You are on page 1of 12

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/253203051

From the Editors: What Grounded Theory Is Not

Article in The Academy of Management Journal · August 2006


DOI: 10.5465/AMJ.2006.22083020

CITATIONS READS

757 5,176

1 author:

Roy Suddaby
University of Victoria
84 PUBLICATIONS 6,621 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Craft, Magic and the Re-Enchantment of the World: A counterpoint to neo-institutionalism


View project

All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Roy Suddaby
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 11 October 2016
娀 Academy of Management Journal
2006, Vol. 49, No. 4, 633–642.

FROM THE EDITORS:


WHAT GROUNDED THEORY IS NOT
ROY SUDDABY
University of Alberta

Editor’s Note. Three years ago, I invited Robert In the manuscripts I review for AMJ I have seen the
(Bob) Gephart to write a “From the Editors” column term “grounded theory” used to describe analysis
designed to help authors improve their chances of via correlations, word counts, and pure introspec-
success when submitting qualitative research to tion. I am not suggesting that these techniques can-
AMJ. Judging from the increasing number of quali- not be used in a grounded theory study. But I note,
tative studies that have been accepted and pub- with some concern, that “grounded theory” is often
lished in AMJ since that time, I would like to think used as rhetorical sleight of hand by authors who
that his article, “Qualitative Research and the are unfamiliar with qualitative research and who
Academy of Management Journal,” has had a pos- wish to avoid close description or illumination of
itive impact. their methods. More disturbing, perhaps, is that it
Continuing in this tradition, I asked Roy Sud- becomes apparent, when one pushes them to de-
daby—an excellent reviewer (and author) of quali- scribe their methods, that many authors hold some
tative research—to tackle another “big issue” that serious misconceptions about grounded theory.
the editorial team has noticed with respect to qual- What are these misconceptions? Before reviewing
itative submissions to AMJ: overly generic use of them, I offer you a short description of what grounded
the term “grounded theory” and confusion regard- theory is. Like most difficult subjects, grounded the-
ing alternative epistemological approaches to qual- ory is best understood historically. The methodology
itative research. Like Bob before him, Roy has, I was developed by Glaser and Strauss (1967) as a
believe, produced an analysis that will greatly ben- reaction against the extreme positivism that had per-
efit those who are relatively new to qualitative re- meated most social research. They disputed the view
search or who have not yet had much success in that the social and natural sciences dealt with the
getting their qualitative research published. Hope- same type of subject matter. Specifically, Glaser and
fully, Roy’s analysis will help even more authors to Strauss challenged prevalent assumptions of “grand
succeed, thus allowing AMJ and other journals to theory,” the notion that the purpose of social research
continue to increase the quality of insights pro- is to uncover preexisting and universal explanations
vided by rich qualitative studies of individual, or- of social behavior. In making their challenge, Glaser
ganizational, and institutional phenomena. and Strauss looked to the pragmatism of Charles
Sara L. Rynes Saunders Peirce (1839 –1914) and early symbolic in-
teractionists, particularly George Herbert Mead
I was not particularly surprised to observe that (1863–1931) and Charles Cooley (1864 –1929), each
most of the articles identified as “interesting re- of whom rejected the notion that scientific truth re-
search” in a recent AMJ survey were the product of flects an independent external reality. Instead, they
qualitative methods (Bartunek, Rynes, & Ireland, argued that scientific truth results from both the act of
2006). New discoveries are always the result of observation and the emerging consensus within a
high-risk expeditions into unknown territory. Dar- community of observers as they make sense of what
win, Columbus, and Freud, each in different ways, they have observed. In this pragmatic approach to
were conducting qualitative inquiries. social science research, empirical “reality” is seen as
I am continually surprised, however, by the pro- the ongoing interpretation of meaning produced by
found misunderstanding of what constitutes quali- individuals engaged in a common project of
tative research. Such confusion is most apparent observation.
when authors claim to be using “grounded theory.” Glaser and Strauss (1967) proposed grounded
theory as a practical method for conducting re-
search that focuses on the interpretive process by
Thanks to Sara Rynes for suggesting and commenting analyzing the “the actual production of meanings
on this paper. Thanks also to Chet Miller, Jean Bartunek, and concepts used by social actors in real settings”
Kristine Fitch, Chris Quinn-Trank, and Marvin Washing- (Gephart, 2004: 457). They argued that new theory
ton for very helpful comments on early drafts. could be developed by paying careful attention to
633
634 Academy of Management Journal August

the contrast between “the daily realities (what is SIX COMMON MISCONCEPTIONS
actually going on) of substantive areas” (Glaser &
Grounded Theory Is Not an Excuse to Ignore the
Strauss, 1967: 239) and the interpretations of those
Literature
daily realities made by those who participate in
them (the “actors”). They also rejected positivist A common misassumption is that grounded the-
notions of falsification and hypothesis testing and, ory requires a researcher to enter the field without
instead, described an organic process of theory any knowledge of prior research. There are several
emergence based on how well data fit conceptual variants of this myth, each based on the false
categories identified by an observer, by how well premise that the researcher is a blank sheet devoid
the categories explain or predict ongoing interpre- of experience or knowledge. An extreme variant is
tations, and by how relevant the categories are to the notion that not only must the researcher enter
the core issues being observed. Most significantly, the field with a blank mind (i.e., without knowl-
Glaser and Strauss offered a compromise between edge of the literature and absent prior experience),
extreme empiricism and complete relativism by ar- but that she or he must also enter the field with a
ticulating a middle ground in which systematic blank agenda (i.e., without a defined research ques-
data collection could be used to develop theories tion). A less extreme, but more problematic, ver-
that address the interpretive realities of actors in sion suggests that the researcher must defer reading
social settings. existing theory until the data are collected and
The method described by Glaser and Strauss analyzed. This notion is reflected in manuscripts
(1967) is built upon two key concepts: “constant whose authors avoid any mention of prior literature
comparison,” in which data are collected and ana- until their papers’ discussions or concluding sec-
lyzed simultaneously, and “theoretical sampling,” tions. In a similar variant, researchers use grounded
in which decisions about which data should be theory to tackle a subject that is in “well-tilled
collected next are determined by the theory that is soil”—that is, a subject that has attracted a long and
being constructed. Both concepts violate long- credible history of empirical research—and use
standing positivist assumptions about how the re- grounded theory as a justification for ignoring prior
search process should work. Constant comparison research in formulating their study.
contradicts the myth of a clean separation between Leaving aside the question of whether it is even
data collection and analysis. Theoretical sampling possible to disregard one’s prior knowledge and
violates the ideal of hypothesis testing in that the experience, the idea that reasonable research can be
direction of new data collection is determined, not conducted without a clear research question and
by a priori hypotheses, but by ongoing interpreta- absent theory simply defies logic. Such research, as
tion of data and emerging conceptual categories. Ronald Coase famously observed, is likely to pro-
Grounded theory, therefore, is a method that is duce a random “mass of descriptive material wait-
more appropriate for some questions than others. ing for a theory, or a fire” (Coase, 1988: 230). To-
Clearly, it is most suited to efforts to understand the tally unstructured research produces totally
process by which actors construct meaning out of unstructured manuscripts that are unlikely to make
intersubjective experience. Grounded theory it past the desk editor at any credible journal of
should also be used in a way that is logically con- social science.
sistent with key assumptions about social reality The notion of using grounded theory as an ex-
and how that reality is “known.” It is less appro- cuse to forgo examining extant literature is perhaps
priate, for example, to use grounded theory when more problematic because it is often based on a
you seek to make knowledge claims about an ob- researcher’s desire to discover something new. This
jective reality, and more appropriate to do so when desire most often manifests when researchers use
you want to make knowledge claims about how grounded theory to tackle well-established areas of
individuals interpret reality. empirical inquiry—leadership, for example. In
Such fine distinctions between abstract catego- most cases, the researchers honestly hope to gain
ries are perhaps more easily seen through concrete fresh insights by keeping out of the ruts early trav-
examples. From my reviewing experience for AMJ elers have worn. They thus avoid formal reviews of
and other management journals, I have identified relevant literature in their manuscripts to create the
six common misconceptions about grounded the- impression that their observations and analytic cat-
ory. Although I make no claim that these categories egorizations of data were not colored by previous
are exhaustive, they accurately reflect my experi- ideas.
ence and offer, I think, a reasonable assessment of Unfortunately, this approach is also based on a
common errors researchers make in conducting serious misreading of the seminal texts in grounded
and presenting grounded theory research. methodology. Although Glaser and Strauss were
2006 Suddaby 635

motivated against grand theory, their formulation Grounded Theory Is Not Presentation
of grounded theory was never intended to encour- of Raw Data
age research that ignored existing empirical knowl-
I occasionally see papers that start with an ap-
edge. They distinguished between substantive the-
propriate and interesting question, are written well,
ory, or theory grounded in extant research in a
and follow a well-constructed method, but produce
particular subject area (e.g., leadership), and
findings that are obvious or trite. You’ve seen such
grounded theory, but they observed a direct and
papers; they conclude that entrepreneurs are risk
necessary link between the two forms of theory:
takers, change is difficult, and leaders are charis-
Substantive theory is a strategic link in the formu- matic. The common thread in such research is that
lation and generation of grounded formal theory. We it tends to present incomplete or relatively undi-
believe that although formal theory can be generated gested data.
directly from data, it is more desirable, and usually
This unfortunate outcome is usually the result of
necessary, to start the formal theory from a substan-
tive one. The latter not only provides a stimulus to a
one of three errors in the practice of grounded
“good idea” but it also gives an initial direction in theory research. First, there may be some confusion
developing relevant categories and properties and in between grounded theory and phenomenology.
choosing possible modes of integration. Indeed it is Phenomenological research emphasizes the subjec-
difficult to find a grounded formal theory that was tive experiences of actors’ “lifeworlds” (Husserl,
not in some way stimulated by substantive theory. 1969; Schutz, 1972). Methodologically, phenom-
(Glaser & Strauss, 1967: 79) enologists attempt to capture the rich, if not mun-
The real danger of prior knowledge in grounded dane, detail of actors’ lived experiences. They often
theory is not that it will contaminate a researcher’s present data in relatively raw form to demonstrate
perspective, but rather that it will force the re- their authenticity and to permit a holistic interpre-
searcher into testing hypotheses, either overtly or tation of the subjects’ understanding of experience.
unconsciously, rather than directly observing. Such data are typically analyzed through some-
Grounded theory methodologists describe a num- what introspective techniques that permit a clear
ber of ways to prevent this from happening. One is focus on the relationship between the language
to avoid research that adheres too closely to a single used and the objects to which language relates
substantive area and, instead, draw from the sev- (Moustakas, 1994).
eral substantive areas that are frequently reflected Although grounded theory retains some sympa-
in a given daily reality. Stephen Barley’s (1986, thy for phenomenological assumptions and tech-
1990) research on technology and structuring, for niques, researchers using grounded theory are less
example, succeeds because it draws from (at least) focused on subjective experiences of individual ac-
two areas of substantive research—technological tors per se and are instead more attentive to how
change and structuration theory—that are both of- such subjective experiences can be abstracted into
ten germane in the same research contexts. Another theoretical statements about causal relations be-
technique is to be continuously aware of the possi- tween actors. The difference between these two
bility that you are being influenced by preexisting approaches can be seen in how each uses the tech-
conceptualizations of your subject area. Particu- nique of interviewing. In a phenomenological
larly when working through well-tilled soil, study, in-depth interviews are a key means of prob-
grounded theorists must retain the capacity to ing individuals’ subjective experiences (Wimpenny
“make the familiar strange” (Spindler & Spindler, & Gass, 2000). The detail and nuance of the stories
1982). A final solution is to try not to overextend interviewees elaborate and the specific words they
the objective of grounded theory research. That is, choose comprise the primary unit of analysis. Be-
researchers may shoot for “the elaboration of exist- cause phenomenology is an effort to probe the lived
ing theory” rather than untethered “new” theory. experience of subjects without contaminating the
None of these approaches justifies ignorance of data (Moustakas, 1994), units of data are often pre-
existing literature or knowledge. The reality of sented in their raw form. In grounded theory, by
grounded theory research is always one of trying contrast, interviews with subjects may start with a
to achieve a practical middle ground between a phenomenological interest in subjective under-
theory-laden view of the world and an unfettered standings, but the primary interest is not in the
empiricism. A simple way to seize this middle stories themselves. Rather, they are a means of elic-
ground is to pay attention to extant theory but iting information on the social situation under ex-
constantly remind yourself that you are only hu- amination. In contrast to phenomenological stud-
man and that what you observe is a function of ies, grounded theory studies rarely have interviews
both who you are and what you hope to see. as their sole form of data collection.
636 Academy of Management Journal August

The second grounded theory application error practice should be encouraged—there must be
that I see as producing obvious and trite output is some degree of congruence between the research
failure to “lift” data to a conceptual level. Again, question (i.e., a researcher’s assumptions about the
this failure may arise from the epistemological nature of reality and how one might know reality)
problem of confusing grounded theory with phe- and the methods used to address the question. A
nomenology. It is more likely, however, that the realist ontology rests on the assumption that the
researcher has simply failed to completely analyze variables of interest exist outside individuals and
the data. A key element of grounded theory is iden- are, therefore, concrete, objective, and measurable
tifying “a slightly higher level of abstraction— (Burrell & Morgan, 1979). An “interpretivist” ontol-
higher than the data itself” (Martin & Turner, 1983: ogy rests on the contrasting assumption that human
147. The movement from relatively superficial ob- beings do not passively react to an external reality
servations to more abstract theoretical categories is but, rather, impose their internal perceptions and
achieved by the constant interplay between data ideals on the external world and, in so doing, ac-
collection and analysis that constitutes the con- tively create their realities (Morgan & Smircich,
stant comparative method. The failure of data to 1980). From this perspective, the key variables of
coalesce into definable conceptual structures that interest are internal and subjective.
move beyond the obvious may well be the result of Keep in mind that the purpose of grounded the-
a researcher’s failure to thoroughly work between ory is not to make truth statements about reality,
data and extant knowledge in an effort to find the but, rather, to elicit fresh understandings about pat-
best fit or the most plausible explanation for the terned relationships between social actors and how
relationships being studied (Locke, 2001). these relationships and interactions actively con-
Finally, the researcher who has produced sim- struct reality (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Grounded
plistic output may simply have stopped collecting theory thus should not be used to test hypotheses
data too early. Unlike more traditional, positivist about reality, but, rather, to make statements about
research, grounded theory offers no clean break how actors interpret reality. As Martin and Turner
between collecting and analyzing data. Rather, a (1986) observed, grounded theory is best used
researcher must continue to collect data until no when no explicit hypotheses exist to be tested, or
new evidence appears. This process, called “cate- when such hypotheses do exist but are too abstract
gory saturation,” is one of the primary means of to be tested in a logical, deductive manner. This is
verification in grounded theory (Strauss & Corbin, where grounded theory is most appropriate—
1998). Premature departure from the field may well where researchers have an interesting phenomenon
result in data that are only partly analyzed and without explanation and from which they seek to
therefore fail to elevate obvious categorizations to a “discover theory from data” (Glaser & Strauss,
more abstract theoretical level. 1967: 1).
There are some qualifications to my statement
that grounded theory is not theory testing, content
Grounded Theory Is Not Theory Testing, Content
analysis, or word counts. Although grounded the-
Analysis, or Word Counts
orists do not use Popperian notions of falsification
I regularly see papers in which the authors at- as a technique for making statements about reality,
tempt to use grounded theory methods to test hy- they do “test” their tentative ideas and conceptual
potheses. These fall into a larger category of manu- structures against ongoing observations. A key
scripts whose authors have engaged in the sloppy component of the constant comparative method is
practice of methodological slurring (Goulding, such critical evaluation of emerging constructs
2002), using interpretive methods to analyze “real- against ongoing observations. Similarly, although
ist” assumptions. In most cases the manuscripts grounded theory is not to be confused with content
begin with clear sets of positivist assumptions, in- analysis or word counting, both techniques can
cluding hypotheses, and then proceed to report form part of grounded theory studies. The impor-
“tests” of the hypotheses with sets of interviews or tant difference is that grounded theory describes an
counts of words in relevant publications. In other overall method for systematically gathering and an-
cases manuscripts will start with interpretive pre- alyzing data, but content analysis describes a spe-
mises, such as the social construction of reputation cific context within which a distinct type of data
in the popular business press, and then report word can be gathered and analyzed. Typically, in a
counts, with the claim of having performed grounded theory study, content analysis is only one
grounded theory. of multiple contexts for acquiring data. Word
Although there is nothing wrong with combining counting, which is a subset of content analysis, is
qualitative and quantitative methods—in fact, the somewhat more problematic in that it bears posi-
2006 Suddaby 637

tivist assumptions about the relationship between In framing our introduction and the following con-
word frequency and meaning, most of which vio- ceptual overviews, we have employed the theoreti-
late the interpretivist assumptions of grounded the- cal concepts that actually emerged from the study.
In the grounded, interpretive research approach
ory (Krippendorff, 2003).
used here, the theoretical concepts and framework
Another important qualification to my comments are grounded in and emerge from the data and anal-
about methodological slurring arises from the way ysis that follow (Glaser & Strauss, 1967; Strauss and
in which grounded theory tends to be presented in Corbin, 1990), rather than being derived from prior
journal articles. Even though grounded theory re- theory that guided data collection and analysis. A
search is conducted iteratively, by analyzing and “purist” rendition of interpretive research reporting
collecting data simultaneously, it is usually pre- would, however, entail a lengthy and complex qual-
itative data presentation before the reader learns
sented sequentially. This gap occurs because the
what the major theoretical dimensions and contri-
norms of presentation in management (and other butions are likely to be. In other words, the theory
academic) journals have positivist origins and im- would normally appear after the data presentation
pose discrete and sequential categories of data col- (Dact, 1985). We suspend this interpretive reporting
lection and analysis on authors trying to present hallmark for the sake of advance clarity, and employ
grounded theory research. In pure form, grounded the more traditional presentational strategy of pro-
theory research would be presented as a jumble of viding a theoretical overview first, to preview the
literature consultation, data collection, and analy- major findings and resulting model. It is important
to keep in mind, however, that these concepts actu-
sis conducted in ongoing iterations that produce
ally emerged from the study itself (along with con-
many relatively fuzzy categories that, over time, sultations with relevant literature that were guided
reduce to fewer, clearer conceptual structures. The- by the emerging thematic analysis). (Anonymous,
ory would be presented last. provided by editor)
Presenting grounded theory in this pure form,
however, would be neither efficient nor compre- I like this caveat to the presentation of grounded
hensible to the majority of researchers who work in theory because it succinctly avoids the impression
the positivist paradigm. The norm that has evolved of methodological slurring by identifying the
is to present grounded theory in the traditional messy, nonlinear reality of grounded theory re-
discrete categories and in the same sequence as search and, simultaneously, providing a practical
justification (i.e., comprehensibility) for presenting
quantitative research: theory, data collection, data
the data and analysis in a traditional “sanitized”
analysis, results. Doing so has the unfortunate con-
format.
sequence of creating the impression of methodolog-
The key objection to mixing grounded theory
ical slurring, even when the constant comparative
with hypothesis testing, however, is that doing so
method has been used. For those unfamiliar with tends to violate the notion of theoretical emer-
grounded theory techniques, the mode of presenta- gence. That is, when a researcher uses grounded
tion may also create the unfortunate impression theory techniques to “test” preconceived notions of
that grounded theory methods can be mixed with a what is likely to be observed, chances are he or she
positivist research agenda. will “see” the intended categories and overlook
There are ways of avoiding this incorrect impres- more emergent ones. This form of methodological
sion while adhering to the sequential norms of slurring tends toward “forced categories” in the
journal presentation. Foremost, the process of data coding process (Glaser, 1992) and reduces
analysis, including coding techniques and category grounded theory technique from its intended pur-
creation, should be made apparent to the reader. pose of identifying new theory to one of simply
Authors can do this in their methods sections and, confirming extant understandings of a social phe-
in my opinion, such accounts are most effective nomenon as a consequence of the researcher im-
when the authors provide illustrative examples of posing intentions on the data.
coding techniques and the evolution of conceptual
categories in a table or appendix.
Similarly, authors can note that, although they Grounded Theory Is Not Simply Routine
are presenting theoretical concepts in a traditional Application of Formulaic Technique to Data
manner (i.e., up front in the study), the concepts Some manuscripts reinforce the myth that
did, in fact, emerge from the study. Perhaps one of grounded theory is a relatively mechanical tech-
the most eloquent versions of this statement came nique. This myth has several manifestations. One is
from a manuscript recently reviewed in the Acad- to present grounded theory as a series of rigid rules,
emy of Management Journal: such as “saturation is achieved when one has con-
638 Academy of Management Journal August

ducted between 25 and 30 interviews.” Another is into what the data are reflecting— because he or she
the implication that pouring textual data into a fails to see what they really indicate except in terms
software package will yield results. A common of trivial or well known phenomena—then the pub-
characteristic of most efforts to use grounded the- lished findings fail on this criterion. Because there
is an interplay between researcher and data, no
ory is a neurotic overemphasis on coding. The re-
method, certainly not grounded theory, can en-
searcher has diligently followed the rules and gone sure that the interplay will be creative. Creativity
from open coding (derived from an initial reading depends on the researcher’s analytic ability, the-
of the data) to more abstract or categorical codes oretical sensitivity, and sensitivity to the subtle-
and finally to conceptual or theoretical codes but ties of the action/interaction (plus the ability to
has made no interpretive effort at any stage of cod- convey the findings in writing). A creative inter-
ing. The result is typically a nice set of conceptual play also depends on the other pole of the re-
categories that, in the process of routine data anal- searcher-data equation, the quality of the data col-
ysis, become divorced from both the data and the lected or analyzed. An unimaginative analysis
original research question. That is, although the may in a technical sense be adequately grounded
in the data, yet be insufficiently grounded for the
rigid application of grounded theory technique
researcher’s theoretical purpose. This occurs if
might produce passable results, such a mechanical
the researcher does not draw on the complete
approach usually lacks the spark of creative insight resources of data or fails to push data collection
upon which exemplary research is based. far enough. (Corbin & Strauss, 1990: 19).
The key issue to remember here is that grounded
theory is an interpretive process, not a logico-de- Those new to grounded theory research must be-
ductive one. Positivist models of science encourage come both patient and tolerant of ambiguity, be-
the notion that researchers stand separate from ob- cause it is the ongoing interaction between re-
jects of inquiry in order to minimize the degree to searcher and data that generates the fundament of
which the act of observation interferes with or con- successful grounded research.
taminates the observation. In interpretive models,
this assumption is not so firmly held. The re- Grounded Theory Is Not Perfect
searcher is considered to be an active element of
the research process, and the act of research has a In reading reviews of manuscripts that undertake
creative component that cannot be delegated to an grounded theory, I detect a growing fundamental-
algorithm. Qualitative software programs can be ism in grounded theory research. That is, there
useful in organizing and coding data, but they are seems to be a growing gap between those who ac-
no substitute for the interpretation of data. The tually engage in grounded theory and those who
researcher must make key decisions about which write about it. The latter group, unsurprisingly,
categories to focus on, where to collect the next tends toward purist idealism and, as a result, they
iteration of data and, perhaps most importantly, the repeat and reinforce many of the myths I have
meaning to be ascribed to units of data. described above: rigid rules about saturation, me-
Successful grounded theory research has a clear chanical application of technique to data, and clear
creative component. Glaser and Strauss were aware demarcation between theory and data. The gap be-
of this component and the tension it would create tween pragmatics and purists has become exacer-
with those who find comfort in trusting an algo- bated by a proliferation of how-to manuals and
rithm to produce results. Glaser (1978) used the textbooks about simplifying and streamlining
term “theoretical sensitivity” to describe the essen- grounded theory research.
tial tension between the mechanical application of Let me be clear that my intent is not to criticize
technique and the importance of interpretive in- the emergence of a community of scholars devoted
sight. The tension ultimately proved to be a point of to improving grounded theory methodology. This
departure between the founders of grounded the- has occurred in the context of quantitative research
ory, with Glaser favoring creativity and openness to as well. There ought to be a healthy tension be-
unanticipated interpretations of data while Strauss tween pure methodologists and practitioners.
(and coauthor Juliet Corbin) became advocates of Keep in mind, however, the pragmatic core of
adherence to formal and prescriptive routines for grounded theory research. It was founded as a prac-
analyzing data (Locke, 1996). Even so, Corbin and tical approach to help researchers understand com-
Strauss continued to caution against an overly me- plex social processes. It was also designed as a
chanical application of method: method that might cccupy a pragmatic middle
ground between some slippery epistemological
If the researcher simply follows the grounded theory boundaries. Because of this genealogy, grounded
procedures/canons without imagination or insight theory techniques are inherently “messy” (Parkhe,
2006 Suddaby 639

1993) and require researchers to develop a tacit tion “is the process of forming an explanatory hy-
knowledge of or feel for when purist admonitions pothesis. It is the only logical operation which in-
may not be appropriate to their research and may troduces any new idea” (Peirce, 1903: 216). Peirce
be ignored. also described abduction as the fallible “flash of
This statement is not an excuse for ignorance of insight” that generates new conceptual views of the
the epistemological and ontological issues sur- empirical world. The notion of abduction has be-
rounding grounded theory methods. Quite the op- come incorporated into grounded theory as “ana-
posite; it speaks to the need for practitioners to lytic induction,” the process by which a researcher
know this terrain well, for it is only through under- moves between induction and deduction while
standing the philosophical underpinnings that one practicing the constant comparative method. Strauss
can safely navigate the thorny questions of when, and Corbin noted that induction had been overem-
for example, one’s method is consistent with a par- phasized in grounded theory research. They observed
ticular view of reality. But being aware of one’s that whenever researchers conceptualize data, they
epistemological position does not justify dogma- are engaging in deduction and that effective grounded
tism about conducting grounded theory research. theory requires “an interplay between induction
Ultimately, questions of when saturation is achieved, and deduction (as in all science)” (1998: 137).
how coding should be done, or when counting is In sum, a healthy tension between methodolo-
appropriate can be resolved pragmatically. gists and practitioners is desirable, but researchers
Perhaps an example will clarify these issues of should try to avoid fundamentalist tendencies in
messiness and tacitness. A key point of confusion how they approach and, more importantly, evalu-
in grounded theory research is the question of ate grounded theory research.
knowing when saturation has occurred during data
collection. Because grounded theory research uses Grounded Theory Is Not Easy
iteration and sets no discrete boundary between
I often get the impression, when reading some of
data collection and analysis, saturation is not al-
the more egregious examples of how not to do
ways obvious, even to experienced researchers. Yet
grounded theory research, that the methodology
every submitted manuscript contains a statement
suffers from its apparent simplicity. That is, much
that saturation occurred. It is a “box” that must be
like modern art or the sparse prose of Ernest Hem-
“checked off” prior to publication.
ingway, examples of outstanding grounded theory
Deciding saturation has happened takes tacit un-
raise the immediate reaction, “Hey, I could do
derstanding, which is achieved as much through
that!” The seamless craft of a well-executed
experience as through a priori criteria. The indeter- grounded theory study, however, is the product of
minate, messy nature of saturation invites a funda- considerable experience, hard work, creativity and,
mentalist drift toward positivism—the 25 interviews. occasionally, a healthy dose of good luck.
But, according to Glaser and Strauss, saturation is a Many of the primary techniques of grounded the-
practical outcome of a researcher’s assessment of ory research are developmental. That is, the quality
the quality and rigor of an emerging theoretical of their application improves with experience. The
model: “The criteria for determining saturation . . . developmental nature of grounded theory research
are a combination of the empirical limits of the derives, in part, from individual differences in re-
data, the integration and density of the theory and searchers’ ability to interpret patterns in qualitative
the analyst’s theoretical sensitivity” (1967: 62). The data (Turner, 1981). Grounded theory is an inter-
signals of saturation, which include repetition of pretive process that depends upon the sensitivity of
information and confirmation of existing concep- a researcher to tacit elements of the data or mean-
tual categories, are inherently pragmatic and de- ings and connotations that may not be apparent
pend upon both the empirical context and the re- from a mere superficial reading of denotative con-
searcher’s experience and expertise. tent. Many grounded theory researchers describe
There are other examples of the pragmatic mid- this interpretion as occurring subconsciously, as a
dle road of grounded theory. Fundamentalists often result of their constant “immersion” in the da-
incorrectly describe quantitative approaches as ta—an event that some describe as akin to “drown-
necessarily deductive and grounded theory as in- ing” (Langley, 1999).
herently inductive. Practically, neither could ever Although the subjective elements of grounded
be true. Pierce recognized that pure induction and theory research confer an advantage on some indi-
pure deduction are necessarily sterile. New ideas viduals, I do not mean to suggest that this bars
result from a combination of these fundamental others from ever conducting grounded theory re-
approaches, which he termed “abduction.” Abduc- search. Rather, the advantage some enjoy suggests
640 Academy of Management Journal August

that we who are less gifted with intuitive skills or Grounded Theory Is Not an Excuse for the
pattern-perceiving abilities require more course Absence of a Methodology
work, training, and experience to acquire these sen-
This essay was motivated by a recurring problem in
sitivities. Indeed, the genius of Glaser and Strauss’s manuscripts I see, in which researchers claim to have
original methodology is that it outlines a procedure performed grounded theory research, support their
by which formerly tacit processes are made ex- claims with a cursory citations to Glaser and Strauss
plicit. The apparent simplicity of grounded theory (1967), and then offer little, if any, description of their
research, however, creates the misperception that it methodology. If a revision is granted and the authors
is easy and encourages many without prior experi- are pushed to reveal how the data were collected and
ence or education in grounded theory methodology analyzed, it becomes clear that the term “grounded
to “have a go.” Not surprisingly, the product of theory” was interpreted to mean “anything goes.”
such an effort will be similar to the product of a Data may have been collected randomly, coded by
researcher untrained in statistical methods “having forced application of preexisting conceptual catego-
a go” at LISREL. ries, and used to test hypotheses. In sum, the research
Exemplary research using grounded theory also either ignored or deliberately violated the core proce-
requires considerable exposure to the empirical dures and tenets of grounded theory methodology
context or subject area of research. Contradicting (Locke, 1996).
prevalent ideals of scientific detachment from con- When I review a paper containing a claim of
text, the constant comparative method implies an grounded theory, I check to ensure that, at a
intimate and enduring relationship between re- minimum, the authors have described their meth-
searcher and site. Because of this close and long- odology transparently enough to reassure me that
standing connection, the personality, experience, they followed core analytic tenets (i.e., theoreti-
and character of a researcher become important cal sampling, constant comparison) in generating
components of the research process and should be the data and that I can reasonably assess how the
made an explicit part of the analysis (Strauss & data were used to generate key conceptual cate-
Corbin, 1998). gories. I’m also interested to see indicia of the
Two significant consequences flow from this. researchers’ theoretical sensitivity—their open-
First, in grounded theory, researchers must account ness to new or unexpected interpretations of the
for their positions in the research process. That is, data, the skill with which they combine litera-
they must engage in ongoing self-reflection to en- ture, data, and experience, and their attention to
sure that they take personal biases, world-views, subtleties of meaning. I look for indications of
and assumptions into account while collecting, in- consistency between research questions (and
terpreting, and analyzing data. Such continuous their assumptions about the world and how peo-
ple come to understand it) and the methods used
reflection is not easy, particularly for those unac-
to answer the questions. Finally, I am very atten-
customed to a model of science in which the re-
tive to the researchers’ use of technical language
searcher is an important focus of the process.
in describing their methodology because I believe
Second, because the somewhat artificial bound-
there is a clear connection between rigor in lan-
ary between researcher and research subject is re-
guage and rigor in action.
moved, the quality of the contact between re-
My observations about the lapses I have observed
searcher and empirical site and the quality of the in reviewing manuscripts may create the unfortu-
research produced have a direct relationship. As a nate impression that few exemplary models of
result, most high-quality grounded theory research grounded theory research exist. This is, of course,
arises from an extensive and ongoing commitment simply not true. There are many and, at the risk of
to a line of theoretical research and an empirical offending a large number of exceptional grounded
site. Consider as an illustration Karl Weick’s com- theory researchers, I invite you to read one of my
mitment to studying firefighters’ “sensemaking” or favorites. Isabella’s (1990) study on managers’ in-
Hinings and Greenwood’s commitment to studying terpretation of processes of organizational change
organizational change in the professions. Such is an outstanding exemplar of grounded theory
illustrative examples are supported by empirical methodology. The methods section states a re-
research showing that the more time researchers search strategy in which the epistemological link
spend on-site in organizational contexts, the between research question and methodology is
more they report high levels of “self-learning,” clear. The process by which interview subjects
and the more their research is cited by peers were identified and selected is made transparent,
(Rynes, McNatt, & Bretz, 1999). as is the mode by which raw data were converted to
2006 Suddaby 641

conceptual categories. The author offers sufficient emy of Management Journal. Academy of Manage-
detail in the data presented to create a sense of ment Journal, 47: 454 – 462.
verisimilitude for the reader but also lifts the data Gersick, C. J. G. 1988. Time and transition in work teams:
by weaving ongoing interpretations, experience, Toward a new model in group development. Acad-
and prior literature into a model of evolving man- emy of Management Journal, 31: 9 – 41.
agerial interpretations of organizational change. Glaser, B. G. 1978. Theoretical sensitivity. Mill Valley,
The study incorporates all of the key analytic tenets CA: Sociology Press.
and procedures of grounded theory and, at the Glaser, B. G. 1992. Basics of grounded theory analysis.
same time, demonstrates a high degree of theoreti- Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press.
cal sensitivity in the researcher. It’s also a very
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. 1967. The discovery of
readable study.
grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative re-
Many other exemplars of grounded theory re- search. New York: Aldine.
search—for instance, Sutton (1987), Gersick (1988),
and Browning, Beyer, and Shetler (1995)—share Goulding, C. 2002. Grounded theory: A practical guide
for management, business and market research-
the devotion to transparency and rigor described
ers. London: Sage.
above. I encourage you to read them, both as how-to
manuals and as sources of inspiration and motiva- Husserl, E. 1969. Cartesian meditations: An introduc-
tion to engage in grounded theory research. One of tion to phenomenology [D. Cairsn, trans.]. The
Hague: Martinus-Nijhoff.
the more satisfying elements of conducting re-
search in management is that, in contrast to the Isabella, L. A. 1990. Evolving interpretations as change
physical sciences, social science research looks at unfolds: How managers construe key organizational
how human invention continually generates new events. Academy of Management Journal, 33:
7– 41.
ways of interaction and organization. Researchers
can best understand those new modes of interact- Krippendorff, K. 2003. Content analysis: An introduc-
ing and organizing by using a methodology that is tion to its methodology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
attentive to issues of interpretation and process and CA: Sage.
that does not bind one too closely to long-standing Langley, A. 1999. Strategies for theorizing from process
assumptions. Fortunately, that’s precisely what data. Academy of Management Review, 24: 691–
grounded theory is. 710.
Locke, K. 1996. Rewriting the discovery of grounded
theory after 25 years? Journal of Management In-
REFERENCES quiry, 5: 239 –245.
Locke, K. 2001. Grounded theory in management re-
Barley, S. R. 1986. Technology as an occasion for struc-
search. London: Sage.
turing: Evidence from the observations of CT scan-
ners and the social order of radiology departments. Martin, P. Y., & Turner, B. A. 1986. Grounded theory and
Administrative Science Quarterly, 31: 78 –109. organizational research. Journal of Applied Behav-
ioral Science, 22: 141–157.
Barley, S. R. 1990. The alignment of technology and
structure through roles and networks. Administra- Morgan, G., & Smircich, L. 1980. The case for qualitative
tive Science Quarterly, 35: 61–104. research. Academy of Management Review, 5:
491–500.
Bartunek, J. M., Rynes, S. L., & Ireland, R. D. 2006. What
makes interesting research and why does it matter? Moustakas, C. 1994. Phenomenological research meth-
Academy of Management Journal, 49: 9 –15. ods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Browning, L. D., Beyer, J. M., & Shetler, J. C. 1995. Build- Parkhe, A. 1993. “Messy” research, methodological pre-
ing cooperation in a competitive industry: SEMAT- dispositions and theory development in interna-
ECH and the semiconductor industry. Academy of tional joint ventures. Academy of Management Re-
Management Journal, 38: 113–151. view, 18: 227–268.
Burrell, G., & Morgan, G. 1979. Sociological paradigms Peirce, C. S. 1903. The essential Pierce: Selected phil-
and organisational analysis: Elements of the soci- osophical writings, vol. 2. Bloomington: Indiana
ology of corporate life. London: Heinemann. University Press.
Coase, R. 1988. The firm, the market, and the law. Rynes, S. L., McNatt, D. B., & Bretz, R. D. 1999. Academic
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. research inside organizations: Inputs, processes, and
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. 1990. Grounded theory research: outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 52: 869 – 898.
Procedures, canons and evaluative criteria. Qualita- Schutz, A. 1972. The phenomenology of the social
tive Sociology, 13: 3–21. world. London: Heinemenn Educational Books.
Gephart, R. P. 2004. Qualitative research and the Acad- Spindler, G., & Spindler, L. 1982. Roger Harker and
642 Academy of Management Journal August

Schonhausen: From the familiar to the strange and Disbanding and reconnecting. Administrative Sci-
back. In G. Spindler (Ed.), Doing the anthropology ence Quarterly, 32: 542–569.
of schooling: 21– 46. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Turner, B. A. 1981. Some practical aspects of qualitative
Winston. data analysis: One way of organizing the cognitive
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. 1998. Basics of qualitative re- processes associated with the generation of grounded
search: Techniques and procedures for developing theory. Quality and Quantity, 15: 225–247.
grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Wimpenny, P., & Gass, J. 2000. Interviewing in phenom-
Sage. enology and grounded theory: Is there a difference?
Sutton, R. I. 1987. The process of organizational death: Journal of Advanced Nursing, 31: 1485–1492.

You might also like