You are on page 1of 10

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179

Research report

A sensitizing regimen of amphetamine that disrupts attentional


set-shifting does not disrupt working or long-term memory
Robert E. Featherstone a,∗ , Zoe Rizos a , Shitij Kapur b,c , Paul J. Fletcher a,b,d
a Section of Biopsychology, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, 250 College Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5T 1R8
b Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Canada
c Schizophrenia/PET Centre, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Canada
d Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Canada

Received 18 September 2007; received in revised form 11 December 2007; accepted 27 December 2007
Available online 20 January 2008

Abstract
Exposure to an intermittent, escalating dose of amphetamine induces a sensitized state that, both behaviourally and neurochemically, mirrors
several features linked to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Increasingly it is being realized that cognitive deficits are a core component
of schizophrenia; therefore we sought to assess the effects of inducing an amphetamine-sensitized state on memory (working and long-term) and
cognitive flexibility, two cognitive domains impaired in schizophrenia. Rats were exposed to a sensitizing regimen of amphetamine (1–5 mg/kg;
three times per week for 5 weeks; escalating at 1 mg/kg per week) or saline. In experiment 1, animals were tested on an operant delayed non-match
to position task (working memory). Experiment 2 used a standard fixed-platform location water maze task (long-term memory), while experiment 3
used a variable-platform location water maze task (long-term memory and working memory). Amphetamine-sensitized animals were not impaired
on any of these tasks. In experiment 4, animals were assessed on a strategy selection task in which they were first required to learn to locate
a food reward using a particular learning strategy (place or response) then to learn to shift to an alternate learning strategy (response or place).
Amphetamine-sensitized animals were not impaired on this task. In the final experiment animals were found to be impaired in performance of the
extra-dimensional shift component of an attentional set-shifting task. These results suggest that while amphetamine sensitization does not produce
memory impairments similar to those seen in schizophrenia, it does produce strong impairments in set-shifting, suggesting changes in prefrontal
function similar to those seen in schizophrenia.
© 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Animal model; Schizophrenia; Dopamine; Cognition; Amphetamine sensitization; Morris water maze; Delayed non-matching to position; Attentional
set-shifting

1. Introduction rupted in schizophrenia, such as pre-pulse inhibition [44,51–53]


although see [31,43], latent inhibition [30,31,43,53], attentional
Amphetamine sensitization has been suggested to model vigilance [8], sustained attention [18] and attentional set-shifting
some aspects of schizophrenia [30,31,40,42,51–53]. Briefly, [13,16]. In the latter task, amphetamine-sensitized animals
amphetamine sensitization refers to a process whereby responses displayed an impairment in making an extra-dimensional atten-
to amphetamine increase through repeated exposure to the drug tional shift, as well as difficulties in reversal learning, similar to
[40]. This increased responsiveness is maintained during with- what has been found in animals with prefrontal lesions [1,28],
drawal from the drug, and has been shown to last up to 1-year post schizophrenic patients [34] and human amphetamine abusers
drug exposure in rats [35]. Following the induction of sensitiza- [32].
tion, animals show impaired performance on a number of tasks These findings suggest that amphetamine sensitization repro-
thought to involve psychological processes similar to those dis- duces many of the attentional and pre-attentional deficits asso-
ciated with schizophrenia. What is less well known, however, is
the extent to which amphetamine sensitization can model other
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 416 535 8501x6242; fax: +1 416 979 6942. areas of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia is
E-mail address: Rob Featherstone@camh.net (R.E. Featherstone). associated with impairments in both working [20] and long-term

0166-4328/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.12.032
R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179 171

memory [7,24]. In non-human primates, exposure to a sensi- (experiment 1: saline n = 12 and amphetamine n = 12; experiment 2: saline n = 10
tizing regimen of amphetamine produced short-term deficits in and amphetamine n = 10; experiment 3: saline n = 10 and amphetamine n = 10.
performance of a delayed response task, a measure of working A single group of animals (saline n = 9 and amphetamine n = 10) were run on the
strategy-shifting (experiment 4) and attentional set-shifting tasks (experiment 5).
memory. In contrast, a brief exposure regimen of amphetamine
(2.5 mg/kg for 5 days) did not disrupt performance of a radial
2.2. Amphetamine sensitization
maze based delayed alternation task in rats [50]. Regarding long-
term memory, one study found that amphetamine sensitization Amphetamine injections (IP) took place three times a week (Monday,
(1–5 mg/kg, three injections per day for 6 days) failed to disrupt Wednesday and Friday) for 5 weeks. During week 1, amphetamine treated ani-
acquisition or retention of a fixed platform position water maze mals received a dose of 1 mg/kg (from salt), with the dose being escalated by
task in rats [41]. In contrast, animals exposed to a repeated sin- 1 mg/kg each subsequent week (dose during the final week was 5 mg/kg). Control
animals received saline. All injections were administered in a 1 ml/kg volume.
gle dose of amphetamine (3 mg/kg, five injections over 10 days)
showed deficits in an object recognition memory task which
2.3. Experiment 1: delayed non-match to position
required the retention of information about objects encountered
2 or 4 h earlier [2], suggesting that some forms of long-term DNMTP training took place in standard operant conditioning chambers (Med
memory may be impaired in sensitized animals. Thus, there Associates, St. Albans, Vermont). Each chamber was equipped with a pellet dis-
are conflicting results from animal studies on the impact of the penser, two retractable response levers, a pellet magazine, two stimulus lights
amphetamine-sensitized state on memory and it is possible that and an infrared photobeam located in the pellet magazine on the front wall. The
pellet magazine was located in the lower centre of the chamber and the response
this could relate to differences between the various procedures levers were located to the left and right of the magazine. One stimulus light
used to induce the sensitized state. was located above each response lever. Additionally, two opaque barriers (5 cm
In our work an intermittent schedule of amphetamine, where deep × 0.5 cm wide), running the height of the chamber were positioned per-
the drug is given three times per week over 5 weeks, with the pendicular to the front wall, separating each response lever from the magazine.
dose escalating at a rate of 1 mg/kg per week, induces a variety of Animals were first trained to respond on a FR1 schedule of reinforcement,
with both response levers available during these sessions. Each session lasted for
behavioural deficits when animals are tested several weeks after 30 min or until 100 responses on either lever had been made. This training lasted
drug treatment [18,51–53]. These deficits include poor atten- for four sessions. Animals were then trained to respond to signalled presentations
tional set-shifting and sustained visual attention. The present of the response lever. At the start of each trial, a stimulus light was illuminated and
studies extended these findings to examine the impact of this the response lever immediately below that stimulus light was extended into the
regimen of amphetamine on working and long-term memory. chamber. Animals were required to press the lever within 20 s in order to receive
reinforcement. Once a response had occurred, the stimulus light was turned off
Working memory was assessed on an operant delayed non- and the response lever was retracted. If no response occurred, the stimulus light
match to position (DNMTP) task [12]. Three experiments were was turned off and the lever was retracted, followed by a 20 s time-out period.
conducted that assessed long-term memory. The first of these Each session consisted of 50 presentations of each lever, for 100 trials, and train-
examined the effect of amphetamine sensitization on long-term ing on this phase lasted for three to five sessions. Animals were then trained on the
memory in a standard fixed platform position water maze task. DNMTP task. Each DNMTP trial consisted of a sample phase and a choice phase.
The sample phase was initiated by the extension of a response lever into the
The second experiment used a more demanding variable plat- chamber, along with illumination of the corresponding stimulus light. A response
form position water maze task, in which animals were required on this lever caused the stimulus light to extinguish, the lever to retract, and the
to locate a hidden platform in a novel position each day. In the delay period to begin. During the delay period animals were required to nose-
final experiment, animals were tested on a strategy shifting task poke in the food magazine. The first nose-poke response after the delay period
which required learning to locate a food reward on a plus maze had timed out triggered the start of the choice phase. Here, both response levers
were extended into the chamber, and both of the stimulus lights were illuminated.
using a particular type of memory (place or response) followed To receive reinforcement, animals were required to respond on the lever that was
by a shift to a different type of memory (response or place). Inter- not present during the sample phase. All DNMTP sessions consisted of 100 trials.
ference with the prefrontal cortex produces a selective deficit in Accuracy, defined as the percentage of trials with a correct response, was the pri-
the ability to switch memory strategies [37] and performance mary measure of performance and was calculated separately for each delay value.
of this and similar tasks is dependent upon prefrontal dopamine Both the barriers and the nose-poke requirement during the delay periods were
used to minimize the use mediating strategies that rats could use to solve the task.
function [19,37–39]. This task was included in order to assess the Animals were initially trained only at the shortest delay (100 trials at 1 s
impact of amphetamine sensitization on prefrontal control over delay). Once performance had reached a level of 85% accuracy, the next delay
learning and memory processes. Finally, experiment 5 assessed was introduced (i.e. 1 s and 4 s delay, 50 trials each). Further delays were gradu-
the effects of amphetamine sensitization on an attentional set- ally phased in until rats were being tested with delays of 1, 4, 8, 16 and 24 s, with
shifting task [1,28] since we have previously found impaired 20 trials at each delay period per session. Once behaviour had stabilized, animals
were given a further 10 sessions of training prior to the start of drug treatment
attentional set-shifting in the amphetamine-induced sensitized (Baseline phase). Based on average performance on this baseline phase rats were
state [16]. matched to two groups to receive amphetamine or saline injections. During the 5
weeks amphetamine sensitization period, animals were given DNMTP sessions
2. Methods on non-injection days (Tuesday and Thursday) (sensitization phase). After the
sensitization phase had finished testing was conducted three times per week
2.1. Subjects (Monday, Wednesday and Friday) for 5 weeks (withdrawal phase).

Male, Spraque–Dawley (Charles Rivers, Saint-Constant, Quebec) rats 2.4. Experiment 2: fixed platform location water maze
weighing between 275 and 300 g at the beginning of each experiment were
used. Animals were housed in a room maintained at 22 (±2) ◦ C and were kept The water maze was constructed from a circular, white plastic tub, 1.5 m in
on a 12:12 light/dark cycle (lights on at 08:00). Eighty-three animals were used diameter and 79 cm deep. The platform was made from clear Plexiglas (15 cm
172 R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179

in diameter and 50 cm tall). Water was maintained at 20–22 ◦ C and a small of the arms (although only the response made during the first trial was considered
amount of tempura paint (approximately 100 ml) was added to render the water indicative of a turn bias). Seven series of trials were conducted with each animal,
opaque. The platform was submerged 2 cm below the surface of the water. A with each subsequent series starting from the start point immediately clockwise
video camera was mounted directly above the water maze, which allowed each from the last. An animal was considered to have a turn bias for whichever turn
session to be recorded for later analysis. it made most frequently during this phase of training.
Training on the fixed platform water maze task took place 30 days following Once this phase had been completed, training began on the strategy-shift
the last amphetamine injection and consisted of three phases, acquisition, storage task. Training required 2 days, with acquisition training taking place on the first
and retention. Acquisition: animals were given six daily sessions of training, day and training on the shift occurring on the following day. During acquisition
with each session consisting of four trials; each trial began from one of four training, approximately one-half of the animals were trained to use a response
equidistant start positions and lasted for 60 s, or until the rat successfully located strategy (e.g., turn left) to locate food reward, while the other half were trained
the platform. Animals were allowed to remain on the platform for 10 s. In the to use a place strategy (e.g., go to the north position). On the second day, animals
event that an animal did not find the platform during the 60-s trial, it was gently had to learn to locate food reward using the alternate strategy to that used on
picked up and placed onto the platform for a 10-s period. The latency to locate the first day. Animals were trained until they performed the correct response 10
the submerged platform served as the primary measure of behaviour. Storage: times consecutively, at which point they were given a probe trial from a novel
after the sixth session of training, animals were given two probe trials. The start position. A probe trial was considered correct if the animal chose the arm
first probe trial occurred immediately following the end of training. The second consistent with the strategy it had been trained to use. In the event of a correct
probe trial was given on the next day, 24 h after the first probe trial. During each probe trial, training was considered to be complete. If an incorrect choice was
probe trial, the platform was removed and the animal was started from a novel made, animals were retrained on the task until they had reached an additional
start position. Each trial lasted for 60 s. The number of times that the animal criterion of five consecutive correct responses, at which time an additional probe
crossed the position where the platform had been during training was recorded. trial was given. This process continued until a correct choice was made during
An identically sized area located in the opposite quadrant was used as a control a probe trial. The procedure for training on the strategy-shift was identical to
location, and the number of crossings of this location was also recorded. Long- that used during acquisition. Animals were considered to have mastered the shift
term retention: in order to assess long-term retention of the platform position, when they were able to complete 10 consecutive correct trials, followed by a
subjects were given a final probe trial 10 days following the end of the second correct probe trial. Probe trials were conducted in a manner identical to that used
phase of training, followed by two sessions of re-acquisition training. during acquisition.

2.5. Experiment 3: variable platform location water maze 2.7. Experiment 5: attentional set-shifting task

The procedure for the variable platform location water maze was similar to Animals were given 4 weeks rest following radial maze training before begin-
that of the acquisition phase of the fixed location version, with the exceptions that ning training on the attentional set-shifting task. Training occurred in a chamber
(1) the platform was located in a different position during each session, and (2) constructed from black Plexiglas (60 cm long × 42 cm wide × 30 cm tall). The
animals received eight trials per session (two from each of the four equidistant chamber was divided into a front and rear compartment, with the front compart-
start positions), instead of four. Animals were run sequentially, such that an ment divided into two separate compartments (21.5 cm long × 20.5 cm wide). An
intertrial interval of approximately 20 min separated each trial for each animal. opaque barrier separated the front and rear chambers, and this barrier was low-
At the end of acquisition training, the task was modified to allow for assessment ered at the start of each trial such that the animal, placed in the rear compartment,
of short-term memory [49]. Animals were given a single initial trial on each of 3 was blocked from accessing the front chamber. Each of the two compartments in
days using a novel platform location. After a fixed delay period (16 s, 1 h or 24 h), the front chamber contained a small ceramic bowl (7 cm in diameter and 4 cm in
animals were given a second trial, with the platform located in the same position depth) that was covered with a texture (fine sandpaper, course sandpaper, velvet,
as on the first trial. The experiment was counterbalanced such that, for any given the reversed side of the velvet material, paper or plastic wrap), and was filled
day, roughly equal numbers of animals were tested at each delay period. with scented bedding material (Bed-O’cobs, The Andersons Inc., Maumee, OH,
scented with paprika, thyme, cinnamon, cumin, cloves or sage).
The procedure for set-shifting has been previously described [14,16].
2.6. Experiment 4: strategy-shifting Animals were first shaped to dig for the food reward (half piece of Froot-
Loop©cereal). The cereal was initially placed directly on top of the bedding and,
Training took place on an eight arm radial maze constructed from dark grey over time, as animals learned to retrieve this food, was placed deeper within the
Plexiglas. Each individual arm (60 cm long and 9 cm wide) was attached to a bedding, eventually requiring the animal to learn to dig to obtain the food. This
central octagonal platform, measuring 40 cm across, elevated 60 cm above the training took place for as many trials as were needed for animals to reliably (six
floor. An insert was placed into the centre of the maze that constricted the shape trials in a row) dig and retrieve food from the bowl. Animals were then trained on
of the central octagonal platform into a plus shape. Additionally, an insert was a simple discrimination, in which food was paired with one stimulus dimension
used to block individual arms when needed. Four metal cups, in which the food (either odor or texture) but not the other. The stimuli during this discrimination
reward (a half piece of Froot-Loop©) would be located, were placed at the far were banana and vanilla or masking tape and paper towel. Training continued
end of each arm. until the animals chose correctly over six consecutive trials.
The procedures for this task followed those of [37]. Training began with a One the next day, training began on the full set of discriminations. Two
series of habituation sessions, designed to accustom the animals to eating on the bowls were placed in the front chamber of the apparatus, with one bowl
maze and to being repeatedly handled by the experimenter. During the first day baited with a half piece of Froot-Loop©and the other empty. The location
of training, five half pieces of Froot-Loop©were placed within each arm of the of the baited bowl was randomly determined on each trial to prevent the
maze (two half-pieces in the food cup and three half-pieces on the arm), and the development of a positional bias. The bowls differed according to odor and
animals were given 15 min of free access to the maze. Arms were rebaited in texture, with food being consistently associated with a particular odor or texture
the event that an animal ate all of the Froot-Loops©. From days 2 to 6, animals depending on the current discrimination. Animals were first trained on a simple
underwent a similar procedure, with the exception that only one half-piece of discrimination involving an exemplar from a single stimulus dimension (either
Froot-Loop©was placed in each food cup, and that animals were picked up and odor or texture). During the next stage of training (complex discrimination),
placed back into the centre of the maze after consuming each Froot-Loop©piece. an exemplar from the same stimulus dimension was added, although food
Sessions lasted for 15 min or until the animal had depleted the maze four times. remained paired with the original stimulus (e.g., odor 1+/odor 2−, or texture
On the final day of habituation, animals were tested for turn bias. One arm of 1+/texture 2−). The third discrimination (reversal 1) was arranged such that the
the maze was blocked and the animal was started from the arm opposite to the previously irrelevant exemplar (e.g., odor 2 or texture 2) was now paired with
blocked arm. Thus, an animal was forced to make either a left or right hand turn food while the previously relevant exemplar was not. The fourth discrimination
when it reached the end of the arm. Both arms were baited with food, and animals (intra-dimensional shift, IDS) required animals to choose between a new series
were given enough trials so that they were able to successfully find food in both of exemplars, with the previously relevant stimulus dimension still being paired
R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179 173

with food. This discrimination was followed by a reversal (reversal 2) in which


the alternate exemplar of the relevant stimulus dimension was now paired with
food. The sixth discrimination (extra-dimensional shift, EDS) required animals
to shift attention to the previously irrelevant stimulus dimension while the final
discrimination involved a reversal of the extra-dimensional shift discrimination
in which the animal had to choose the previously irrelevant exemplar of
the new stimulus dimension. Animals were determined to have mastered a
discrimination once they had made six correct responses in a row.

2.8. Locomotor activity testing

For each experiment, locomotor responsiveness to amphetamine was


assessed following completion of behavioural training. The monitoring system
consisted of 16 Plexiglas cages, each of which was equipped with 6 photo-
beam cells capable of detecting horizontal movement. The purpose of this stage
of testing was to confirm that amphetamine sensitization had, indeed, occurred.
Animals were given three, 2 h sessions of habituation training, followed by a test
day. On the test day, animals were given 30 min in which to habituate to the boxes,
at which point they received an injection of amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg). Activity
was then assessed for 60 min. For experiments 1–3, locomotor activity testing
took place between days 60 and 80 of amphetamine withdrawal. Animals from
experiments 4 and 5 were given locomotor testing around day 100 of withdrawal.

3. Results

3.1. Experiment 1: DNMTP

Fig. 1 depicts data from the DNMTP task. Rats were assigned
to either the amphetamine or saline groups based on baseline
testing, such that the two groups had equivalent levels of per-
formance. As such, no significant differences were found for
Fig. 1. Graph depicts DNMTP performance in saline treated (white circle) and
group or group × delay during baseline testing. A significant
amphetamine-sensitized (black circle) animals. Graph A depicts average choice
main effect was found for delay, with performance decreasing accuracy during both of the drug free days (Tuesday and Thursday) for each
as the delay increased [F(4, 88) = 139.04, p < 0.05]. For testing week of sensitization (S1–S5). Graph B depicts average choice performance for
that took place during amphetamine sensitization, data from the each week during the 5-week withdrawal period (W1–W5). Although animals
two sessions per week were averaged for each of the 5 weeks. displayed a delay-dependent decrease in accuracy throughout each session, no
group differences were observed during sensitization (A: S1–S5) or withdrawal
Separate repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted on the
(B: W1–W5). Error bars depict S.E.M.
data from each week, using group as the between subject fac-
tor and delay as the repeated within subject factor. A significant
main effect of delay was found for each of the 5 weeks [small-
est F(4, 88) = 56.18, p < 0.05]. Thus, accuracy declined with
increases in the delay period. Neither the main effect of group nor
the group × delay interaction terms were significant, indicating
that there were no differences between saline and amphetamine-
sensitized animals in the performance of this task. Data from the
5 weeks of testing during withdrawal were averaged across each
week (three sessions per week) and separate repeated measures
ANOVAs were conducted for each week. Again a significant
main effect was found for delay during each week (smallest
F(4, 88) = 68.61, p < 0.05), indicating that accuracy deteriorated
as the length of the delay period increased. Neither group nor
the group × delay interaction term was significant, showing that
amphetamine sensitization had no detrimental impact on the
performance of this task.
Fig. 2. Graph depicts acquisition of the fixed platform water maze task in saline
3.2. Experiment 2: fixed platform location water maze treated (saline) and amphetamine-sensitized (AMPH) animals. The figure shows
the average latency (s) over each of the four daily trials that comprised each ses-
sion. Amphetamine-sensitized animals showed significantly longer latencies to
A repeated measure ANOVA was carried out on latency
find the platform on sessions 1, 4 and 6 (p < 0.05). No other significant differ-
to find the platform, using group as a between-subject vari- ences were found. Error bars depict S.E.M. Asterisks denote significant group
able and session as a within-subject variable. Fig. 2 depicts differences.
174 R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179

animals in the two groups making significantly more crossings


of the location where the platform had been during training in
comparison to a similar location in the opposite quadrant. Nei-
ther group nor the group by platform interaction was significant
during either probe test.
To assess long-term retention of the platform position, ani-
mals were given a single probe test 10 days following the end
of acquisition training. A repeated measure ANOVA was con-
ducted on the number of platform location crossings, with group
as the between subjects factor and location (number of cross-
ings of the trained platform location versus a neutral location
in the opposite quadrant) as the within subjects factor. A sig-
nificant main effect was observed for platform location [F(1,
18) = 24.5, p < 0.05], showing that animals in both groups made
Fig. 3. Graph depicts the number of crossings of the previously trained platform a greater number of crossings of the trained platform location
position vs. a control location (opposite quadrant) in saline treated (saline) and than they did the neutral location (see Fig. 3). No significance
amphetamine-sensitized (AMPH) animals during probe trials conducted imme- was observed for the main effect of group or the group × location
diately, 24 h or 10 days after the end of training. Both groups crossed the platform interaction.
position significantly more often than the control condition during each of the
three test periods (p < 0.05). There were no differences between the two groups.
Error bars depict S.E.M. 3.3. Experiment 3: variable platform location water maze

the effects of prior amphetamine treatment on the acquisition Individual repeated measure ANOVAs were conducted on the
phase of the water maze task. A significant main effect was latency scores from each day of testing. Fig. 4 depicts perfor-
found for group [F(1, 18) = 10.17, p < 0.05], and session [F(5, mance on the variable platform position water maze task. Each
90) = 22.87, p < 0.05], while the group by session interaction ANOVA used group as a between subjects factor and trial as a
term was not significant. Planned comparison analyses were repeated within subjects variable. Significant main effects were
conducted which looked at group differences in latency scores found for trial on each of the 8 days of training [smallest F(7,
during each daily session. Amphetamine-sensitized animals per- 126) = 2.84, p < 0.05]. Additionally, a marginal group effect was
formed significantly poorer than saline treated animals on days seen on day 1 [F(1, 18) = 4.69, p < 0.05], with saline treated ani-
one [F(1, 18) = 6.36, p < 0.05], four [F(1, 18) = 6.98, p < 0.05] mals showing higher latencies, and a significant group by trial
and six [F(1, 18) = 4.65, p < 0.05]. At the end of acquisition train- interaction was observed on day 3 [F(7, 126) = 2.58, p < 0.05].
ing on the water maze, animals were given a series of probe tests Planned comparison analyses suggested that this latter effect
(immediate and 24 h). Two separate repeated measure ANOVAs was due to saline treated animals taking a significantly longer
were used to analyze the number of platform location cross- time than amphetamine-sensitized animals in finding the plat-
ings from these probe tests, with group as a between subjects form during the first trial [F(1, 18) = 4.55, p < 0.05]. Later trials
factor and platform location (trained platform versus opposite showed no differences between the two groups.
quadrant location) as a within subjects variable. Fig. 3 depicts After acquisition, animals were given additional sessions
performance during these probe trials. A main effect was found which assessed retention of a novel platform position across
during the probe tests for platform location [immediate: F(1, three delay periods (16 s, 1 h and 24 h). Fig. 5 depicts these data.
18) = 106.1, p < 0.05 and 24 h: F(1, 18) = 7.86, p < 0.05], with Repeated measure ANOVAs were conducted separately for each

Fig. 4. Graph depicts performance on the variable platform position water maze task in saline treated (white) and amphetamine-sensitized (black) animals. Saline
treated animals took a greater amount of time to learn the platform location on day 1 and during the first trial on day 3. No other differences were significant. Error
bars depict S.E.M.
R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179 175

Fig. 5. Graph depicts latency to locate a submerged platform during an initial


trial, when the platform location was novel (trial 1), and during a second trial (trial
2) with the same platform position, in saline treated (saline) and amphetamine-
sensitized (AMPH) animals. The two trials were separated by a delay of 16 s, 1 h
or 24 h. While the two groups showed significantly shorter latencies during the
second trial at both the 16 s and 1 h delays (but not after a 24 h delay), no signifi-
cant differences were observed between the two groups. Error bars depict S.E.M.

delay period, with group serving as the between subject factor


and time (trial 1 versus trial 2) as the repeated within subject
factor. No significant differences were found during any of the
three delay periods for group or for the group by time interac-
tion. The main effect of time was significant during the 16 s [F(1,
18) = 8.95, p < 0.05] and 1 h [F(1, 18) = 23.33, p < 0.05] delay
periods, showing that animals in both groups were able to retain
information about the platform position across the delay period.

3.4. Experiment 4: strategy-shifting task

Fig. 6 depicts the data from the strategy-shift task. Two ani-
mals (one saline and one amphetamine) were removed from the
study due to an inability to learn the initial phase of the task. Data
were analyzed with a repeated measure ANOVA, using group
(amphetamine-sensitized versus saline) and shift type (response
to place versus place to response) as between subject variables
and phase (acquisition versus shift) as a repeated within subject
variable. No significant main effects were found for group, shift
type or phase. Likewise, neither the group by phase nor the three-
way interaction was significant. A significant effect was found
for the interaction between shift type and phase [F(1, 14) = 24.8, Fig. 6. Graph depicts performance on the strategy-shifting task in saline treated
(saline) and amphetamine-sensitized (AMPH) animals. Rats were required to
p < 0.05], likely due to the fact that, overall, animals learned the use either a place or response strategy to locate food on the maze. Once animals
place discrimination more easily than the response discrimina- reached a criterion of 10 consecutive correct trials, they were required to switch
tion. Nonetheless, amphetamine-sensitized animals performed strategies (i.e. from place to response or response to place). No differences were
as well as saline treated animals on both the place to response observed between the two groups in the number of trials needed for animals
and the response to place versions of the task. to reach criteria either during acquisition or reversal. The top graph depicts
performance collapsed across both shift types (place to response and response to
place). The middle graph depicts performance on the place and response versions
3.5. Experiment 5: attentional set-shifting task of the task during acquisition while the bottom graph depicts performance on
the place and response versions of the task after being required to shift strategy.
Data from the attentional set-shifting task were analyzed
using a repeated measure ANOVA, with group as a between 17) = 23.9, p < 0.05], for stage [F(6, 102) = 126.5, p < 0.05] and
subject variable and stage (simple discrimination, complex the group × time interaction term [F(6, 102) = 2.94, p < 0.05].
discrimination, etc.) as the repeated within subject vari- Two planned comparison analyses were conducted to assess
able. A significant main effect was found for group [F(1, group differences during each stage of the task. Significant dif-
176 R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179

assessed. Fig. 8 depicts locomotor data from each of the four


groups of animals. Subjects were given an initial 30 min habitua-
tion session, followed by an injection of 0.5 mg/kg amphetamine
and an additional 60 min locomotor test. For each experiment,
two t-test were conducted; one that compared group differences
in locomotion following saline injection and the other that com-
pared group differences following amphetamine treatment. In
all four experiments, amphetamine-sensitized animals had sig-
nificantly higher rates of locomotion than saline treated animals
during the 60 min post injection period [experiment 1, t22 = 2.59;
experiment 2, t14 = 4.58; experiment 3, t14 = 2.29; experiment 4
and 5, t17 = 3.62, all p < 0.05], but not during the 30-min habit-
uation session.

4. Discussion

Fig. 7. Graph depicts performance on an attentional set-shifting task. Compared Prior exposure to a sensitizing regimen of amphetamine had
to saline treated animals (saline) amphetamine-sensitized (AMPH) animals little impact on the performance of a delayed non-match to posi-
required a significantly greater (p < 0.05) number of trials to learn the extra-
tion task, suggesting that working memory was not altered in
dimensional shift component of the task. A similar difference was observed for
the third reversal (Rev 3). No differences were observed during any of the other sensitized animals. Operant based working memory tasks such
stages of the task. Error bars depict S.E.M. Asterisks denote significant group as the DNMTP have been criticized as being open to the influ-
differences. ence of mediating behaviours that can occur during delay periods
and which aid in the retention of positional information [6,22].
ferences were observed between the amphetamine-sensitized While we attempted to minimize such behaviours by installing
and saline treated groups only during the extra-dimensional shift barriers between the lever and the magazine, and by requiring
discrimination [F(1, 17) = 19.3, p < 0.05] and one of the reversals rats to perform a nose poke task during the delay period, it is
(reversal 3) [F(1, 17) = 7.1, p < 0.05]. Fig. 7 depicts performance difficult to rule out the possibility that such behaviours may
on the set-shifting task. have affected task performance. The fact that both groups of
animals showed increasing rates of error as the delay interval
3.6. Locomotor activity test increased suggests that these animals were relying to a large
extent on memory processes to perform the task. In additional
Five weeks following completion of each experiment, the
support of this position, sensitized animals did not show any
locomotor response to a challenge dose of amphetamine was
impairment on the variable platform position task in the water
maze when assessed on the short-term retention (16 s) of infor-
mation about the platform position. Taken together, the two sets
of results suggest that working memory ability was unaffected
by the induction of amphetamine sensitization.
Two prior studies have examined the impact of amphetamine
sensitization on working memory in rodents, using delayed alter-
nation paradigms [47,50]. As with the present experiments, both
studies showed a lack of impairment of working memory fol-
lowing chronic amphetamine exposure. In contrast, impairments
in the performance of a delayed response task were observed in
non-human primates following the induction of amphetamine
sensitization [5]. While this discrepancy could be suggested to
reflect differences in amphetamine regimen or differences in the
nature of the tasks, the present study would seem to rule out such
explanations. First, the amphetamine regimen used here was
clearly effective in inducing robust sensitization, as indicated
Fig. 8. Following behavioural training, animals were assessed for locomo-
tor changes in response to the administration of an amphetamine challenge by increased locomotor activity to an amphetamine challenge.
(0.05 mg/kg). The graph depicts the mean number of beam breaks in saline Secondly, both the rodent and primate studies have used delayed
treated (saline) and amphetamine-sensitized (AMPH) animals for each of response tasks that are quite similar to one another. It is possible,
the four experiments (exp 1 = experiment 1, exp 2 = experiment 2, etc.). given that many of the sensitized animals in the primate study
Amphetamine-sensitized animals had a significantly greater (p < 0.05) number of
showed difficulties at the 0 s delay when trained following the
beam breaks than did saline treated animals during the post-injection period for
all four experiments. For simplicity, performance prior to amphetamine treat- establishment of sensitization [5], that the deficits observed in
ment (habituation) is not depicted. Error bars depict S.E.M. Asterisks denote these animals could have been partly due to the disruption of
significant group differences. some non-mnemonic process, such as attention.
R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179 177

Working memory performance has been linked to altered tion over a short delay from the first to second trial (working
dopaminergic neurotransmission in the prefrontal cortex. Prior memory) [15]. Thus, it was expected that the variable platform
exposure to a sensitizing regimen of amphetamine or cocaine task would provide a more demanding test of memory than that
disrupts multiple aspects of prefrontal cortex function, includ- provided by the fixed platform position task. The fact that per-
ing blunting dopaminergic function [5,25,48,54]. In primates, formance was not disrupted on the variable platform task further
reduced prefrontal dopaminergic function has been shown to be strengthens the conclusion that long-term memory is not altered
associated with poor working memory [45,46,55]. In rats, how- by the induction of amphetamine sensitization.
ever, the picture is less clear-cut. For example, direct infusion The lack of effect of amphetamine sensitization on water
of the dopamine D1 receptor antagonist, SCH23390 into the maze performance reported in the present experiment is consis-
medial prefrontal cortex in rats produced performance impair- tent with the results of a previous study that showed no effect of
ments rather than memory impairments in a DNMTP task similar amphetamine sensitization on either the acquisition or retention
to the one used here [3]. SCH23390 also failed to affect working of a fixed platform position water maze task [41]. Sensitized
memory in a delayed alteration task in rats [56]. Interestingly, animals did show a decrease in latency to find a novel platform
that study showed that a high dose of a D1 receptor agonist position following training (interpreted as reversal learning), as
impaired working memory, but that this disruption could be well as an increase in number of crossings of the former platform
blocked by administration of a D1 antagonist. [56]. These data position during probe trials relative to saline treated animals.
suggest that in the rat it is increased, rather than reduced, PFC DA While this latter effect may suggest improved spatial learning
function that impairs working memory. Given that amphetamine in amphetamine treated animals, the effect was not large and
sensitization appears to decrease prefrontal dopamine activity was not always observed. Thus, while deficits in long-term,
[5,25,48,54] it is not surprising that working memory remains hippocampal dependent memory have been widely reported
intact in sensitized rodents. Alternatively, tasks typically used to in schizophrenia [7,23,24], such deficits are not reproduced in
assess working memory in rodents often heavily depend upon rodents following the induction of amphetamine sensitization.
the use of hippocampal memory processes [4], a factor which It is likely that amphetamine sensitization is not a useful model
may make the performance of such tasks less dependent upon of this aspect of schizophrenia.
prefrontal involvement and, thus, less susceptible to disruption The final two experiments assessed behavioural flexibil-
by the amphetamine-induced sensitized state. ity in amphetamine-sensitized rats using both a radial maze
Amphetamine-sensitized animals also failed to show an based strategy selection task and an attention set-shifting task.
impairment in long-term, hippocampal dependent memory, as Amphetamine-sensitized animals did not show an impairment
assessed using either a fixed platform or variable platform posi- on the radial maze based strategy-shifting task. In contrast,
tion water maze task. Sensitized animals showed slightly higher amphetamine-sensitized animals were impaired in performance
escape latencies on the fixed platform position water maze task of the extra-dimensional shift component of the set-shifting task,
on days 1, 4 and 6 of training. That this impairment was due despite showing good performance during the earlier stages of
to a disruption in spatial learning and memory is unlikely given the task, as has been previously reported [16]. Superficially, the
that no differences were seen between the two groups during the two tasks would appear to be measuring attentional set-shifting
post acquisition probe tests carried out immediately and 24 h and both have been shown to be impaired by disruptions of pre-
following the last trial of training. Such probe tests represent a frontal cortex function [1,19,28,37–39]. Likewise, deficits have
purer measure of learning, since animals are started from a novel been reported on a radial maze based strategy selection task fol-
position on the maze and, as a result, are unable to rely upon non- lowing the establishment of cocaine sensitization [21], although
spatial navigation strategies, such as route learning or response this was limited to an increase in perseverative responding in
learning, which may be used when started from a previously used sensitized animals with no change in number of trials needed to
start position. Animals use both spatial and non-spatial infor- reach criterion. Further, interfering with prefrontal cortex activ-
mation simultaneously on navigation tasks [9,10,29,33] and it ity by direct infusion of the dopamine antagonist SCH23390, a
is possible that the deficit observed during acquisition reflected manipulation which may mirror the changes in dopamine func-
a change in ability in non-spatial memory. No differences were tion induced following amphetamine sensitization, produces a
observed during retesting on the water maze 10 days follow- deficit in switching memory strategy [36]. Taken together, these
ing the end of acquisition training, demonstrating that the two results suggest that the two tasks are both mediated by the
groups were able to retain information about the platform posi- prefrontal cortex. As such, it is surprising that amphetamine
tion equally well over the retention interval. Sensitized animals sensitization did not induce similar impairments in performance
also showed normal performance of the variable platform posi- of the two tasks.
tion task. In this task performance depends upon the use of A number of explanations could be put forth to explain the dif-
a number of different cognitive processes and, hence, may be ferential effects of amphetamine sensitization on the set-shifting
dependent upon a wide network of neural structures. For exam- and strategy-shifting tasks. First, major procedural differences
ple, in addition to remembering a particular platform position between the two tasks exist that could place different demands
(spatial memory), animals need to develop strategies to effi- on cognitive processing. For example, on the set-shifting task,
ciently search the water maze (strategy learning), an ability that animals learn a series of discriminations in which a particular
appears to be dependent, in part, upon the prefrontal cortex [26], stimulus dimension is always used to signal reinforcement. The
and animals are required to remember the novel platform loca- relevant stimulus dimension is repeatedly reinforced across dif-
178 R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179

ferent stimulus pairings/combinations, most likely resulting in schizophrenia, although the lack of effect on the similar strategy-
the development of a strong attentional set. In contrast, animals shifting task suggests that the cognitive processes affected by
on the strategy-shifting task only experience one particular dis- sensitization may be specific to those required to complete an
crimination, wherein a single stimulus or collection of stimuli extradimensional shift. As such, amphetamine sensitization can-
signals reinforcement over a number of trials. As such, it is pos- not be regarded as a complete model of schizophrenia, as it
sible that animals develop a much stronger attentional set on the does not capture the entirety of cognitive deficit observed in
set-shifting task than on the strategy-shifting task and, as a result, this disease. The reliability with which extradimensional shift
find the shift required by the former task more difficult. It should performance is disrupted provides further confirmation that
be noted that, while robust, the deficit observed on the attentional amphetamine sensitization is a good model of the attentional
set-shifting task following sensitization is relatively mild in disruptions associated with schizophrenia.
comparison to that observed following lesions of the prefrontal
cortex (as reported by [1]). It is possible that the strategy-shifting Acknowledgements
task is not sensitive enough to detect changes in the relatively
mildly impaired sensitized animal, despite being sufficient to This work was supported by a special initiative grant from
detect changes in the more impaired prefrontal lesioned animal. the Ontario Mental Health Foundation. REF was supported by a
The attentional set-shifting task differs from the other tasks Research Fellowship from the Ontario Mental Health Founda-
used in the present study in that it uses odor cues and, thus, tion and SK was supported by a Canadian Research Chair.
places a demand on olfactory processing. Some evidence sug-
gests that acute treatment with psychostimulants may impair References
olfactory-based learning. For example, one study showed that
exposure to cocaine could disrupt performance of an odor-based [1] Birrell JM, Brown VJ. Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional
working memory task in animals chronically exposed to cocaine set shifting in the rat. J Neurosci 2000;20:4320–4.
through self-administration training, despite the fact that such [2] Bisagno V, Ferguson D, Luine VN. Chronic D-amphetamine induces sex-
training failed to alter the ability of cocaine to disrupt perfor- ually dimorphic effects on locomotion, recognition memory, and brain
monoamines. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003;74:859–67.
mance on a similar visual based memory task [27]. Likewise, [3] Broersen LM, Heinsbroek RP, de Bruin JP, Uylings HB, Olivier B. The role
animals exposed to amphetamine showed reduced detection of the medial prefrontal cortex of rats in short-term memory functioning:
rates for an odor cue on a signal detection task [11]. It could further support for involvement of cholinergic, rather than dopaminergic
thus be argued that amphetamine-sensitized rats were deficient mechanisms. Brain Res 1995;674:221–9.
on the attentional set-shifting task because of altered olfactory [4] Castner SA, Goldman-Rakic PS, Williams GV. Animal models of work-
ing memory: insights for targeting cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia.
processing induced by amphetamine treatment. However, this Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;174:111–25.
is unlikely for a number of reasons. First, the present animals [5] Castner SA, Vosler PS, Goldman-Rakic PS. Amphetamine sensitization
were tested in a drug-free state, and neither of these previous impairs cognition and reduces dopamine turnover in primate prefrontal
studies allows us to determine whether a sensitizing regimen of cortex. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:743–51.
amphetamine is sufficient to disrupt olfactory processing in the [6] Chudasama Y, Muir JL. A behavioural analysis of the delayed non-
matching to position task: the effects of scopolamine, lesions of the fornix
absence of the acute effects of the drug. Second, amphetamine- and of the prelimbic region on mediating behaviours by rats. Psychophar-
sensitized animals were equally impaired when required to make macology (Berl) 1997;134:73–82.
an extradimensional shift from odor to texture or from texture to [7] Clare L, McKenna PJ, Mortimer AM, Baddeley AD. Memory in
odor, suggesting that the deficits observed in sensitized animals schizophrenia: what is impaired and what is preserved? Neuropsychologia
on the attentional set-shifting task also occur in the somatosen- 1993;31:1225–41.
[8] Deller T, Sarter M. Effects of repeated administration of amphetamine on
sory modality. Finally, if the amphetamine-sensitized state alters behavioral vigilance: evidence for “sensitized” attentional impairments.
olfactory based learning then it might be predicted that sensi- Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1998;137:410–4.
tized rats would be impaired on other types of discriminations [9] Devan BD, McDonald RJ, White NM. Effects of medial and lateral caudate-
involving odor, most notably the compound discrimination when putamen lesions on place- and cue-guided behaviors in the water maze:
a second irrelevant stimulus dimension was introduced. This did relation to thigmotaxis. Behav Brain Res 1999;100:5–14.
[10] Devan BD, White NM. Parallel information processing in the dorsal stria-
not occur; amphetamine-sensitized rats were impaired only on tum: relation to hippocampal function. J Neurosci 1999;19:2789–98.
the EDS. [11] Doty RL, Ferguson-Segall M. Odor detection performance of rats following
The current study assessed working and long-term memory in d-amphetamine treatment: a signal detection analysis. Psychopharmacol-
rats that had undergone amphetamine sensitization. While prior ogy (Berl) 1987;93:87–93.
work has demonstrated that this model replicates some of the [12] Dunnett SB, Evenden JL, Iversen SD. Delay-dependent short-term memory
deficits in aged rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988;96:174–80.
cognitive deficits associated with schizophrenia, amphetamine- [13] Featherstone RE, Kapur S, Fletcher PJ. The amphetamine-induced sensi-
sensitized animals did not show evidence of memory deficits in tized state as a model of schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol
any of the tasks used in the current experiments. These results Psychiatry 2007;31:1556–71.
suggest that amphetamine sensitization does not produce simi- [14] Featherstone RE, Rizos Z, Nobrega JN, Kapur S, Fletcher PJ. Gestational
lar memory impairments as are seen in schizophrenia, and, thus, methylazoxymethanol acetate treatment impairs select cognitive functions:
parallels to schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007;32:483–92.
might not be an adequate model of this aspect of schizophrenic [15] Ferbinteanu J, Ray C, McDonald RJ. Both dorsal and ventral hippocam-
cognition. The deficit in attentional set-shifting is consistent pus contribute to spatial learning in Long–Evans rats. Neurosci Lett
with a deficit in cognitive flexibility, a common feature of 2003;345:131–5.
R.E. Featherstone et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 189 (2008) 170–179 179

[16] Fletcher PJ, Tenn CC, Rizos Z, Lovic V, Kapur S. Sensitization to [36] Ragozzino ME. The effects of dopamine D(1) receptor blockade in
amphetamine, but not PCP, impairs attentional set shifting: reversal by the prelimbic-infralimbic areas on behavioral flexibility. Learn Mem
a D(1) receptor agonist injected into the medial prefrontal cortex. Psy- 2002;9:18–28.
chopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:190–200. [37] Ragozzino ME, Detrick S, Kesner RP. Involvement of the prelimbic-
[18] Fletcher PJ, Tenn CC, Sinyard J, Rizos Z, Kapur S. A sensitizing reg- infralimbic areas of the rodent prefrontal cortex in behavioral flexibility
imen of amphetamine impairs visual attention in the 5-choice serial for place and response learning. J Neurosci 1999;19:4585–94.
reaction time test: reversal by a D1 receptor agonist injected into [38] Ragozzino ME, Kim J, Hassert D, Minniti N, Kiang C. The contribution
the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007;32:1122– of the rat prelimbic-infralimbic areas to different forms of task switching.
32. Behav Neurosci 2003;117:1054–65.
[19] Floresco SB, Magyar O, Ghods-Sharifi S, Vexelman C, Tse MT. Multi- [39] Ragozzino ME, Wilcox C, Raso M, Kesner RP. Involvement of rodent
ple dopamine receptor subtypes in the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat prefrontal cortex subregions in strategy switching. Behav Neurosci
regulate set-shifting. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31:297–309. 1999;113:32–41.
[20] Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neu- [40] Robinson TE, Becker JB. Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced
ropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994;6:348–57. by chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal
[21] Goto Y, Grace AA. Dopaminergic modulation of limbic and cortical models of amphetamine psychosis. Brain Res 1986;396:157–98.
drive of nucleus accumbens in goal-directed behavior. Nat Neurosci [41] Russig H, Durrer A, Yee BK, Murphy CA, Feldon J. The acquisition, reten-
2005;8:805–12. tion and reversal of spatial learning in the morris water maze task following
[22] Gutnikov SA, Barnes JC, Rawlins JN. Working memory tasks in five- withdrawal from an escalating dosage schedule of amphetamine in Wistar
choice operant chambers: use of relative and absolute spatial memories. rats. Neuroscience 2003;119:167–79.
Behav Neurosci 1994;108:899–910. [42] Russig H, Murphy CA, Feldon J. Clozapine and haloperidol reinstate latent
[23] Hanlon FM, Weisend MP, Hamilton DA, Jones AP, Thoma RJ, Huang M, inhibition following its disruption during amphetamine withdrawal. Neu-
et al. Impairment on the hippocampal-dependent virtual Morris water task ropsychopharmacology 2002;26:765–77.
in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2006;87:67–80. [43] Russig H, Murphy CA, Feldon J. Prepulse inhibition during withdrawal
[24] Heckers S, Rauch SL, Goff D, Savage CR, Schacter DL, Fischman AJ, et from an escalating dosage schedule of amphetamine. Psychopharmacology
al. Impaired recruitment of the hippocampus during conscious recollection (Berl) 2003;169:340–53.
in schizophrenia. Nat Neurosci 1998;1:318–23. [44] Russig H, Murphy CA, Feldon J. Behavioural consequences of withdrawal
[25] Hedou G, Homberg J, Feldon J, Heidbreder CA. Expression of sensiti- from three different administration schedules of amphetamine. Behav Brain
zation to amphetamine and dynamics of dopamine neurotransmission in Res 2005;165:26–35.
different laminae of the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Neuropharmacology [45] Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS. D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal
2001;40:366–82. cortex: involvement in working memory. Science 1991;251:947–50.
[26] Hoh TE, Kolb B, Eppel A, Vanderwolf CH, Cain DP. Role of the neocortex [46] Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS. The role of D1-dopamine receptor
in the water maze task in the rat: a detailed behavioral and Golgi-Cox in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the
analysis. Behav Brain Res 2003;138:81–94. prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-
[27] Kantak KM, Udo T, Ugalde F, Luzzo C, Di Pietro N, Eichenbaum HB. response task. J Neurophysiol 1994;71:515–28.
Influence of cocaine self-administration on learning related to prefrontal [47] Shoblock JR, Maisonneuve IM, Glick SD. Differences between d-
cortex or hippocampus functioning in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) methamphetamine and d-amphetamine in rats: working memory, tolerance,
2005;181:227–36. and extinction. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;170:150–6.
[28] McAlonan K, Brown VJ. Orbital prefrontal cortex mediates reversal [48] Sorg BA, Davidson DL, Kalivas PW, Prasad BM. Repeated daily cocaine
learning and not attentional set shifting in the rat. Behav Brain Res alters subsequent cocaine-induced increase of extracellular dopamine in
2003;146:97–103. the medial prefrontal cortex. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1997;281:54–61.
[29] McDonald RJ, White NM. Parallel information processing in the water [49] Steele RJ, Morris RG. Delay-dependent impairment of a matching-to-place
maze: evidence for independent memory systems involving dorsal striatum task with chronic and intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA-antagonist
and hippocampus. Behav Neural Biol 1994;61:260–70. D-AP5. Hippocampus 1999;9:118–36.
[30] Murphy CA, Di Iorio L, Feldon J. Effects of psychostimulant with- [50] Stefani MR, Moghaddam B. Effects of repeated treatment with
drawal on latent inhibition of conditioned active avoidance and prepulse amphetamine or phencyclidine on working memory in the rat. Behav Brain
inhibition of the acoustic startle response. Psychopharmacology (Berl) Res 2002;134:267–74.
2001;156:155–64. [51] Tenn CC, Fletcher PJ, Kapur S. Amphetamine-sensitized animals show
[31] Murphy CA, Fend M, Russig H, Feldon J. Latent inhibition, but not pre- a sensorimotor gating and neurochemical abnormality similar to that of
pulse inhibition, is reduced during withdrawal from an escalating dosage schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2003;64:103–14.
schedule of amphetamine. Behav Neurosci 2001;115:1247–56. [52] Tenn CC, Fletcher PJ, Kapur S. A putative animal model of the “prodromal”
[32] Ornstein TJ, Iddon JL, Baldacchino AM, Sahakian BJ, London M, Everitt state of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:586–93.
BJ, et al. Profiles of cognitive dysfunction in chronic amphetamine and [53] Tenn CC, Kapur S, Fletcher PJ. Sensitization to amphetamine, but not
heroin abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000;23:113–26. phencyclidine, disrupts prepulse inhibition and latent inhibition. Psy-
[33] Packard MG, McGaugh JL. Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus chopharmacology (Berl) 2005;180:366–76.
with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learn- [54] Vanderschuren LJ, Schmidt ED, De Vries TJ, Van Moorsel CA, Tilders FJ,
ing. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1996;65:65–72. Schoffelmeer AN. A single exposure to amphetamine is sufficient to induce
[34] Pantelis C, Barber FZ, Barnes TR, Nelson HE, Owen AM, Robbins TW. long-term behavioral, neuroendocrine, and neurochemical sensitization in
Comparison of set-shifting ability in patients with chronic schizophrenia rats. J Neurosci 1999;19:9579–86.
and frontal lobe damage. Schizophr Res 1999;37:251–70. [55] Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Modulation of memory fields by
[35] Paulson PE, Camp DM, Robinson TE. Time course of transient behavioral dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Nature 1995;376:572–5.
depression and persistent behavioral sensitization in relation to regional [56] Zahrt J, Taylor JR, Mathew RG, Arnsten AF. Supranormal stimulation
brain monoamine concentrations during amphetamine withdrawal in rats. of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1991;103:480–92. working memory performance. J Neurosci 1997;17:8528–35.

You might also like