected. Using a test of math severe difficulties with number fact
significantly lower on mathematics word problems, only 16 children with retrieval. In 3.74% of the cases, these
than one would expect on the basis of MLD were detected. On the basis of a children had severe difficulties with
his or her general school results or in- test of mathematics number facts, only domain-specific mathematics knowl-
54 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

comprehension and production skills are


TABLE 1
the cognitive skills that are necessary
Prevalence of Mathematics Learning Disabilities in Flanders by
for the reading, writing, and compre-
Grade, Gender, and Diagnostic Test
hension of numbers composed of one
Grade 2 Grade 3 Grade 4 or more digits (e.g., read 5 or 14; Mc-
Boysa Girlsb Boysc Girlsd Boyse Girlsf Closkey & Macaruso, 1995; Van Borsel,
Diagnosis/Test (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) 1998). Problems with these cognitive
skills lead to mistakes such as 15 + 9 =
MLD 18 (confusion between 5 and 2 and 9
Total 2.11 2.42 7.15 8.32 6.99 6.22 and 6). Operation symbol comprehension
NF 1.21 1.81 4.43 4.39 3.26 2.52
and production skills are cognitive skills
DS 0.91 0.91 3.29 4.24 4.66 4.29
that enable the reading, writing, and
MLP comprehension of operation symbols
Total 23.27 23.67 27.79 23.15 24.89 25.90
NF 19.33 14.37 16.16 16.01 14.91 10.96
(such as +, –, ×, =, <, >; e.g., Veenman,
DS 11.78 13.31 11.73 13.81 10.87 15.26 1998). Problems with these cognitive
skills lead to mistakes such as 15 × 9 =
AMA
NF 79.46 83.81 79.39 79.59 81.83 86.66 24. Number system comprehension and
DS 87.31 85.93 84.98 81.95 84.47 80.44 production skills are the cognitive skills
dealing with number system knowl-
Note. MLD = mathematics learning disabilities (−2 SD below norm); MLP = mathematics learning problems edge and the position of decades and
(−1 SD below norm); AMA = adequate mathematics abilities (less than −1 SD below norm); NF = based on
units (e.g., Veenman, 1998). Children
number facts, DS = based on domain-specific test.
an = 662. bn = 661. cn = 699. dn = 637. en = 644. fn = 675. having problems with these skills often
make mistakes with the place of a
number on a number line and do not
edge and skills. Furthermore, in the order to prevent the choice of test from know how many decades and units
fourth grade, 6.59% of the children determining the diagnosis. there are, for example, in 15. Procedural
(6.99% boys and 6.22% girls) had MLD skills are domain-specific cognitive
in Flanders (see Table 1). MLD diag- skills involved in calculating and solv-
noses were based on number fact re- Mathematics Problems in ing mathematics tasks in number prob-
trieval and domain-specific knowl- Children With and lem formats (e.g., 15 + 9 = __ or 81 –
edge in 2.88% and 4.47% of the cases, Without LD 5 = __; e.g., McCloskey & Macaruso,
respectively. 1995; Noel, 2000; Veenman, 1998).
About 25.47% of the second graders In the last decade, substantial progress Problems with these cognitive skills
had MLP according to the teacher and has been made in characterizing the lead to mistakes such as 15 + 9 = 105 or
a mathematics test battery (–1 SD on cognitive and metacognitive skills that 114. Language comprehension skills are
one of the tests), in Grade 2 (see Ta- are important for success in mathemat- cognitive conceptual skills that enable
ble 1). In Grade 3 and Grade 4, the ical problem solving (Boekaerts, 1999; children to understand and solve one-
prevalence rates of MLP were 25.58% Geary, 1993; Grégoire & Van Nieuwen- sentence word problems (e.g. 9 more
and 25.41%, respectively. The mean hoven, 1999; Hacker, Dunlosky, & than 15 is __). Language plays a central
mathematics scores for boys and girls Graesser, 1998; Lucangeli & Cornoldi, role according to several authors (e.g.,
in second, third, and fourth grade did 1997; Montague, 1997; Simons, 1996; Campbell, 1998; McCloskey & Maca-
not differ on domain-specific knowl- Wong, 1996). On the basis of this re- ruso, 1995). Veenman (1998) stressed
edge or on number fact knowledge. search and of an analysis of 150 math the importance of general conceptual
In summary, several authors use dif- protocols of third graders in our study, knowledge in mathematics. Van Borsel
ferent concepts for overlapping phe- a conceptual model of mathematical (1998) went even further and defended
nomena as mathematics difficulties. problem solving was developed (De MLD as a special kind of language dis-
Moreover, the prevalence rate of MLD Clercq, Desoete, & Roeyers, 2000; De- order. We would not go so far. How-
in lower elementary schools in Bel- soete, 2002; Desoete, Roeyers, & De ever, we can see that if children do
gium varied from 2.27% to 7.70%. We Clercq, 2002a, 2002b; see Figure 1). not know what “more” means, then
argue that the diagnosis of children A variety of studies based on differ- word problems such as “9 more than
with MLD must be made carefully. ent theoretical approaches have pro- 15 is what?” cannot be solved correctly.
More specifically, it is important in vided information regarding the cog- Mental representation skills are cognitive
young children to test number facts as nitive skills that are important for skills that enable an adequate mental
well as domain-specific mathematics young children in learning how to representation of the problem or task
and general conceptual knowledge in solve mathematical problems. Numeral (e.g., Geary, 1993; Montague, 1997; Ver-
VOLUME 37, NUMBER 1, JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2004 55

meer, 1997; Verschaffel, 1999). A men- tion symbol comprehension skills, lin-
tal representation is required in most guistic and relevance and number Cognition
word problems, because the simple sense skills, and mental representation Numeral comprehension and
translation of key words in a problem and relevance and number sense skills production
(e.g., “more”) into calculation proce- (Desoete, Roeyers, & Buysse, 2000). Operation symbol compre-
hension and production
dures (e.g., “addition”) without repre- It is nowadays widely accepted that
Number system comprehen-
sentation leads to blind calculation or metacognition also influences mathe-
sion and production skills
number crunching. This superficial ap- matical problem solving (Brown, 1987; Procedural skills
proach leads to errors, such as answer- Carr & Jessup, 1995; Desoete, Roeyers, Language comprehension
ing “24” to questions such as “15 is 9 & De Clercq, 2003a; Hacker et al., 1998; Context comprehension
more than __ ,” “27 is 3 less than __ ,” Lucangeli, Cornoldi, & Tellarini, 1998; Mental representation
and “48 is half of __.” Contextual com- Veenman, Kerseboom, & Imthorn, Selecting relevant informa-
prehension skills are cognitive skills that 2000; Verschaffel, 1999). In recent stud- tion
enable the solving of tasks in word ies, however, the term metacognition Number sense
problems consisting of more than one has come to be used in multiple and Metacognition
sentence (e.g., “Bert has 14 Digimon™ nearly unrelated senses to refer to a Metacognitive Knowledge
[Akiyoshi Hongo] cards. Griet has 5 wide range of phenomena (Borkowski, Declarative knowledge
Digimon cards more than Bert. How 1992; Carr & Biddlecomb, 1998; Schoen- Procedural knowledge
many cards does Griet have?”). In chil- feld, 1992; Wong, 1996). Metacognitive Conditional knowledge
dren with MLD, problems involving knowledge has been described as knowl- Metacognitive Skills
these tasks (cognitive complexity) edge and deeper understanding of Prediction
Planning
were found to be related to problems one’s own cognitive skills and prod-
Monitoring
with working memory (and cognitive ucts (Flavell, 1976, 1979). Within meta-
Evaluation
overload) and knowledge base and ex- cognitive knowledge, Cross and Paris Metacognitive Beliefs
pertise (Baddeley, 1999; Logie & Gil- (1988) and Jacobs and Paris (1987) dis- Self-concept
hooly, 1998; McCloskey & Macaruso, tinguished between declarative knowl- Self-efficacy
1995). Relevance skills are cognitive edge, procedural knowledge, and con- Motivation
skills enabling the solving of word ditional knowledge. Metacognitive skills Attribution
problems with irrelevant information relate to the voluntary control that peo- Conception of intelligence
included in the assignment (e.g., “Bert ple have over their own cognitive skills and learning
has 14 Pokémon™ [Nintendo] cards (Veenman, in press). The number of
and 3 Digimon cards. Griet has 5 Poké- metacognitive skills being distin- FIGURE 1. Mathematical problem
mon cards more than Bert. How many guished varies from 3 to 10 (Desoete, solving: A conceptual model.
Pokémon cards does Griet have?). 2001). Substantial data have been ac-
Children can have difficulty ignoring cumulated on 4 metacognitive skills:
and not using information (e.g., “3 prediction, planning, monitoring, and skills were measured either before or
Digimon cards”) in an assignment. evaluation (Lucangeli & Cornoldi, after the solving of exercises, we la-
Children with MLD often think that all 1997; Lucangeli et al., 1998). Simons beled this metacognitive component
the numbers have to be used in order (1996) described a third metacognitive off-line (measured) metacognition. In 165
to solve a mathematical problem, and component (metacognitive beliefs) as the third graders, we were able to differ-
will therefore answer “22.” Number broader general ideas and theories entiate between different mathematics
sense skills are the ninth category of (e.g., self-concept, self-efficacy, motiva- ability groups on the basis of the off-
cognitive skills. These skills enable the tion, attribution, and conceptions of in- line metacognitive component (Des-
solving of tasks such as “the answer to telligence and learning; see Figure 1) oete, Roeyers, & Buysse, 2001).
5 more than 14 is nearest to__. Choose that people have about their own and Recent studies have underlined the
between 5, 10, 15, 70 and 50.” The abil- other people’s cognition (McCombs, importance of several cognitive and
ity to estimate without calculating the 1989; Vermeer, 1997; Wong, 1996). A metacognitive skills for the purpose of
exact answer is called number sense. In principal components analysis of distinguishing between children with
clinical practice, almost all children metacognition revealed three metacog- MLD, children with MLP, and children
with MLD have problems with num- nitive components that explained 66% with age-appropriate mathematics per-
ber sense tasks (e.g., Sowder, 1992). In to 67% of the common variance (Des- formance in Flanders (Desoete, 2003;
our study, significant partial correla- oete, Roeyers, & Buysse, 2001). Predic- Desoete & Roeyers, 2002).
tions were found between all cognitive tion and evaluation were found to be In terms of cognition, several chil-
skills except between linguistic and interrelated as one of these compo- dren with combined domain-specific
procedural skills, linguistic and opera- nents. As both these metacognitive and automatization MLD were found
56 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

to have less developed language com- is, however, a certain risk of misinter- psychologist and the care coordinator
prehension skills (Desoete & Roeyers, preting these studies, because they (zorgcoördinator) then look at exercises
2001). Moreover, several children with cannot be automatically applied to in- done in the classroom and, if necessary,
domain-specific MLD were found to dividual children. Not all children give an additional mathematics test.
have less developed mental represen- with MLD were found to have the Special groups may be formed within
tation skills. However, children with same inadequate cognitive or metacog- the classroom for several hours during
merely automatization MLD (and age- nitive skills. For example, children the week in order to differentiate be-
appropriate domain-specific mathe- with a mathematics automatization tween fast and slow learners (broader
matics knowledge and skills) did not disability did not fail on linguistic, pro- care or zorgverbreding). In addition,
fail on mental representation tasks, cedural, or mental representation tasks STICORDI (Stimulating, Compensat-
and some even had higher scores than (Desoete & Roeyers, 2001). Moreover, ing, Relativating, and Dispensating)
peers without MLD (Desoete & Roey- only a small majority of the third measures can be agreed upon in some
ers, 2001). These findings support the graders with MLD had inaccurate off- kind of pedagogical charter. When
idea that some children with specific line metacognitive skills. Furthermore, broader care is insufficient, then bring-
MLD use blind calculation techniques a minority of the children without ing in a remedial teacher (taakleer-
depending on a simple translation of learning problems also had a severe kracht) is the usual next step. Here the
key words in an instruction. Finally, deficit (–2 SD) on off-line metacogni- children are given special exercises in a
several children with domain-specific tive skills (Desoete, 2001). special classroom for 6 months (on an
MLD had problems with procedural Taking all these findings together, individual basis or in small groups) in
skills, using several erroneous proce- what we see is a broad spectrum of an attempt to remedy their problems.
dures (Desoete & Roeyers, 2001; Van MLD that manifest themselves in a After 6 months to 1 year, children
Lehn, 1990). great variety of individual cognitive with resistant, severe problems with
In terms of metacognition, a majority and metacognitive profiles in young mathematics in primary school often
of children with MLD were found to children. It might, therefore, be impor- move to one of the rehabilitation cen-
have less accurate prediction skills tant to assess cognitive and off-line ters or centers specialized in handling
than their peers without LD. Younger metacognitive skills in children with educational problems to get a more
children outperformed all children MLD. Certainly procedural, linguistic, elaborate assessment. After an inter-
with MLD in the area of prediction mental representation, prediction, and view with the parents, the child, the
skills on tasks designed for first-grade evaluation skills have to be tested in teacher, and sometimes the school psy-
students (so-called easy tasks). Fur- order to detect whether these skills are chologist, and after an elaborate series
thermore, children with specific MLD appropriately developed. Moreover, a of tests, interviews, and observations,
had less accurate prediction skills re- general protocol of cognitive or meta- the diagnosis and label of LD is often
lating to number system knowledge cognitive intervention for all children given.
and procedural calculation. Also, chil- with MLD might represent overcon- Some of the children with LD are
dren with combined MLD and reading sumption of therapeutic resources be- also advised to repeat a year in general
disabilities were found to have less ac- cause not all individual children were education. In Flanders, 10% of all sixth
curate prediction skills in word prob- found to have below-average perfor- graders have repeated 1 or more years
lems involving language-related and mance on tasks depending on these of primary school (Hellinckx & Gh-
mental representation tasks. Moreover, skills. This leads us to the final section, esquière, 1999). In other cases, children
a majority of the children with MLD namely the inventory of the instruc- are referred to special education (Type
had less accurate evaluation skills than tional interventions and accommoda- 8), although, as previously mentioned,
their peers without LD. Furthermore, tions implemented to help students there is a strong movement toward in-
children with MLD had problems es- with MLD in Belgium. clusive education.
pecially in estimating their chances of In Flanders, young children of aver-
success on the easy tasks. Finally, chil- age intelligence up to the age of 15 who
dren with MLD scored worse in the Instruction for Children with have a learning disability, a score lower
evaluation of number knowledge and MLD in Belgium than Percentile 4 on a mathematics test,
procedural calculation than did younger and a need for a multidisciplinary ap-
children who were their equals in the When problems with mathematics are proach, can follow a course of therapy
area of mathematical problem solving detected, the teacher or parent can ask for 2 years (two or three times a week)
(Desoete, 2003; Desoete & Roeyers, the advice of the school psychologist. in a rehabilitation center. Moreover,
2002; Desoete, Roeyers, & De Clercq, In most cases, problems with mathe- there are some situations in which psy-
2001). matics are regarded as problems to be chologists and educators, and in some
Studies at the group level certainly solved within the classroom and cases also remedial teachers and care
revealed interesting information. There within the school system. The school coordinators, set up a special thera-
VOLUME 37, NUMBER 1, JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2004 57

peutic program for these children. This tween 3% and 8% of students in Flan- in general education in kindergarten.
program sometimes fits within the ders have MLD in primary school. Once the children are in primary
general education system, although for Moreover, MLD seems to be very per- school, the learning disabilities often
many children, it is provided by psy- sistent, leading to residual problems become obvious. With the new mathe-
chologists and other specialists outside with mathematics in adolescence and matics curriculum and the realistic ap-
the school. In other cases, when a spe- even in adulthood. proach (RME), additional research is
cific isolated mathematics retardation We have argued that although many certainly needed to determine their
of 1 year (for 7- to 9-year-olds) or 2 authors agree that an operational defi- effect on children with MLD. Once
years (for 9- to 14-year-olds) is found, nition of LD is meaningful, most stud- the problems with mathematics are
a monodisciplinary therapy is advised, ies are rather vague when it comes to detected, the school psychologist is
and the children go to private mathe- objectively defining the characteristics consulted. This psychologist often
matics therapists (in Flanders, often on the basis of which they identify chil- suggests a differentiated approach
specialized speech–language thera- dren with learning disabilities. Over- (broader care) or remedial teaching for
pists or specialized physiotherapists). lapping concepts and terms are fre- 6 months. If this is not sufficient, a
The different approaches taken to quently used to deal with a wide range more elaborate assessment is carried
therapy for children with MLD in Flan- of phenomena relating to children who out, and a therapy plan is established.
ders can vary greatly. However, most have difficulties with mathematical This therapy is partially financed by
therapists apply a rather cognitive/ problem solving. In an effort to be the government for a period of 2 years.
metacognitive and task-analytical more explicit, we have adopted three After this period, in theory, the MLD
therapy based on the learning of algo- criteria (discrepancy criterion, severe- should be remedied. However, in prac-
rithms and heuristics. The instruction ness criterion, and resistance criterion) tice, children in secondary or higher
principles are isolation (orientation, for ascertaining whether an individual education with MLD encounter even
demonstration, practice, shortening, child belongs to the group of children more difficulties, which may lead to a
control, working faster, knowing when with MLD. very low self-concept and to behavior
and where to use the principles), inte- Moreover, we have stressed the need problems. These findings indicate that
gration, and generalization (Ruijsse- for care in the assessment and diagno- more in-depth research is certainly
naars, 2001). This is frequently combined sis of children with MLD. More specif- needed on the prevention and early de-
with stimulating a realistic sense of self- ically, we have referred to the impor- tection of signals indicating potential
esteem in young children and a correct tance of using a combination of MLD in preschool children. Moreover,
identification of their successes and mathematical tests for young children, studies on adolescents and even adults
failures in school performance. Often, including at least one test of number with MLD would be useful in order to
therapists also use self-instructional facts and one test of domain-specific gain more insight into the emotional
techniques, for example to teach chil- mathematics knowledge or word prob- and behavioral impact of their disabil-
dren how to handle word problems lems, in order to avoid basing an entire ities.
(“read the instructions,” “see if you un- diagnosis on a single test. Further- The findings from recent studies also
derstand all the words,” “make a dia- more, we have found that teachers’ provided evidence that educational in-
gram or drawing of the situation,” judgments seem to be an absolute re- terventions for students with LD can
etc.). A minority of therapists in Flan- quirement for confirming the test re- produce positive effects of respectable
ders use a more psychomotor ap- sults. Our research revealed the impor- magnitude (Desoete & Roeyers, 2003;
proach to dealing with mathematical tance of also testing the reading skills Desoete, Roeyers, Buysse, & De Clercq,
problems in young children. However, of children with MLD in order to dif- 2002; Efklides, Papadaki, Papantoniou,

in many cases, a combination of thera- ferentiate children with a specific & Kiosseooglou, 1997; Masui & De
peutic approaches is used from a more mathematics learning disability from Corte, 1999; Swanson, Hoskyn, & Lee,
eclectic perspective. The form that the children with combined LD. Further- 1999). However, a number of problems
course of therapy actually assumes de- more, quite a number of children with that students with MLD encounter con-
pends to a great extent on the assess- MLD were found to have less devel- tinue to make themselves felt into
ment of the problems of the individual oped linguistic, procedural, and men- adulthood. In Belgium, most therapists
child. tal representation skills, and a majority were found to apply a rather cognitive/
of the children with MLD were found metacognitive therapy, based on the
to have inaccurate off-line metacogni- learning of algorithms and heuristics.
Summary tive skills. It may be advisable also to This approach was often found to be
assess the cognitive and metacognitive combined with efforts to remedy the
Flemish students seem to do quite well skills of young children with MLD. emotional aspects of the learning dis-
on mathematics compared with young- We further clarified that in Belgium, ability. In most cases, a combination of
sters in other countries. However, be- most young children with LD remain therapeutic approaches is used for an
58 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

individual child, depending on the as- REFERENCES toward comprehensive programming?


sessment results. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 250–
American Psychiatric Association. (1994).
It is important to keep in mind that 259.
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
we have restricted most of our studies Cobb, P., Yackel, E., & Wood, T. (1992). A
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Au-
constructivist alternative to the represen-
to third graders with MLD in Flanders. thor.
tational view of mind in mathematics ed-
Therefore, on several aspects, further Baddeley, A. (1999). Working memory. Ox-
ucation. Journal for Research in Mathemat-
research can be recommended. On the ford, UK: Clarendon Press.
ics Education, 23, 2–33.
one hand, cognition and off-line meta- Baroody, A. J., & Dowker, A. (2003). The de- Commissie Wiskundeleerplan van het
cognition needs to be studied in velopment of arithmetic concepts and skills: Gemeenschapsonderwijs. (1998). Wiskun-
younger and older children and in Constructing adaptive expertise. Mahwah, deleerplan [Mathematics curriculum].
children with below-average or above- NJ: Erlbaum. Brussels, Belgium: Author.
Blöte, A. W., Van der Burg, E., & Klein, A. S.
average intelligence. Moreover, indi- Cross, D. R., & Paris, S. G. (1988). Devel-
(2001). Students’ flexibility in solving opment and instructional analyses of
vidual studies of children with MLD
two-digit addition and subtraction prob- children’s metacognition and reading
are clearly needed to help us translate lems: Instruction effects. Journal of Educa-
the findings at the group level into re- comprehension. Journal of Educational
tional Psychology, 93, 627–638. Psychology, 80, 131–142.
medial measures specifically suited to Boekaerts, M. (1999). Metacognitive experi- De Clercq, A., Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H.
individual children. We believe that ences and motivational state as aspects of (2000). EPA2000: A multilingual, pro-
the research data derived from such self-awareness: Review and discussion. grammable computer assessment of off-
studies would improve our under- European Journal of Psychology of Educa- line metacognition in children with
standing of individuals with MLD. tion, 14, 571–584. mathematical learning disabilities. Behav-
In conclusion, we have found that Borkowski, J. G. (1992). Metacognitive the- ior Research Methods, Instruments, and
MLD can be very persistent. We have ory: A framework for teaching literacy, Computers, 32, 304–311.
also found that a well-elaborated as- writing, and math skills. Journal of Learn- DeMeyer, I., De Vos, H., Van De Poele, L.
ing Disabilities, 25, 253–257. (2002). Worldwide learning at age 15: first
sessment and intervention procedure
Bormans, L. (Ed.). (2002). Vlaamse jongeren results from PISA 2000. Ghent, Belgium:
is of key importance, because there ap-
zijn wereldtop. Klasse [Flemish young- Ministry of the Flemish Community and
pears to be a broad spectrum of MLD
sters head global classification]. Maand- the University of Ghent.
that come to expression in a broad blad voor Onderwijs in Vlaanderen, 121, 11. Desoete, A. (2001). Off-line metacognition in
range of highly individual cognitive Broekaert, E., & Van Hove, G. (1995). Spe- children with mathematics learning disabili-
and metacognitive profiles in young cial education Ghent. Volume I. Special ed- ties. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
children. ucation in the XXI century. Integration– RUG University, Ghent, Belgium.
school system (Orthopedagogische reeks Desoete, A. (2002). Dyscalculie: Stand van
Gent n° 7). Ghent, Belgium: CBOO. zaken [Dyscalculia: State of the art]. Tijd-
ABOUT THE AUTHORS Brown, A. (1987). Metacognition, executive schrift voor Logopedie and Audiologie,32(2),
control, self-regulation, and other more 57–74
Annemie Desoete, PhD, is a researcher at
mysterious mechanisms. In F. Reiner & R. Desoete, A. (2003). The assessment of off-
Ghent University in Ghent, Belgium. Her re-
Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, line metacognition in young children
search interests are in the area of mathematics
and understanding (pp. 65–116). Hillsdale, with mathematics learning disabilities.
learning disabilities. Herbert Roeyers, PhD, is
NJ: Erlbaum. Psychologie in Osterreich, 2/3, 71.
assistant professor of clinical psychology at
Campbell, J. I .D (1998). Linguistic influ- Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2000). Reken-
Ghent University. His research interests are in
ences in cognitive arithmetic: Comment stoornissen bij negenjarigen: Bepalen de
the area of developmental and behavioral dis-
on Noel, Fias, and Brysbaert (1997). Cog- tests de diagnose? [Mathematics learning
orders. Armand De Clercq, PhD, is professor
nition, 67, 353–364. disabilities in nine-year-olds: Does the
of applied mathematics. His research interests
Carr, M., & Biddlecomb, B. (1998). Meta- chosen test determine the diagnosis?].
are in the area of computer science. Address:
cognition in mathematics from a con- Diagnostiek-wijzer, 4, 3–16.
Annemie Desoete, Ghent University, Depart-
structivist perspective. In D. J. Hacker, Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2001). Het
ment of Clinical Psychology, Research Group
J. Dunlosky & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Meta- enigma van de rekenstoornis. Proce-
Developmental Disorders, Henri Dunantlaan
cognition in educational theory and practice durele, talige en representatiedeficieten
2, B-9000 Gent, Belgium; e-mail: Anne.
(pp. 69–91). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. bij achtjarigen met rekenstoornissen.
Desoete@UGent.be
Carr, M., & Jessup, D. L. (1995). Cognitive [The enigma of the mathematics learning
and metacognitive predictors of mathe- disability. Procedural, linguistic, and rep-
AUTHORS’ NOTE
matics strategy use. Learning and Individ- resentation deficits in eight-year-olds
ual Differences, 7, 235–247. with mathematics learning disabilities].
This study was supported by the Artevelde Cawley, J. F., & Miller, J. H. (1989). Cross- Significant. Electronisch Wetenschappelijk
College in Ghent and the Stichting Integratie sectional comparisons of the mathemati- Tijdschrift voor Klinische Research en Re-
Gehandidapten (SIG), to whom the authors ex- cal performance of children with learning views voor Revalidatie en Psychosociale
tend their thanks. disabilities: Are we on the right track Hulpverlening, 1, 1–18.
VOLUME 37, NUMBER 1, JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2004 59

Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2002). Off-line hance mathematical problem solving? to calculation problems of students with
metacognition: A domain-specific retar- Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 188– mathematics difficulties in elementary
dation in young children with learning 200. general education]. Tijdschrift voor Or-
disabilities? Learning Disability Quarterly, Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., & De thopedagogiek, 35, 243–259.
25, 123–139. Clercq, A. (2002). Assessment of metacog- Ginsburg, H. P. (1997). Mathematics learn-
Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2003, August). nitive skills in young children with math- ing disabilities: A view from develop-
Conditions for the training of prediction and ematics learning disabilities. In J. Carlson mental psychology. Journal of Learning
evaluation skills in mathematical learning of (Series Ed.) & D. Van der Aalsvoort, Disabilities, 30, 20–33.
Belgian third graders. Paper presented at a W. C. M. Sesing, & A. J. J. M. Ruijssenaars Grégoire, J. (1997). Diagnostic assessment
symposium at the 10th biennial confer- (Vol. Eds.), Advances in cognition and edu- of learning disabilities: From assessment
ence, Improving Learning: Fostering the cational practice: Vol. 7. Learning potential of performance to assessment of compe-
Will to Learn, European Association for assessment and cognitive training: Actual re- tence. European Journal of Psychological As-
Research on Learning and Instruction, search and perspectives in theory building sessment, 13, 10–20.
Padua, Italy. and methodology (pp. 307–333). Amster- Grégoire, J., & Van Nieuwenhoven, C.
Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & Buysse, A. dam, the Netherlands: JAI Press/Elsevier (1999). Le développement du comptage
(2000). Achtjarigen, waarbij rekenen Science. et son rôle dans les troubles num-
nooit routine wordt. Rekenstoornissen in Dumont, J. J. (1994). Leerstoornissen 1: Theo- ériques [Development of counting and its
Vlaanderen: Aard en prevalentie van de rie en model [Learning disabilities l: The- role in mathematics difficulties]. In S.
problematiek [Eight-year olds for whom ory and model]. (5th ed.). Rotterdam: Vinter & A. Ménissier, Les activités
arithmetic never becomes routine. Math- Lemniscaat. numériques. Opérations logiques et for-
ematics learning disabilities in Flanders: Efklides, A., Papadaki, M., Papantoniou, mulations langagières. [Mathematical ac-

Nature and prevalence of problems]. G., & Kiosseoglou, G. (1997). Effects of tivities. Logical operations and linguistic
Tijdschrift voor Orthopedagogiek, 10, 430– cognitive ability and affect on school formulations]. Besançon, France: Presses
441. mathematics performance and feelings of Universitaires Franc-Comtoises.
Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & Buysse, A. difficulty. American Journal of Psychology,
Grégoire, J., Van Nieuwenhoven, C., &
(2001). Metacognition and mathematical 110, 225–258.
Noel, J. (2003). TEDI-MATH Vlaanderen:
problem solving in Grade 3. Journal of Feys, R. (1998). Rekenen tot honderd. Basis-
Test in functie van de diagnostiek van basis
Learning Disabilities, 34, 435–449. vaardigheden en zorgverbreding [Calcula-
mathematische kennis en vaardigheden
Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & De Clercq, A. tion up to 100. Basic skills and expansion
[Functional diagnostic test of basic math-
(2001, August). The modifiability of cogni- of support]. Diegem, Belgium: Kluwer.
ematical knowledge and skills]. Brussels:
tion and metacognition in young children Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects
TEMA.
with mathematics learning disabilities. of problem solving. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.),
Hacker, D., Dunlosky, J., & Graesser, A.
Paper presented at the European Associ- The nature of intelligence (pp. 231–236).
(Eds.). (1998). Metacognition in educational
ation for Research in Learning and In- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
theory and practice: The educational psychol-
struction (EARLI), Fribourg, Switzer- Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cog-
ogy series. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
land. nitive monitoring, American Psychologist,
Hanich, L. B., Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., &
Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & De Clercq, A. 34, 906–911.
Dick, J. (2001). Performance across differ-
(2002a). Assessment of off-line metacog- Fletcher, J. M., & Morris, R. (1986). Classifi-
ent areas of mathematical cognition in
nitive skills in young children with math- cation of disabled learners: Beyond ex-
children with learning difficulties. Journal
ematics learning disabilities. Focus on clusionary definitions. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.),
of Educational Psychology, 93, 615–626.
Learning Problems in Mathematics, 24(2), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsy-
53–69. chological aspects of learning disabilities Hellinckx, W., & Ghesquière, P. (1999). Als
Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & De Clercq, A. (pp. 55–80). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. leren pijn doet: Opvoeden van kinderen met
(2002b). The measurement of individual Freudenthal, H. (1991). Revisiting mathemat- een leerstoornis [When learning hurts:
metacognitive differences in mathemati- ics education. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Educating children with learning disabil-
cal problem solving. In M. Valcke, D. Kluwer. ities]. Leuven, Belgium: Acco.
Gombeir, & W. C. Smith (Eds.), Learning Galle, C. (2001, December 5). Vlaamse Jacobs, J. E., & Paris, S. G. (1987). Children’s
styles: Reliability and validity. Proceedings of tieners leren goed maar haten school metacognition about reading: Issues in
the 7th annual ELSIN conference, Ghent [Flemish teenagers learn well but hate definition, measurement, and instruc-
University, Belgium (pp. 93–102). Ghent, school]. De Morgen, p. 7. tion. Educational Psychologist 22, 255–278.
Belgium: Academia Press. Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabili- Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (2000). What
Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & De Clercq, A. ties: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and definitions of learning disability say and
(2003a, July). The assessment of off-line genetic components. Psychological Bul- don’t say: A critical analysis. Journal of
metacognition in young children with math- letin, 114, 345–362. Learning Disabilities, 33, 239–256.
ematics learning disabilities. Paper pre- Ghesquière, P., Ruijssenaars, A., Grietens, Kennes, R. (Ed.). (2001). Inclusief beleid voor
sented at the 8th European Congress of H., & Luyckx, E. (1996). Een orthodidac- personen met een handicap: Voorbeelden uit
Psychology, Vienna. tische aanpak van rekenproblemen bij de beleidspraktijk [Inclusion of persons
Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & De Clercq, A. rekenzwakke leerlingen in het regulier with a disability: Examples from prac-
(2003b). Can off-line metacognition en- basisonderwijs [Orthodidactic approach tice]. Leuven, Belgium: Acco.
60 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

Kroesbergen, E. (2002). Mathematics educa- Miller, S. P., & Mercer, C. D. (1997). Educa- structional psychology (pp. 436–444). Ox-
tion for low-achieving students: Effects of tional aspects of mathematics disorders. ford, England: Elsevier Science.
different instructional principles on multipli- Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 47–56. Sowder, J. (1992). Estimation and number
cation learning. Doetinchem, the Nether- Milo, B. (2001, June). Recent research and re- sense. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of
lands: Gravant Educatieve Uitgaven. sults of intervention studies. Paper pre- research on arithmetics teaching and learn-
Lebeer, J. (2003). Bouwen aan leren leren: cog- sented at the Congress on Dyscalculia, ing: A project of the National Council of
nitieve leeerbevordering bij kinderen met Dyslexia, and Persistent Disabilities, Teachers of Arithmetics (pp. 371–387). New
risico op ontwikkelings of leerstoornissen Antwerp, Belgium. York: Simon & Schuster/Macmillan.
[Building on learning to learn: Cognitive Montague, M. (1997). Cognitive strategy in- Standaert, R. (2001). Inspectorates of educa-
learning enhancement in children at risk struction on arithmetics for students with tion in Europe. A critical analysis. Leuven,
for development of learning disorders]. learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Belgium: Acco.
Leuven, Belgium: Acco. Disabilities, 30, 164–177. Swanson, H. L. (2000). Issues facing the
Logie, R. H., & Gilhooly, K. (1998). Working Muthukrishna, N., & Borkowski, J. (1996). field of learning disabilities. Learning Dis-
memory and thinking: Current issues in Constructivism and motivated transfer of ability Quarterly, 23, 37–49.
thinking and reasoning. Hour, England: skills. In M. Carr (Ed.), Motivation in math- Swanson, H. L., Hoskyn, M., & Lee, C.
Psychology Press. ematics (pp. 63–87). Cresskill, England: (1999). Interventions for students with learn-
Lucangeli, D., & Cornoldi, C. (1997). Math- Hampton Press. ing disabilities: A meta-analysis of treatment
ematics and metacognition: What is the Noel, M. P. (2000). La dyscalculie dével- outcomes. New York: Guilford Press.
nature of the relationship? Mathematical oppementale: Un état de la question [De- Thiery, E. (2001, November 21). Persoon-
Cognition, 3, 121–139. velopmental dyscalculia : state of the art]. lijkheidsprofiel van het kind met leerstoor-
In M. Meseti & X. Seron (Eds.), Neuropsy- nissen [Personality profile of children
Lucangeli, D., Cornoldi, C., & Tellarini, M.
chologie des troubles du calcul et du traite- with learning disabilities]. Paper pre-
(1998). Metacognition and learning dis-
ment des nombres [Neuropsychology of sented at the 23rd congress of the
abilities in mathematics. In T. E. Scruggs
mathematics and numeracy difficulties]. Vlaamse Vereniging voor Logopedisten,
& M. A. Mastropieri (Eds.), Advances in
(pp. 59–84). Marseille, France: Solal. Wilrijk, Belgium.
Learning and Behavioral Disabilities (pp.
Pluspunt. (2002). Reken-wiskunde methode Thiery, E., & Anthonis, C. (2003, April).
219–244). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
voor de basisschool [Calculation and math- Neuropsychologische inzichten naar een
Mardulier, T. (2001). Inclusief onderwijs in
ematics method for the elementary gerichtere aanpak toe [Neuropsychological
Vlaanderen : Een stand van zaken [Inclu-
school]. Wommelgem, Belgium: Van In. insights toward a more directed ap-
sive education in Flanders: State of the
Pluspunt. (2003). Retrieved May 5, 2003, proach of dyscalculia]. Paper presented
art]. In R. Kennes, (Ed.) Inclusief beleid
from http://www.pluspunt-malmberg. at the meeting of the Posthogescho-
voor personen met een handicap: Voorbeelden
nl/home.htm olvorming Dyscalculie, Brugge, Belgium.
uit de beleidspraktijk (pp. 107–131). Leu-
Rivera-Batiz, F. L. (1992). Quantitative liter- Torbeyns, J., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquiere,
ven, Belgium: Acco.
acy and the likelihood of employment P. (2003, August). Strategy development in
Masui, C., & De Corte, E. (1999). Enhancing
among young adults in the United States. children with mathematical disabilities: Evi-
learning and problem solving skills: Ori-
Journal of Human Resources, 27, 313–328. dence for the developmental delay hypothesis.
enting and self-judging, two powerful
Rourke, B. P., & Conway, J. A. (1997). Dis- Paper presented at the 10th biennial con-
and trainable learning tools. Learning and
orders of arithmetics and mathematical ference, Improving Learning: Fostering
Instruction 9, 517–542.
reasoning: Perspectives from neurology the Will to Learn, European Association
McCloskey, M., & Macaruso, P. (1995). Rep- and neuropsychology. Journal of Learning for Research on Learning and Instruction,
resenting and using numerical informa- Disabilities, 30, 34–45. Padua, Italy.
tion. American Psychologist, 50, 351–363. Ruijssenaars, A. J. J. W. (2001). Leerproblemen Treffers, A. (1991). Didactical background
McCombs, B. L. (1989). Self-regulated en leerstoornissen. Remedial teaching en be- of a mathematics program for primary
learning and academic achievement: A handeling. Hulpschema’s voor opleiding en education. In L. Streefland (Ed.), Realistic
phenomenological view. In B. J. Zimmer- praktijk. [Learning problems and learning mathematics education in primary school
man & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated disabilities. Remedial teaching and treat- (pp. 21–56). Utrecht, the Netherlands:
learning and academic achievement: Theory, ment. Schemas for teachers and practi- Freudenthal Institute.
research, and practice. Progress in cognitive tioners]. Rotterdam: Lemniscaat. Van Borsel, J. (1998). Rekenproblemen lin-
development research (pp. 51–82). New Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think guïstisch bekeken [Mathematical learning
York: Springer Verlag. mathematically: Problem solving, meta- problems from a linguistic perspective].
Meltzer, L., Roditi, B., Houser, R. F., & Perl- cognition, and sense making in arith- Tijdschrift voor Logopedie en Audiologie, 28,
man, M. (1998). Perceptions of academic metics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook 6–11.
strategies and competence in students of research on arithmetics teaching and learn- Van Hove, G., & Roets, G. (2000). ‘Empow-
with learning disabilities. Journal of Learn- ing. A project of the National Council of erment’ en volwassenen met een ver-
ing Disabilities, 31, 437–451. Teachers of Arithmetics (pp. 334–370). New standelijke beperking [Empowerment
Mercer, C. D., Jordan, L., & Miller, S. P. York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan. and adults with mental retardation]. In
(1996). Constructivist math instruction Simons, P. R. J. (1996). Metacognition. In E. P. Ghesquière & J. M. A. M. Janssens
for diverse learners. Learning Disabilities De Corte & F. E. Weinert (Eds.), Interna- (Eds.), Van zorg naar ondersteuning: On-
Research & Practice, 11, 147–156. tional encyclopedia of developmental and in- twikkelingen in de begeleiding van personen
VOLUME 37, NUMBER 1, JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2004 61

met een verstandelijke handicap [From care den die relevant zijn voor reken- ucation: Part I. Backgrounds] (pp. 95–
to support: Recent developments in the wiskunde taken [Knowledge and skills: 128). Brussels, Belgium: Studiecentrum
services provided to persons with a men- Different knowledge and skills relevant voor Open Hoger Afstandsonderwijs.
tal disability] (pp. 41–52). Houten, the to calculation and mathematics tasks]. In Verschaffel, L. (1999). Realistic mathemati-
Netherlands: Bohn Stafleu Van Loghum. H. W. Bakker-Renes & C. M. Fennis-Poort cal modelling and problem solving in the
Van Lehn, K. (1990). Mind bugs: The origins (Eds.), Hulp bij leerproblemen: Rekenen– upper elementary school: Analysis and
of procedural misconceptions. Cambridge, Wiskunde [Aid for learning problems: improvement. In J. H. M. Hamers, J. E. H.
MA: MIT Press. Calculation–mathematics] (pp. G0050/ Van Luit, & B. Csapo (Eds.), Teaching and
Van Luit, J. E. H. (1992). De effectiviteit van 1–13). Alphen aan den Rijn, the Nether- learning thinking skills: Contexts of learning
traditionele (structuurverlenende) en re- lands: Samsom. (pp. 215–240). Lisse, the Netherlands:
alistische rekenwiskunde-methoden bij Veenman, M. V. J. (in press). The assessment Swets & Zeitlinger.
kinderen in het speciaal onderwijs [The of metacognitive skills: What can be Vlaams Verbond van het Katholiek ba-
effectiveness of traditional (structuralis- learned from multi-method designs? In sisonderwijs. (1999). Wiskundeleerplan
tic) and realistic mathematics methods on B. Moschner & C. Artelt (Eds.), Lern- [Mathematics curriculum]. Brussels, Bel-
children in special education]. In A. J. J. M. strategien und metakognition: Implikationen gium: Author.
Ruijssenaars & J. H. M. Hamers (Eds.), für forschung und praxis [Learning strate-
Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measur-
Rekenen als probleem. Praktijk en onderzoek gies and metacognition: Implications for
ing self-regulated learning. In M.
[The problem of calculation. Practice and research and practice].
Boekaerts, P. E. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner
research] (pp. 149–162). Leuven, Belgium: Veenman, M. V. J., Kerseboom, L., &
(Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp.
Acco. Imthorn, C. (2000). Test anxiety and
531–566). San Diego: Academic Press.
metacognitive skillfulness: Availability
Van Luit, J. E. H. (1994). The effectiveness of Wong, B. Y. L. (1996). Metacognition and
versus production deficiencies. Anxiety,
structural and realistic arithmetic curric- learning disabilities. In B. Y. L. Wong
Stress, and Coping, 13, 391–412.
ula in children with special needs. Euro- (Ed.), The ABCs of learning disabilities (pp.
Vermeer, H. (1997). Sixth-grade students’
pean Journal of Special Needs Education, 2, 120–139). San Diego: Academic Press.
mathematical problem solving behaviour:
16–26. World Health Organization. (1997). Interna-
Motivational variables and gender differ-
Van Luit, J. E. H. (1998). Rekenstoornissen. ences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, tional classification of impairments, activi-
[Mathematics disabilities]. Destelbergen, Leiden University, Leiden, the Nether- ties, and participation: A manual of dimen-
Belgium: Studiedag Strichting Integratie lands. sions of disablement and functioning.
Gehandicapten. Verschaffel, L. (1995). Ontwikkelingen in de Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Van Luit, J. E. H. (1999). Teaching mathe- opvattingen over en de praktijk van het Woodward, J., & Baxter, J. (1997). The ef-
matical thinking to children with special reken/wiskundeonderwijs op de basis- fects of an innovative approach to math-
needs. In J. H. M. Hamers, J. E. H. Van school [Shifts in the conceptualization ematics on academically low-achieving
Luit, & B. Csapo (Eds.), Teaching and learn- and practice of elementary mathematics students in inclusieve settings. Excep-
ing thinking skills: Contexts of learning (pp. education]. In L. Verschaffel & E. De tional Children, 63, 373–388.
241–257). Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Corte (Eds.), Naar een nieuwe reken/ Wuyts, B. (1996). Anders maar niet minder:
Zeitlinger. wiskundedidactiek voor de basisschool en de Leven met een handicap [Different but not
Veenman, M. V. J. (1998). Kennis en vaar- basiseducatie: Deel I. Achtergronden [To- lesser: Living with a handicap]. Leuven,
digheden: Soorten kennis en vaardighe- ward a new approach to mathematics ed- Belgium: Davidsfonds.
View publication stats

You might also like