You are on page 1of 14

International Journal of Mental Health Nursing (2004) 13, 39–52

FEATURE ARTICLE
New evidence or changing population? Reviewing
the evidence of a link between mental illness and
violence
Brodie Paterson, Peter Claughan and Sandy McComish
Department of Nursing and Midwifery, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland

ABSTRACT: There has been an apparent shift in majority opinion within psychiatry over the last
20 years on the nature of the relationship between mental illness and violence. Where once there was
perhaps widespread scepticism, research, while sometimes producing conflicting results, appears
ultimately to have led to the emergence of an almost universal consensus that there is a link. This
paper will review the nature of the evidence for such a link between mental illness and violence and
explore some of the newer suggestions about why mental illness may sometimes be related to violence
KEY WORDS: mental illness, research review, violence.

INTRODUCTION probability of their committing a violent crime then our


findings show that this does not exceed the dangerous-
Lionel Penrose in 1939 famously asserted that there was
ness of the legally responsible adult population as a
unequivocally a relationship between mental illness and
whole’. There was therefore widespread rejection of any
violence claiming that there was inverse relationship
link between mental illness and violence on the basis of
between the amount of violent crime in a number of
the available evidence and indeed of the relationship
European countries and the number of hospital beds that
between crime in general and mental disorder (Gunn
those countries devoted to the care of the mentally ill
1977).
(Conacher 1996). Such unsubstantiated claims notwith-
standing, expert opinion within psychiatry on the nature
of the relationship between mental illness and violence AIMS
would seem to have been largely to the contrary until
The relationship between mental illness and violence has
comparatively recently.
social and political dimensions as well as clinical implica-
Monahan and Steadman (1983; p. 211) concluding a
tions. There have, in particular, within the UK been
review of studies of patients discharged from psychiatric
concerns in respect of the influence the issue of violence
hospitals suggested that they showed no support for the
perpetrated by people experiencing mental illness and
premise that people with mental illness were more dan-
the role of media representations of the nature, extent
gerous than the general population when ‘matched for
and significance of such a relationship have had on social
demographic factors and prior criminal history’. Hafner
policy (Paterson & Stark 2001). There have also been
and Boker (1992; p. 89) reporting the results of a review
more recent concerns that proposals in England in rela-
of violent crime in Germany suggested that ‘if we define
tion to the management of Anti Social Personality Disor-
dangerousness of the mentally abnormal as the relative
der represent an unwelcome extension of the role of
psychiatry in respect of public safety (Pilgrim & Rogers
2003). This paper will however, restrict its focus to
Correspondence: Brodie Paterson, Department of Nursing and considering the nature and extent of the evidence which
Midwifery, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland FK9 4LA, United
Kingdom. Email: b.a.paterson@stir.ac.uk appears to have informed a new and different consensus
Brodie Paterson, MEd, BA(Hons), RMN, RNLD on the nature and degree of the relationship between
Peter Claughan, BN, RMN
Sandy McComish, MA, BSc, RMN, RNT mental illness and violence rather than the implications
Accepted November 2003. of such evidence for social policy.
40 B. PATERSON ET AL.

BEFORE TARASOFF Campus police were then contacted and asked to contact
Poddar with a view to removing him to hospital. The
Shah (1975) argued that research on the accuracy of
police visited his apartment on campus but found Podarr
clinical predictions of dangerousness and thus an individ-
apparently rational, he denied any threat to Tarasoff and
ual likelihood of committing a violent act cast ‘serious
after warning him to stay away from her, the police
doubt on the ability of clinicians to predict dangerous
departed without taking him into care. The responsible
behaviour at all’ (Lidz & Mulvey 1995; p. 43). This
psychiatrist and psychologist then did nothing further to
opinion was endorsed by the American Psychiatric Asso-
pursue commitment and Poddar discontinued therapy.
ciation, which in 1973 took the seemingly extraordinary
Two months later Poddar went to Tarasoff’s home and
step for a professional body of explicitly stating that their
stabbed her to death. Tarasoff’s parents subsequently
members were not competent to make valid or useful
filed a negligence suit against the University alleging that
predictions of an individuals probability of engaging in
its clinicians had been negligent in failing to detain
dangerous behaviour (Monahan & Walker 1994) Even
Poddar and in failing to warn Tarasoff of the threats
under best conditions ‘neither psychiatrists or behav-
made against her. In an initial ruling (the case for
ioural scientists can select persons who will become
negligence was settled out of court), the California
dangerous’ (Steadman & Cocozza 1974; p. 188). It seems
Supreme Court found that:
ironic however, that at the same time psychiatry and
psychology were in broad agreement on the view that once a therapist determines, or under applicable proce-
there was not a relationship between mental illness and dures should have determined, that a patient poses a
violence and that by implication therefore, they could serious danger of violence to others, he bears a duty to
have no particular expertise in predicting violent behav- exercise reasonable care to warn the foreseeable victim
iour, that what should emerge but, of that danger (Tarasoff v Regents of the University of
California 551 P2d 555 (Cal 1974).
an increasing demand, and expectation that mental
health professionals should engage in activities predi- The establishment of what has become known as a
cated both on the mentally disordered presenting a ‘duty to warn’ caused considerable concern among
threat to their fellow citizens and on a capacity to predict mental health professionals (Mason 1998). The Court
that dangerousness (Mullen 1997; p. 4). ruling was thus promptly challenged by the APA (1974)
Almost contemporaneous with the American Psychi- who asserted that as psychiatrists and other mental health
atric Associations disavowal of its members’ abilities to professionals had no ability to predict violence, the
make accurate judgements of an individual’s potential requirement to warn would result in many false positive
dangerousness, a case was being heard in the Supreme reports creating fear and alarm in potential victims when
Court of California, Tarasoff v Regents 1976, which there was no real danger and that the requirement to
continues to have a significant influence in this area. warn would undermine the confidentiality of therapist
Some brief background details of the case will be given to and patient unnecessarily (Anfang & Applebaum 1996).
place the judgement reached in context. The Supreme Court agreed to hear the case again and in
Tatania Tarasoff was a female graduate student at the 1976 (Tarasoff v Regents of California 551 P2d 333 Cal
University of California in the Autumn of 1968 when she 1976) they issued a second opinion. In this instance the
met a fellow student Prosenjitt Poddar. They dated over Court again found that therapists have a duty to potential
several weeks with Poddar apparently believing that the victims but defined this as a ‘duty to protect’ rather than
relationship was serious. Tarasoff however, broke off the a ‘duty to warn’. In their judgement if a therapist appre-
relationship and by 1969 Poddar, tearful and depressed, hends or should have apprehended, ‘that his patient
discontinued his studies and contacted the University represents a serious danger of violence to another, he
Health Service. He was initially evaluated by a psychi- incurs an obligation to use reasonable care to protect the
atrist who did not believe he required compulsory admis- intended victim against such danger’ (Tarasoff v Regents
sion although he was prescribed a low dose antipsychotic of California 551 P2d 333 Cal 1976). Actions necessary
and referred to a psychologist for out-patient therapy. might include warning the anticipated victim(s), alerting
During therapy Poddar reported fantasies of harming others including the police and taking whatever reason-
and even killing a young girl, readily identifiable as able steps might be necessary to minimize the risk of
Tarasoff. The psychologist, aware in addition from a third violence (Beck 1985).
party, that Poddar had been considering purchasing a The Court did not impose a duty for the therapist to
firearm became concerned and contacted and discussed be either accurate or perfect in their prediction; rather
his anxieties with the psychiatrist. They concluded that they argued that the practitioner should not be ‘negli-
commitment was necessary and the psychologist and the gent’. Once a prediction was made, as in the case of
MENTAL ILLNESS AND VIOLENCE 41

Poddar, then ‘reasonable’ steps should be taken to warn the month prior to a first admission (for schizophrenia)
the intended victim or otherwise protect them, for (Johnston et al. 1986) and 26% in the 6 months preceding
example warning the police or taking steps to have the admission (Binder & McNiel 1988). The studies how-
patient committed (Mason 1998). In the USA however, ever, have used different means of gathering data includ-
Tarasoff and related rulings effectively served to create ing case note analysis, arrest records, and patient and
the expectation, subsequently codified by statute in a carer interviews, and therefore are not directly compar-
number of US States, that predictions of future danger- able. There remains the possibility that an unknown
ousness were integral to working in a professional capac- proportion of violence may go undetected, particularly in
ity with people experiencing mental illness (Carstenson community samples (Lagos et al. 1977). If, for example,
1994). arrest data are used, there may be incidents that are not
reported or that result in the perpetrator’s hospitalization
rather than arrest (Klassen & O’Connor 1988).
EVIDENCE FOR A NEW CONSENSUS? These results must of course be interpreted in the
One indirect result of the imposition of this responsibility context of mental health practice. Given the almost
was to give impetus to research into the possible links universal adoption, of ‘dangerousness’ as perhaps the key
between mental illness and violence. The nature of the potential mental health admission criteria, people with a
research, which emerged over the last two decades, will perceived mental illness who have acted violently, or are
now be reviewed. Four broad categories of studies assessed by professionals as posing a high risk of acting
appear to have dominated research into the general violently, are more likely to be seen as a priority for
relationship between mental illness and violence in the admission. This has been described as ‘ascertainment
period in question. Each of these will be examined and bias’ (Wessley & Taylor 1991). Retrospective samples
explored in turn. based on admission populations are thus irretrievably
1. Studies that have examined the level of violent behav- flawed in terms of their lack of representativeness of the
iour among those with mental illness. These include incidence of violence across the whole population of
studies of violence prior to admission to psychiatric people with a mental illness (Monahan & Steadman
hospital, violence during hospitalization and violence 1994).
post discharge from hospital.
Violence by inpatients
2. Studies which examine the frequency of mental illness Similar problems occur when violence by psychiatric
among those who have exhibited violent behaviour. inpatients is considered. Only a small percentage of
These include studies of prisoners on remand and people with severe mental illness are in hospital at any
prisoners who have been convicted, both for general given time and they tend to be those experiencing acute
criminal behaviour and for crimes of violence, includ- episodes, those where drug compliance is poor or whose
ing homicide. symptoms are not well controlled. There have, however,
3. Community studies where the presence of mental been a large number of studies on inpatient populations.
illness and the level of engagement in violent behav- The immediate availability of a convenient sample
iour are established separately and the relationship coupled with the fact that many hospitals have pre-
between the two then analysed. existing recording and reporting systems for violent
4. Birth cohort studies where very large random popula- behaviours appears to have prompted much research.
tion samples, such as people born on the same day, Haller and Deluty (1988; p. 174) caution however, that
are studied over extended time periods and the rela- researchers ‘relying on formal incident reports of assaults
tionships between such variables as diagnosis of schizo- on staff very likely underestimate the actual incidence of
phrenia and the likelihood of committing a violent assaults’. Reporting systems may consistently under-
offence are analysed. estimate the real frequency of incidents by as much as
60% (Daffern et al. 2003; Foust & Rhee 1993; Lion et al.
Violence prior to admission 1991; Wenk et al. 1972) although the frequency and
A series of studies have examined violent behaviour reliability of reporting has been shown to increase in line
involving people with a mental illness. These have with the severity of injuries to the parties concerned
focused around hospital and highlight violence prior to (Infantino & Musingo 1985).
admission, violence by inpatients and violence by those It has also been suggested that violence in psychiatric
discharged from hospital. High rates of violent behaviour inpatient settings may not represent a relationship
have been reported prior to admission ranging from 12% between mental illness (or even specific symptoms) and
48 hours prior to admission (Rossi et al. 1986), 19% in violence, as much as patients’ reactions to the experience
42 B. PATERSON ET AL.

of admission, to the ward or service culture (Garnham records over 10 years for patients discharged from four
2001), to deliberate or unwitting provocation by other Swedish psychiatric hospitals in 1986. Thirteen per cent
patients or staff (Powell et al. 1994; Shepherd & Laven- of the study group were convicted of a violent crime
der 1999), to the lack of activities and staff patient during the 10 years of follow up with 1% of the study
interactions (Sclafani 2000) or to the restrictions on group (11 people from 893) sentenced for murder,
liberty which may accompany admission (Sheridan et al. attempted murder or manslaughter. A number of studies
1990). Given differences in the nature of the units have looked at specifically selected high risk groups and
studied which have ranged from maximum security found an even higher incidence of violence. Klassen and
psychiatry (Larkin et al. 1988), through to locked obser- O’Connor (1988) followed up 304 male patients identi-
vation wards (Cooper et al. 1983) to open admission areas fied as at risk for future violence for a year post-discharge
and units for elderly people experiencing dementia and found that 25% acted violently during that period.
(Fottrell 1980) as well as the inconsistencies in opera- Steadman et al. (1994), however, noted that literature
tional measures adopted, it is perhaps unsurprising that on violence by discharged patients must be interpreted
research involving inpatient populations has failed to with considerable caution and make a number of criti-
produce consistent results. cisms of the methodology employed. First, they suggest
Critics of studies of preadmission rates of violence and that to rely solely on official arrest or conviction records,
the frequency of violence in inpatient populations have records of re-hospitalization or self-reports by patients
suggested that both have failed to demonstrate that the without corroboration to determine the frequency and
incidence and frequency of violence established differ severity of violence may result in inaccuracies. This may
significantly from the base rates in the non-disordered lead to an effective inability to compare the violence by
community population once appropriate socio- ‘ex-patients’ or people with mental health problems who
demographic variables are controlled for (Monahan have not been admitted, to rates in the relevant commu-
1992). Comparing arrest rates for violence between nity. Second, they note that few studies have examined
patients admitted to hospital or patients discharged from the timing of violent acts and observe that this is ‘despite
hospital against notional community norms fails to ade- the implications for intervention that timing may have’
quately reflect the preponderance of the poor, male, (Steadman et al. 1998; p. 393). Monahan and Steadman
single, alcoholic and black patients admitted and dis- (1994) note that most samples of discharged patients are
charged by most US public mental hospitals where much unrepresentative of either the more general population of
of the research was conducted (Wessley 1997). people with a mental illness or of hospital populations in
that they exclude two groups of patients. The first consists
Violence by discharged patients of those seen as posing a very high risk of violence who are
Criticisms can also be applied to a number of the studies unlikely to be discharged and the second, comprising
in which individuals hospitalized for treatment for mental those experiencing mental illness viewed as representing a
illness have been followed up on discharge (Asnis et al. minimal risk but who were never admitted in the first
1997). Methodologically, with some minor variations, place. Excluding these two groups results again in the
these studies have compared the arrest rates of dis- problem of ascertainment bias.
charged former patients with that of the general popula- One research study that addressed some of these
tion over periods of time ranging from 12 months to weaknesses is reported by Steadman et al. (1998). They
5 years. A review of the literature by Link et al. (1992) studied 1136 male and female patients across three
concluded that ex-psychiatric patient arrest rates were American sites for a year after discharge from hospital.
300% greater than the average for the general popula- Diagnosis was confirmed by research interview using the
tion. While arrests reflect a range of criminal behaviour DSM III-R checklist (American Psychiatric Association
and not necessarily violence, the difference may be even 1987) and patients were followed up by interviews every
more marked when solely violent crimes were considered 10 weeks post-discharge. Information from a collateral
(Rabkin 1979). respondent, usually a family member, was also obtained
Lindqvist and Allebeck (1990) followed up 644 indi- weekly. Respondents were paid to participate in the
viduals, with a diagnosis of schizophrenia discharged study and violence was established using self report and
from hospital in Stockholm in 1971, over a 15-year the collateral information to establish; if and when the
period. They found that the rate of recorded convictions individual had been violent, where the violence occurred,
for violent offences was four times higher in the group what type of violence had been exhibited, how often had
than the general population average. Of the 6% of the this behaviour been displayed and who were the actual or
sample who had been violent nearly half had an alcohol intended targets of the violence. This information was
misuse problem. Belfrage (1998) studied the conviction then collated together with arrest and re-hospitalization
MENTAL ILLNESS AND VIOLENCE 43

records. There are some obvious weaknesses in this study population statistics. Schizophrenia was not increased in
in that it did not employ a randomized sample using only comparison to the estimated prevalence in the general
patients who volunteered to participate. However, at population but 70% were diagnosed as having a sub-
1 year follow up the patient group identified as having a stance misuse problem, personality disorder and depres-
major mental disorder (schizophrenia, schizophreniform sion were also significantly higher than in the general
illness, schizoaffective disorder, depression, dysthymia, population.
mania, cyclothymia, mania or other psychotic disorder) in
conjunction with a substance misuse disorder were Mental illness and violence in prison
reported to have a violence rate of 31.1%. The rate The prevalence of mental illness in populations of
reported for the group identified as having a major convicted offenders has also been studied. Monahan
mental disorder, but without substance misuse, was (1993) in a Californian study estimated the incidence of
17.9%. schizophrenia at 6%, more than three times the rate
Steadman et al. (1998) compared these rates with a estimated in the general US population (1.7%). Mania
group of 519 community subjects matched carefully for and bipolar illness appear increased with 2.1% reported
socio-demographic variables and noted that in one in offenders in comparison to an estimated general
sample area the rates for violence in the group with population frequency of 0.3%. Taylor and Gunn (1984)
major mental disorder (without comorbid substance in a British study followed up a group of detainees and
misuse) did not exceed the matched community sample. concluded that that 9% of those subsequently convicted
Recent findings from The McArthur Community Risk for non-violent crimes and 11% convicted for violent
Study (Monahan 2002) have also suggested that the crimes had schizophrenia. Only 8% were seen as being
prevalence of violence among people discharged from a symptom-free at the time of the offence (Taylor &
(psychiatric) hospital who do not have symptoms of Gunn 1984).
substance abuse is similar to the prevalence rates for
violence among other people living in the community not Homicide
diagnosed as substance abusers. Homicide by people with mental illness has attracted
attention for study as a separate crime and has been the
Mental illness in remand populations subject of a number of studies in several countries.
In contrast to studies looking at the arrest records of Finland has a very high ‘clear up rate’ for homicide, with
individuals with a diagnosis of mental illness a number of the perpetrator identified, in over 95% of cases (in
studies have tried to ascertain the prevalence rate of comparison to the rate in the USA of approximately
mental illness among populations of individuals detained 66%). All offenders routinely receive a psychiatric evalu-
either on remand or in custody. Such studies are poten- ation followed by an in depth examination by a specialist
tially valuable because criminal behaviour in people with forensic psychiatrist when the initial assessment has indi-
mental illness, even when the illness is severe, may result cated that this may be of benefit (Eronen et al. 1996a). In
in remand or imprisonment rather than hospitalization. a review of the forensic examinations carried out on 693
Of note, however, is that most criminal justice systems offenders, from a total of 994 offenders initially exam-
have procedures for treating those offenders who have a ined, Eronen et al. (1996a) found that 6.4% were diag-
serious mental illness at the time of arrest, or who nosed with schizophrenia or schizophrenic type
develop one during incarceration, which may involve psychoses. This compares with an estimated prevalence
their transfer from prison to hospital. Reviews suggest rate of schizophrenia of 1.7% in the Finnish population
prevalence rates for all mental disorders ranging from 5% (Eronen et al. 1996a). Other studies have reported an
to 16% with somewhat obviously, higher rates found in even higher incidence of psychosis. Gottleib et al. (1987)
prisoners referred for psychiatric examination in compar- studied all 251 homicides in the Copenhagen area over a
ison to random samples of the those detained (Malmquist 25-year period and reported that 20% of the male and
1995). Taylor and Gunn (1984) used a random sample of 44% of female perpetrators were psychotic.
consecutive admissions to study 1241 males remanded to In North Sweden of those convicted of homicide
Brixton prison. They recorded the incidence of schizo- between 1970 and 1980, 8% were reported to suffer
phrenia at 6% in comparison to the estimated rate in the schizophrenia with a further 4% schizophreniform psy-
South London population at that time of approximately chosis (Lindqvist 1989). These figures are compatible
0.5% (Wessley 1997). Although women have been less with a retrospective study by Petursson and Gudjonsson
frequently studied, Teplin et al. (1996) reviewed a random (1981) of all homicides in Iceland between 1900 and
sample of 1272 female detainees in North America. DSM 1979, which estimated that 15% of all homicides were
III diagnosis was determined and compared to general committed by people with schizophrenia.
44 B. PATERSON ET AL.

Community studies: The key evidence? subjects who develop schizophrenia and major affective
Mullen (1997) suggests that the study which did most to disorders including mania and bipolar illness appear
convince sceptics, particularly in the USA, of the exist- more likely to be convicted for violent crimes than non-
ence of an association between mental illness and vio- disordered subjects. Hodgins (1994) reports a study
lence was a community study by Swanson et al. (1990). involving a Danish cohort born between 1 January 1944
This study involved the reanalyses of data gathered from and 31 December 1947 (giving a sample once exclusions
10 059 interviews carried out as part of the Epidemio- for death and emigration were made of 158 799 women
logic Catchment Area Surveys (ECA). Subjects were and 164 602 men) who were followed up to age 43.
interviewed in three locations in the USA using the Hospital admission and diagnosis data were cross-
Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) to generate diagno- matched with data from the Danish National Police
sis established with reference to the Diagnostic and database to determine convictions incurred by individ-
Statistical Manual (3rd Edition) (DSM III) (American uals. For both males and females, psychiatric hospitali-
Psychiatric Association 1987). The presence of any Axis 1 zation raised the risk of offending behaviour significantly,
psychiatric diagnosis appeared to be associated with an including violent crime; the only exception was organic
increased risk of violence in this sample. However, there disease. A Finnish study by Räsänen et al. (1998) exam-
were significant effects resulting from comorbidity, the ined a sample of 11 017 women in North Finland, up to
presence of more than one disorder. This was particularly the age of 26 years, drawn from a potential group of
true for depression when coexistent with substance 12 068, who gave birth to 12 058 (live) children in 1966.
misuse. When categories were evaluated in the absence Data on psychiatric hospitalizations were derived from
of comorbidity, only schizophrenia and substance misuse the Finnish Hospital Discharge Register up until 1993,
were significantly elevated in comparison to the non- which contains all psychiatric diagnoses of patients
disordered community sample (Table 1). A more recent treated in either psychiatric or general hospital. A total of
community study by Steadman et al. (1998) of discharged 561 subjects were identified and cross-referenced with
patients, has produced somewhat different results sug- entries in the Finnish National Crime Register. In the 26
gesting that patients with a diagnosis of psychosis were years follow up, 7.5% of the study group who had a
not more dangerous in comparison to other ‘non- diagnosis of schizophrenia had been convicted of a
patients’ in their neighbourhood. However, the study did violent crime; this compared with 2.2% of non-
confirm Swanson et al. (1990) findings that substance hospitalized men. In a Danish cohort study covering a
abuse is an independent risk factor for violence and that time period of 44 years, Brennan et al. (2000) found a
its effects are compounded in psychotic substance abus- significant positive relationship between the mental dis-
ers. There remains therefore some scope for different orders that led to hospitalization and criminal violence.
interpretations of the literature (Pilgrim & Rogers 2003). Even where demographic factors and/or comorbid sub-
stance abuse were controlled for there was still a strong
Cohort studies effect.
There is, however, another valuable source of data which
support the contention that there is a link between
COMORBIDITY
mental illness and violence. Hodgins (1994) reports that
a series of Scandinavian studies using unselected ‘birth The issue of comorbidity is of interest due to the poten-
cohorts’, followed into adulthood have demonstrated that tial contribution it appears to make to the possibility of

TABLE 1: Diagnostic Interview Schedule Generated DSM III diagnosis and respondents reporting violence in preceding year (adapted from
Swanson et al. 1990)
Diagnosis Weighted no. respondents % reporting violence in preceding year

None (no disorder) 7870 2.05%


Phobia 1323 4.97%
Obsessive compulsive disorder 182 10.66%
Panic disorder 90 11.56%
Major depression 282 11.68%
Major depression with grief 308 10.70%
Mania of bipolar disorder 30 11.025
Schizophrenia of schizophreniform disorder 114 12.69%
Cannabis abuse or dependence 191 19.25%
Alcohol abuse of dependence 586 24.57%
Other drug abuse or dependence 99 34.74%
MENTAL ILLNESS AND VIOLENCE 45

violence. Results from a large-scale study of comorbidity experienced by psychotic patients, instructing violence
indicate that 54% of individuals diagnosed with one Axis pose little danger with the majority of patients not
1 DSM III diagnosis had an additional Axis 11 DSM III responding to them (Cheung et al. 1997; Hellerstein
diagnosis (Kessler et al. 1994). Most research interest has et al. 1987; McNiel & Binder 1994). Two studies by
centred on schizophrenia comorbid with alcohol or other Junginger (1990, 1995) cast doubt on this presumption,
substance misuse. The ECA study (Swanson 1994) indicating a high degree of general compliance with
clearly supported earlier research linking alcoholism and command hallucinations (39% in the first study sample
drug misuse to increased violence, although interactions and 43% in the second) by people suffering hallucina-
between substance misuse and violence have been tions. In the second study by Junginger (1995) compli-
described as complex and multidimensional (Friedman ance with ‘dangerous commands’ was investigated, 23%
1998). The link between substance misuse, specifically reported that they had complied with a ‘very dangerous
alcoholism, and violence has also been observed in hom- command’, defined as involving a very serious threat to
icide. Eronen et al. (1996a) report that although schizo- another’s safety (Junginger 1996). A study by Taylor
phrenia was found in perpetrators of homicide in Finland (1985) of incarcerated prisoners with a diagnosis of
more frequently than in the general population, alcohol schizophrenia however, found that 90% of those whose
misuse was overwhelmingly the most frequently reported criminal behaviour was ‘definitely’ or ‘probably’ influ-
mental disorder in those convicted of homicide with 40% enced by their psychosis were influenced by delusional
of male and 32% of female perpetrators diagnosed as beliefs rather than hallucinations. More recently, interest
alcoholic. The potential effect of schizophrenia or other has developed in whether there may be a ‘violence
major mental disorder with alcohol or substance misuse escalating interaction between delusions and hallucina-
appears therefore of particular concern. Eronen et al. tions’ (Bjørkly 2002; p. 612) with suggestions that in the
(1996b) report that comorbid alcoholism with schizo- absence of delusions hallucinations may only have a very
phrenia increased the risk of ‘all violence’ twofold in small violence triggering effect (Taylor et al. 1998).
comparison to a group with only schizophrenia. In Hiday (1997) asserts that it is not the presence of
Räsänen et al.’s (1988) study comorbid alcoholism dra- specific psychotic symptoms, or even command hallu-
matically increased the likelihood of conviction for a cinations that lead to violence, given that the majority of
violent crime with 36% of individuals with schizophrenia these are not acted upon. One suggestion proposed is
and a comorbid diagnosis of alcoholism being convicted that violence may be related to the severity of symptoms
of a violent crime over a period in comparison to 7.5% of the individual experiences. Link et al. (1992) in a com-
those with schizophrenia alone. munity study in New York found psychiatric patients,
when compared to matched community residents, had
significantly higher rates of arrest on all indices of violent
HOW DOES MENTAL ILLNESS INCREASE behaviour. Link et al. (1992) then controlled for psy-
THE RISK OF VIOLENCE? chotic symptoms using a 13-item scale and found the
Even when the role of comorbid alcohol or substance differences in violence between the patient and commu-
misuse is excluded there is strong evidence from both nity sample were significantly reduced suggesting that
community and cohort studies to support the hypothesis the greater the number and higher the severity of symp-
that certain forms of severe mental illness, particularly toms experienced, the greater the likelihood was that an
schizophrenia and schizophreniform psychoses, appear individual would be violent. Link and Stueve (1995) went
to be associated with an increased risk of violence. This on, however, to hypothesize that it was not the presence
has led to research seeking to explain why these diag- or severity of all symptoms which increased the risk of
noses may increase the risk of violence. Historically the violence but the presence of a particular constellation of
presumption has been that psychosis somehow removes symptoms that produced feelings of personal threat or
or inhibits an individual’s capacity for self control and in involved the intrusion of thoughts which could ‘over ride’
so doing removes the psycho–dynamic barriers which the individual’s self control. This presumes, essentially,
prevent violence (Gerbner et al. 1981). Interest to date that the internal constraints which function to restrict
has focused on two particular subtypes of psychotic symp- violent behaviour in line with relevant social norms are
toms; first, ‘command hallucinations’ (Shawyer et al., more likely to be over ridden when someone with psy-
2003) and second, the role of ‘delusions of persecution’ chosis believed that they themselves are in danger from
(Taylor et al. 1994). In command hallucinations, the indi- an individual, group of persons or organization. Link and
vidual experiences direct instructions to carry out an act, Stueve (1995; p. 143) describe this as the ‘principle of
usually in the form of an auditory hallucination (Junginger rationality – within irrationality’, if we accept that the
1996). There are claims that command hallucinations, patient’s experience while delusional is experienced by
46 B. PATERSON ET AL.

them as ‘real’ then their actions are logically consistent an association between high levels of ‘threat control over
with their beliefs. Link et al. returned to this general ride’ (TCO) symptoms and violence. Cheung et al. (1997)
premise in 1999, suggesting that the relationship investigated the nature of delusional beliefs in a patient
between delusional ideation and the consequent actions sample with a DSM III-R diagnosis of schizophrenia.
of a patient are examples of ‘The Thomas Theorem’: They found that violent patients (defined as having one
Within sociology the Thomas Theorem has been closely violent incident or more per month) had a significantly
associated with a symbolic interactionist approach that higher frequency of delusions of persecution than
directs attention to the importance of understanding patients categorized as ‘non- violent’, supporting the
peoples interpretations of situations for understanding premise that it is the nature of the delusional beliefs,
their behaviour (Link et al. 1999; p. 316) rather than simply the presence of delusional beliefs, that
In a 1995 study Link and Steuve investigated their may influence rates of violence.
hypothesis of ‘rationality within irrationality’. The study Link and Stueve’s (1994) model of ‘threat control-
compared 232 patients (78 with a diagnosis of major override’ causing violence by creating fear and anger and
depression, 45 with schizophrenia, 24 with another psy- impairing internal controls, is however, essentially an
chotic disorder, and 85 with diagnosis other than depres- intra-psychic one in which violence is mediated by factors
sion or psychosis) with a community sample of 521 unique to the perpetrator. Hiday (1997) reminds us that
matched for socio-demographic characteristics. The this is perhaps overly simplistic, asserting that both
presence of ‘threat control override’ symptoms was meas- interpersonal and social factors may play their part in the
ured using a 13-item psychotic symptoms scale (Table 2) promotion of violence. Hiday (1997; p. 400) has sug-
in which symptoms were separated into those believed to gested that the relationships between mental illness and
characterize this phenomena (1–3) and other symptoms violence are mainly indirect and contingent rather than
of psychosis believed to be unrelated to it (4–13). Each linear and that both mental illness and violence must be
scale item was measured on a 1–4 continuum from very understood in terms of the ‘structural arrangements in
often (4) to never (1), each section was then summed to which individuals are embedded’. An individual experi-
give a maximum possible score for threat override of 12 encing high levels of symptoms may become involved in
and other psychotic symptoms of 40. tense situations where their delusional behaviour is prob-
Link and Stueve (1995) found individuals who scored lematic for others or where others attempts to remon-
highly on the threat control override scale (with a score of strate with them or to encourage them to comply with
6–12) were significantly over-represented in terms of treatment create conflict (Hiday 1997). Novaco (1976)
violence in comparison to those with low levels or who has observed that anger as a form of arousal is a conse-
had high scores on the other psychotic symptoms inven- quence of interpretation of an event or incident as
tory. These findings have been supported by subsequent provocative. His perspective views emotion largely as a
research by Swanson et al. (1996, 1997) on community consequence of an internal mental process involving an
samples and inpatient populations showing evidence of interaction between an individual’s interpretations of

TABLE 2: Psychotic Symptom Scale separated into threat/control – override and other psychotic symptom subscales
Psychotic Symptom Scale

Threat control – override symptoms


During the past year
1. How often have you felt that your mind was dominated by forces beyond your control?
2. How often have you felt that that thoughts were put into your head that were not your own?
3. How often have you felt that there were people who wished to do you harm?
Other psychotic symptoms
During the past year
1. How often have you felt that you did not exist at all, that you are dead, dissolved?
2. How often have you heard things that other people say they can’t hear?
3. How often have you felt your unspoken words were being broadcast?
4. How often have you thought that you were possessed by a spirit or the devil?
5. How often have you had visions or seen things that other people say they can’t see?
6. How often have you felt that you had special powers?
7. How often have you thought that something odd was going on?
8. How often have you felt your thoughts were being taken away from you by some external force
9. How often have you had ideas or thoughts that nobody else would understand if you talked about them
10. How often have you seemed to hear your thought spoken aloud – almost as if someone standing nearby could hear them?
MENTAL ILLNESS AND VIOLENCE 47

events. An individual labouring under persecutory delu- Anti-social personality disorder (ASP) is an Axis II diag-
sions, particularly if the delusion involves a threat to their nosis in DSM IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994)
safety, according to Link and Stueve (1994) ‘rationality the primary features of which are evident from childhood
within irrationality principal’ would be expected to expe- and include persistent irresponsible behaviour, impuls-
rience a high level of arousal. The interaction between ivity and aggression including violence. The concept of
arousal and anger has been described as ‘invidious’ with personality disorder in its various forms and its use as a
high arousal levels increasing the likelihood of negative diagnosis remain controversial, with critics asserting that
attributions in respect of the actions of or intent of others they are used to medicalize a range of deviant behaviour
(Mueller 1983). The experience, or perceived experience, (Hart et al. 1994). ASP has been linked to violence but
of conflict with another may then be sufficient to increase this link has been suggested to be ‘tautological’ in that an
their arousal level to a point where their cognitive control individual who exhibits violent behaviour may attract the
is overwhelmed (Breakwell & Rowett 1989). It remains diagnosis of ASP. If the individual then goes on to exhibit
important to recognize that patients’ reported perceptions further violence then the diagnosis of ASP is said to
may not always reflect paranoia but sometimes well predict violence (Hiday 1997). However, ASP is found
grounded and realistic fears of being a victim of violence frequently in association with severe mental illness and
based on experience (Estroff & Zimmer 1994). substance abuse, particularly it is suggested among those
The likelihood of any given conflict situation resulting who have a history of violence (Robbins 1993). Webster
in violence, will however be influenced by the predom- et al. (1994) suggest that comorbid personality disorder
inant beliefs within the participants’ cultures regarding significantly increases the risk of future violence by
how conflict should be dealt with and thus the ‘legit- people with severe mental illness.
imacy’ or otherwise of violence (Werner et al. 1983). Hiday’s (1997) argument compellingly illustrates that
Hiday (1997) argues that beliefs supporting violence as a violence perpetrated by people experiencing mental
means of conflict resolution may be more prevalent in illness shares many characteristics with non-disordered
communities which have been disproportionately samples in terms of the structural factors which influence
affected by unemployment, where social disorganization it. However, there is another similarly of note, that likely
and poverty function as stressors, and individuals or victims are overwhelmingly known to the perpetrator and
groups may become disassociated from the values of the often intimates (i.e. family members) (Estroff & Zimmer
wider community. Growing up in such an environment is 1994). Individuals experiencing severe mental illness may
likely to increase an individual’s risk of being a victim of on occasion kill someone they do not know but this is
violence in comparison to more affluent areas (National highly unusual (Lindqvist & Allebeck 1989).
Research Council 1993) and may over time ‘foster mis-
trust and suspicion of others, an outlook that psychosis
exacerbates’ (Stueve & Link 1997; p. 329). Such influ- ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS
ences may find expression in violence by patients. Silver It is tempting to observe that more sophisticated larger
2001; p. 405) in a study involving some 270 discharged scale and/or longitudinal studies were able to identify a
patients found that environment functioned as an inde- relationship between mental illness and violence where
pendent risk factor for violence. Even when a range of earlier research either failed or was compromised by
other variables including gender, diagnosis and level of methodological problems most notably ascertainment
symptoms were controlled for, ‘patients discharged into bias. Monahan and Steadman (1983; p. 156) noted that:
disadvantaged neighbourhoods were significantly more
likely to commit violent acts than patients discharged into in the four reports published before 1965 … released
mental patients were found to have rates of arrest lower
less disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Hiday (1997) argues
than those of the general population.
that social disorganization and poverty are found in
increased association with substance dependency creat- As is evident from this review such findings were
ing an invidious combination. The role of alcohol and increasingly contradicted by research which began emerg-
substance misuse as precursors to violence is complex ing in the US as early as the late 1960s (Rappeport &
and outside the scope of this review, but it has already Lassen 1965). Despite continuing problems with some of
been noted that there is an association, and that comor- this literature, such studies often found rates for crime
bid alcohol or other substance misuse appears to signifi- among discharged patients to be significantly higher than in
cantly increase the risk of violence in those with major the general population, particularly for violent crimes. This
mental disorders. Hiday (1997) argues further that social change cannot be readily attributed to methodological
disorganization and poverty may increase the prevalence improvements in discharge studies evident in some of the
of antisocial personality disorder in communities affected. more recent work but a highly plausible alternative
48 B. PATERSON ET AL.

explanation exists. It proposes that rather than represent- ization there are continuing inconsistencies between
ing better experimental design the findings of such studies both in respect of how violence is defined and
studies reflected the gradual emergence of highly signifi- how mental illness is ascertained that makes comparative
cant changes in the nature of the psychiatric hospital analysis extremely difficult (Mullen 1997). In addition,
population in which de-institutionalization may have data established from a study in one country or area may
played a major part. Psychiatric hospital bed numbers be of little general significance if the magnitude of the
have radically dropped in many countries across the link between mental illness and violence is context spe-
world over the last few decades (Thornicroft & Bebbing- cific and culturally mediated (Stueve & Link 1997). Even
ton 1989). This downward trend commenced in the late although a potential link appears to have been estab-
1950s in the US, continued steadily throughout the 1960s lished between severe mental illness and violence it is
and 1970s before rapidly accelerating, in the UK, in the clear that a much smaller group with particular symp-
context of the ‘community care’ programmes of the toms may present the greatest risk (Link & Stueve 1995).
1990s. It has, however, been suggested that as alternative The risk of violence arising in individuals appears to be
to hospitals developed (albeit at differing rates in differ- significantly increased when comorbid substance misuse
ent settings) newer community resources tended to and/or ASP is present. Substance misuse increases the
‘cream off’ those patients deemed more suitable for risk of violence, independent of mental illness, even
discharge and/or community treatment. This, it is sug- where socio-demographic variables are carefully control-
gested, has led some services to an overall increase in the led for (Monahan 2002). However, the literature on
dependency of the hospital population. In the UK, even violence by people with mental illness, including both
in the midst of de-institutionalization, many patients community studies and studies of convicted offenders,
continued to be admitted to hospital, often those with strongly indicates that comorbid alcoholism and/or sub-
intractable psychoses and high levels of symptoms (Royal stance misuse appears to multiply their likelihood of
Commission 1979). An inevitable corollary of a change in violence. Given the reliance of many studies on written
the population admitted will be, over time, a change in reports, arrest records or case records, the strength of the
the nature of those discharged. If the number of patients link may in fact be underestimated as studies comparing
discharged experiencing psychoses increases as the pro- self reported incidents with case records suggest ‘vio-
portion of patients overall then, given the association lence and offending are under-reported in case-records’
observed between schizophrenia and violence, we might (Wright et al. 2002; p. 49).
reasonably expect the proportion of patients arrested or People experiencing severe mental illness emerge
otherwise involved in violence to increase post-discharge from this selective review of the research as appearing to
(Monahan & Steadman 1983). It has also been argued account for only a small proportion of the violence in
that moves towards deinstitutionalization have unfortu- society (Link & Stueve 1998). This proportion may vary
nately occurred during a time period in which changes in however, and homicides and other violence perpetrated
social attitudes towards drugs have led to much greater by people with mental illness may form a much greater
use of illicit substances and with significant increases in proportion of the overall total in societies where the
average alcohol consumption and consequently in alcohol overall rate of violent crime is lower (Mullen 1997).
dependency and alcoholism both among the general Violence by people experiencing mental illness, while
population and in people experiencing severe mental clearly not insignificant, makes it would appear only a
illness (Regier et al. 1990). Given the invidious relation- relatively modest contribution to overall rates of violence
ship clearly demonstrated between severe mental illness, in most societies in comparison to other factors such as
substance abuse, alcoholism and violence we would age, gender and alcohol. There is, however, now a
expect this to be reflected in admission data and this is considerable literature indicating that the public percep-
evident in a number of studies (Wheatley 1998). If the tion of the nature and extent of the association between
population being admitted and discharged has signifi- violence and mental illness may be both distorted and
cantly altered, as seems evident in terms of both the exaggerated which suggests that considerable work needs
prevalence of psychoses and the increasing frequency of to be done to inform the public more accurately (Crisp
comorbidity, it is perhaps unsurprising that the fre- 2001).
quency of violence reported has appeared to increase. It is also important to acknowledge despite the
conclusions of this review and others (Angermeyer
2000; Mullen 1997) that there remains a body of
CONCLUSION opinion who consider that ‘the evidence that psychosis
There remain significant problems with interpreting the predicts violence is contested’ (Pilgrim & Rogers 2003;
literature in this area; despite repeated calls for standard- p. 8).
MENTAL ILLNESS AND VIOLENCE 49

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Daffern, M., Mayer, M. M. & Martin, T. (2003). A preliminary


investigation into patterns of aggression in an Australian
The authors would like to thank Sandy McComish for forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry
commenting on this review. and Psychology, 14, 67–84.
Eronen, M., Panu, P. & Tiihonen, J. (1996a). Mental disorders
and homicidal behaviour in Finland. Archives of General
REFERENCES Psychiatry, 53, 497–501.
American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statis- Eronen, M., Tiihonen, J. & Hakola, P. (1996b). Schizophrenia
tical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd edn, revised. Wash- and homicidal behaviour. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 83–89.
ington DC: American Psychiatric Association. Estroff, S. E. & Zimmer, C. (1994). Social networks, social sup-
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statis- ports and violence among persons experiencing severe and
tical Manual of Mental Disorders II. Washington DC: Amer- persistent mental illness. In J. Monahan & H. Steadman
ican Psychiatric Association. (Eds). Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments In Risk
Anfang, S. A. & Applebaum, P. S. (1996). Twenty years after Assessment. (pp. 81–98) Chicago: University of Chicago
Tarasoff: reviewing the duty to protect. Harvard Review of Press.
Psychiatry, 4, 67–76. Fottrell, E. (1980). A study among patients of violent behaviour
Angermeyer, M. C. (2000). Schizophrenia and Violence. Acta among patients in psychiatric hospitals. British Journal of
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 102 (Suppl. 407), 63–67. Psychiatry, 136, 216–221.
Asnis, G. M., Kaplan, M. L., Hundoerfean, G. & Saheed, W. Foust, D. & Rhee, K. J. (1993). The incidence of battery in an
(1997). Violence and homicidal behaviours in psychiatric urban emergency department. Annals of Emergency Medi-
disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20, cine, 22, 583–585.
405–425.
Friedman, A. S. (1998). Substance use/abuse as a predictor to
Beck, J. C., ed. (1985). The Potentially Violent Patient and the illegal and violent behaviour a review of the relevant liter-
Tarasoff Decision in Psychiatric Practice. New York: Ameri- ature. Aggression and Violent Behaviour, 3, 339–353.
can Psychiatric Press.
Garnham, P. (2001). Understanding and dealing with anger,
Belfrage, H. (1998). A ten year follow-up of criminality in aggression and violence. Nursing Standard, 16 (6), 37–42.
Stockholm mental patients: new evidence for a relation
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M. & Signorelli, N. (1981).
between violence and mental disorder and crime. British
Health and medicine on television. New England Journal of
Journal of Criminology, 38, 145–155.
Medicine, 305, 901–904.
Binder, R. L. & McNiel, D. E. (1988). Effects of diagnosis and
context on dangerousness, American Journal of Psychiatry, Gottleib, P., Gabrielson, G. & Kramp, P. (1987). Increasing
145, 728–732. homicides in Copenhagen from 1959 to 1983. Acta Psychiat-
rica Scandinavica, 77, 301–308.
Bjørkly, S. (2002). Psychotic symptoms and violence towards
others: a literature review of some preliminary findings. Gunn, J. (1977). Criminal behaviour and mental disorder.
Aggression and Violent Behaviour, 7, 602–605. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 317–329.
Breakwell, G. & Rowett, C. (1989). Violence in social work. In Hafner, H. & Boker W. (1992). Crimes of Violence by Mentally
K. Browne (Ed.). Human Aggression Naturalistic Abnormal Offenders. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Approaches. London: Routledge. Press.
Brennan, P. A., Mednick, S. A. & Hodgins, S. (2000). Major Haller, R. M. & Deluty, R. H. (1988). Assaults on staff by
mental disorders and criminal violence in a Danish birth psychiatric in-patients: a critical review. British Journal of
cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 494–500. Psychiatry, 152, 174–179.
Carstenson, P. C. (1994). The evolving duty of mental health Hart, S. D., Hare, R. D. & Forth, A. E. (1994). Psychopathy as a
professionals to third parties. International Journal of Law risk marker for violence: Development and validation of a
and Psychiatry, 17, 1–42. screening version of the revised psychopathy checklist. In
Cheung, P., Schweitzer, I., Crowley, K. & Tuckwell, V. (1997). J. Monahan & H. Steadman (Eds). Violence and Mental
Violence in schizophrenia: role of hallucinations and delu- Disorder: Developments In Risk Assessment (pp. 81–98)
sions. Schizophrenia Research, 26, 181–190. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Conacher, N. G. (1996). Psychiatric hospital downsizing and the Hellerstein, D., Frosch, W. & Koeningsberg, H. (1987). The
Penrose effect. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, clinical significance of command hallucinations. American
184, 708–710. Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 219–221.
Cooper, S. J., Browne, F. W. A., McLean, K. J. & King, D. J. Hiday, V. A. (1997). Understanding the connection between
(1983). Aggressive behaviour in a psychiatric observation mental health and violence. International Journal of Law
ward. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 68, 386–393. and Psychiatry, 20, 399–417.
Crisp, A. H., Gelder, M., Rix, S., Metzler, H. I. & Rowlands, O. J. Hodgins, S. (1994). Schizophrenia and violence: are new mental
(2001). Stigmatisation of people with mental illness. British health policies needed? Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 5,
Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 4–7. 473–477.
50 B. PATERSON ET AL.

Infantino, J. & Musingo, S. Y. (1985). Assaults and injuries Lion, J. R., Snyder, W. & Merril, G. (1991). Under reporting of
among staff with and without training in aggression control assaults on staff in a state hospital. Journal of Hospital and
techniques. Journal of Hospital and Community Psychiatry, Community Psychiatry, 34, 44–47.
36, 1312–1314. Malmquist, C. P. (1995). Depression and homicidal violence.
Johnston, E., Crow, T., Johnson, A. & Macmillan, F. (1986). International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 18, 145–162.
The Northwick Park Study of first episodes of schizophrenia Mason, T. (1998). Tarasoff liability: its impact for working with
1. Presentation of the illness and problems relating to admis- patients who threaten others. International Journal of
sion. British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 51–56. Nursing Studies, 35, 109–114.
Junginger, J. (1990). Predicting compliance with command hal- McNiel, D. E. & Binder, L. (1994) Relationship between acute
lucinations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 245–247. psychiatric symptoms, diagnosis and short-term risk of
Junginger, J. (1995). Command hallucinations and the predic- violence. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 45, 133–137.
tion of dangerousness. Psychiatric Services, 46, 911–914. Monahan, J. (1992). Mental disorder and violent behaviour:
Junginger, J. (1996). Psychosis and violence: the case for a perceptions and evidence. American Psychologist, 47,
content analysis of psychotic experience. Schizophrenia Bul- 511–521.
letin, 22, 91–101.
Monahan, J. (1993). Mental disorder and violence: another
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A. & Zhao, S. (1994). Lifetime look. In A. Hodgins (Ed.). Mental Disorder and Crime
and twelve month prevalence of DSM III-R diagnosis in the (pp. 287–302). California: Sage.
United States: Results From The National Comorbidity
Monahan, J. (2002). The Macarthur studies of violent risk.
Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19.
Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 12, 567–572.
Klassen, D. & O’Connor, W. (1988). Crime, inpatient admis-
Monahan, J. & Steadman, H. (1983). Crime and mental illness:
sions and violence among male hospital patients. Inter-
an epidemiological approach. In N. Morris & M. Tonry
national Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 11, 305–312.
(Eds). Crime and Justice, Vol. 4 (pp. 145–189). Chicago:
Lagos, J., Perlmutter, K. & Saxinger, H. (1977). Fear of the University of Chicago Press.
mentally ill: empirical support for the common mans
response. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1134–1137. Monahan, J. & Steadman, H. (1994). Toward a rejuvenation of
risk assessment research. In J. Monahan & H. Steadman
Larkin, E., Murtagh, S. & Jones, S. (1988). A preliminary study
(Eds). Violence and Mental Disorder Developments in Risk
of violent incidents in a special hospital. British Journal of
Assessment, (pp. 1–17) Chicago: University of Chicago
Psychiatry, 153, 226–231.
Press.
Lidz, C. W. & Mulvey, E. P. (1995). Dangerousness: from legal
Monahan, J. & Walker, L. (1994). Social Sciences in Law, Cases
definition to theoretical research. Law and Human Behav-
and Materials, 3rd edn. New York: Foundation Press.
iour, 19, 41–48.
Mueller, C. W. (1983). Environmental Stressors and Aggressive
Lindqvist, P. (1989). Violence against the person: the role of
behaviour. In R. G. Green & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds).
mental disorder and abuse. Umayer University Medical
Aggression: Theoretical and Empirical Reviews, Vol. 2.
Dissertations New Series, 254, Umea, Sweden.
(pp. 135–162). New York: Academic Press.
Lindqvist, P. & Allebeck, P. (1990). Criminal homicide in North
West Sweden 1970–81. Alcohol intoxication, alcohol abuse Mullen, P. E. (1997). A reassessment of the link between
and mental disease. International Journal of Law and mental disorder and violent behaviour and its implications
Psychiatry, 8, 19–37. for clinical practice. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry, 31 (3), 3–11.
Link, B. G., Andrews, H. & Cullen, F. T. (1992). The violent
and illegal behaviour of mental patients reconsidered. Amer- National Research Council (1993). Understanding and Prevent-
ican Sociological Review, 57, 275–292. ing Violence. Washington DC: National Academy Press.
Link, B. G., Monahan, J., Stueve, A. & Cullen, F. T. (1999). Novaco, R. (1976). The function and regulation of the arousal of
Real in their consequences: a sociological approach to anger. American Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 1124–1128.
understanding the association between psychotic symptoms Paterson, B. & Stark, C. (2001). Social policy and mental illness
and violence. American Sociological Review, 64, 316–330. in England in the 1990s: violence, moral panic and critical
Link, B. G. & Stueve, A. (1994). Psychotic symptoms and the discourse. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nurs-
violent/illegal behaviour of mental patients compared to ing, 8, 257–267.
community controls. In J. Monahan & H. Steadman (Eds). Petursson, H. & Gudjonsson, G. H. (1981). Psychiatric
Violence and Mental Disorder Developments in Risk Assess- aspects of homicide. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 64,
ment, (pp. 136–160). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 363–372.
Link, B. G. & Stueve, A. (1995). Evidence bearing on mental Pilgrim, D. & Rogers, A. (2003). Mental disorder and violence:
illness as a possible cause of violent behaviour. Epidemio- An empirical picture in Context. Journal of Mental Health,
logical Reviews, 19, 1–19. 12, 7–18.
Link, B. G. & Stueve, A. (1998). New evidence on the violence Powell, G., Cann, W. & Crowe, M. (1994). What events
risk posed by people with mental illness. Archives of General precede violent incidents in psychiatric hospitals? British
Psychiatry, 55, 403–404. Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 107–112.
MENTAL ILLNESS AND VIOLENCE 51

Rabkin, J. (1979). Criminal behaviour of discharged mental Stueve, A. & Link, B. G. (1997). Violence and psychiatric disor-
patients: a critical appraisal of the research. Psychological ders results from an epidemiological study of young adults in
Bulletin, 86, 1–29. Israel. Psychiatric Quarterly, 68, 327–342.
Rappeport, J. R. & Lassen, G. (1965). Dangerousness: arrest Swanson, J. W. (1994). Mental disorder, substance abuse and
rate comparison of discharged patients and the general pop- community violence: an epidemiological approach. In
ulation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 413–419. J. Monahan & H. J. Steadman (Eds). Violence and Mental
Räsänen, P., Tiihonen, J., Isohanni, M., Rantakallio, P., Lehto- Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment (pp. 101–136).
nen, J. & Moring, J. (1998). Schizophrenia, alcohol abuse Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
and violent behaviour: a 26-year follow up study. Schizo- Swanson, J. W., Borum, R., Monahan, J. & Swartz, M. (1996).
phrenia Bulletin, 24, 437–441. Psychotic symptoms and the risk of violent behaviour:
Regier, D. A., Farmer, M. E., Rae, D. S. et al. (1990). Comor- evidence from the epidemiologic catchment area surveys.
bidity of mental disorders with alcohol and other drug abuse: Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 41, 317–338.
results from the epidemiologic catchment area (ECA) study. Swanson, J. W., Estroff, S., Swartz, M. et al. (1997). Violence
Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, and severe mental disorder in clinical and community popu-
2511–2518. lations: The effects of psychotic symptoms, comorbidity and
Robbins, L. (1993). Childhood conduct problems, adult psycho- lack of treatment. Psychiatry, 60, 1–22.
pathology and crime. In S. Hodgins (Ed.). Mental Disorder Swanson, J. W., Holzer, C., Ganja, V. & Jono, R. (1990). Vio-
and Crime, (pp. 173–193). London: Sage. lence and psychiatric disorder in the community: evidence
Rossi, A. M., Jacobs, M. & Monteleone, M. (1986). Characteris- from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area Surveys. Hospital
tics of psychiatric patients who engage in assaultive behav- and Community Psychiatry, 41, 761–770.
iour and or other fear inducing behaviours. Journal of Taylor, P. J. (1985). Motives for offending among violent and
Nervous and Mental Diseases, 174, 154–160. psychotic men. British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 491–498.
Royal Commission On the National Health Service (1979).
Taylor, P. J., Garety, P., Buchanan, A. et al. (1994). Delusions
London: HMSO.
and Violence. In J. Monahan & H. J. Steadman (Eds).
Sclafani, M. J. (2000). Developing a clinical violence prevention Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk
and intervention plan for psychiatric mental healthcare set- Assessment (pp. 161–182). Chicago: University of Chicago
tings. Journal of Health Care Quality, 22 (2), 8–12. Press.
Shah, S. (1975). Dangerousness and civil commitment of the Taylor, P. J. & Gunn, J. (1984). Violence and psychosis, 1: risk
mentally ill: some public policy considerations. American of violence amongst psychotic men. British Medical Journal,
Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 501–505. 288, 1945–1949.
Shawyer, F., Mackinnon, A., Farhall, J., Trauser, T. & Coplov, D.
Taylor, P. J., Leese, M., Williams, D., Butwell, M., Daly, R. &
(2003). Command hallucinations and violence. implications
Larkin, E. (1998). Mental disorder and violence: a special
for detention and treatment. Psychiatry, Psychology and the
(high security) hospital study. British Journal of Psychiatry,
Law, 10, 97–101.
172, 218–226.
Shepherd, M. & Lavender, T. (1999). Putting aggression into
Teplin, L. A., Abram, K. M. & McClelland, G. M., (1996). The
context, an investigation into the contextual factors influenc-
prevalence of psychiatric disorder among incarcerated
ing the rate of aggressive incidents in a psychiatric hospital.
women 1: Pre-trial detainees. Archives of General Psychi-
Journal of Mental Health, 8, 159–170.
atry, 53, 505–512.
Sheridan, M., Henrion, R., Robinson, L. & Baxter, V. (1990).
Precipitants of violence in a psychiatric inpatient setting. Thornicroft, G. & Bebbington, P. (1989). Deinstitutionalisation
Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 41, 776–780. – from hospital closure to service development. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 739–753.
Silver, E. (2001). Neighbourhood social disorganisation as a
cofactor in violence among people with mental health Webster, D., Harris, G. T., Rice, M., Cormier, C. & Quinsay, V. L.
problems. International Journal of Offender Therapy and (1994). The Violence Prediction Scheme. Toronto: University
Comparative Criminology, 45, 403–406. of Toronto.
Steadman, H. J. & Cocozza, J. J. (1974). Careers of the Crim- Wenk, E., Robinson, J. O. & Smith, J. W. (1972). Can violence
inally Insane: Excessive Social Control of Deviance. Lexing- be predicted. Crime and Delinquency, 18, 393–394.
ton MA: Lexington Books. Werner, P. D., Rose, T. K. & Yesavage, J. A. (1983). Reliability,
Steadman, H. J., Monahan, J., Applebaum, P. S. et al. (1994). accuracy and decision making strategy in clinical predictions
Developing a new generation of risk assessment research. In of imminent dangerousness. Journal of Consulting Clinical
J. Monahan & H. J. Steadman (Eds). Violence and Mental Psychology, 51, 812–825.
Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment (pp. 297–318). Wessley, S. (1997). The epidemiology of crime, violence and
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170 (Suppl.
Steadman, H. J., Mulvey, E. P., Monahan, J. et al. (1998). Vio- 32), 8–11.
lence by people discharged from acute psychiatric inpatient Wessley, S. & Taylor, P. (1991). Madness and crime: crimin-
facilities and by others in the same neighbourhoods. ology vs psychiatry. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health,
Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 393–401. 1, 193–228.
52 B. PATERSON ET AL.

Wheatley, M. (1998). The prevalence and relevance of sub- Wright, S., Gournay, K., Glorney, E. & Thornicroft, G. (2002).
stance use in detained schizophrenic patients. The Journal of Mental Illness substance abuse demographics: dual diag-
Forensic Psychiatry, 9, 114–129. nosis in the suburbs. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 13,
35–52.

You might also like