You are on page 1of 19

Social Factors in the Development of

Autobiographical Memory:
The State of the Art
Elaine Reese, Clark University

Abstract
This review addresses theory and research on the role of self understanding, language,
theory of mind, attachment security, and parental style in children’s autobiographical
memory development. Social-cognitive factors appear to interact with parental style
in producing children’s first verbal memories. Emotional factors, such as attachment
security, may also prove to be critical for the socialization of life histories. Further
longitudinal studies will be necessary to examine individual differences in the growth
of this complex and multiply determined skill.

Keywords: autobiographical memory; self understanding, theory of mind; attachment


security

Autobiographical memory is a complex and multiply determined skill, consisting of


neurological, social, cognitive, and linguistic components. At the most basic level,
autobiographical memories refer to personally experienced past events. More conser-
vative definitions are that memories of past events are only autobiographical if they
are verbally accessible (Pillemer & White, 1989), in narrative form (Nelson, 1996),
involve a sense of recollection (Perner, in press a) and constitute part of the life story
(Nelson, 1996). One problem with stipulating recollection as a prerequisite for auto-
biographical memory is that recollection is difficult to measure in adults, and may be
impossible to measure in preverbal infants (Rovee-Collier, 1997). A further problem
with restricting autobiographical memories to those that fit into the life story is that
often it is impossible to know at the time of an event how important that event will
later be to the life story. Indeed, autobiographical memories may fall in or out of the
life story depending on the stage of life and the context in which the life story is being
told (e.g., Linde, 1993).
Most autobiographical memories, however, do involve verbal communication.
Although adults sometimes engage in private reminiscing that consists mostly of
images, a primary purpose of reminiscing is social (Hyman & Faries, 1992). This com-
munication of autobiographical memories with and to others necessarily entails that
the memory be verbally accessible. Focusing on verbally accessible memories does
not preclude very young children from the phenomenon of interest. Children start to
talk about the past nearly as soon as they begin to talk, at about 18 months of age

Correspondence to Elaine Reese, Clark University, Department of Psychology, 950 Main Street,
Worcester, MA 01610, USA. Email: ereese@clarku.edu

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002. Published by Blackwell Publishers, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street,
Malden, MA 02148, USA.
Autobiographical Memory Development 125
(Reese, 1999; Sachs, 1983). Children’s early verbal reports only consist of two-word
associations, at most, whereas by the time they are 3-1/2 years old, they are capable
of telling a fairly coherent story about a past event with little prompting from an adult
(Fivush, Haden, & Adam, 1995; see Reese, in press, for a review of the research on
children’s very early verbal memory).
The goal of this review is to provide an update of recent advances in the field of
children’s autobiographical memory development. The focus will be on verbally acces-
sible memories of past events, because these memories are the best possible candi-
dates for eventually making their way into the life story. The neurological components
of memory and the growth of nonverbal memory in infancy and early childhood have
been reviewed extensively (see Barr & Hayne, 2000; Howe & Courage, 1993, 1997;
Nelson, 1995; Wheeler, Stuss, & Tulving, 1997). Recent reviews have also covered
the role of stress (Howe, 1997) and suggestibility in children’s event memory (Ceci
& Bruck, 1993). This review will instead focus on the social, cognitive, and linguis-
tic components of autobiographical memory, going into depth on those recent studies
that have related children’s autobiographical memory to independent assessments of
their social, cognitive and linguistic skills. Most of the studies reviewed have not
assessed the accuracy of the autobiographical memories, because for the purpose of
building up a life history, accuracy may not be the most important criterion for a
memory’s eventual significance. Instead, a belief in the value of the memory, whether
true or false, for the self may be a more important determinant of what gets into the
life story (Singer & Salovey, 1993). That is, if one believes that the event really hap-
pened, if one bases his/her identity in part upon that event, and if one bases his/her
future behavior upon that event, then the event is a true autobiographical memory.

Recent Theories of Autobiographical Memory Development


The last decade saw a veritable explosion in theories of autobiographical memory
development. Most of these theories have been cast in terms of explaining infantile
amnesia, the phenomenon by which adults cannot recall events from their early child-
hood (Dudycha & Dudycha, 1941). The conundrum is that if indeed children show
evidence of long-term memory at an earlier age than previously thought possible, why
then are adults unable to recall most of their early childhood experiences? The average
age of adults’ earliest childhood memories is between 3 and 3-1/2 years in most
retrospective studies. Thus, the task became to understand why infantile amnesia
ended (and presumably true autobiographical memory began) at the beginning of the
preschool period.

(i) Primary influence theories. One group of theories rests on the assumption that
children lack a critical cognitive or social-cognitive framework before this age that
would enable them to encode memories in such a way that they could later be retrieved
as self relevant. Howe and Courage (1993, 1997), for instance, proposed that visual
self-recognition, as commonly measured in the mirror task of self-recognition (MSR;
Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979), was the critical conceptual framework enabling auto-
biographical memory. Before children pass the MSR task at about 18 months to 2
years, they are not capable of encoding and storing memories as self relevant. Later,
when trying to retrieve these memories from the perspective of things that happened
to ‘me,’ they are unsuccessful because they did not yet have a ‘me’ to which to attach
the memory. Growth in self understanding continues to occur after age 2 years, of
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
126 Elaine Reese
course, but in this theory MSR is the necessary prerequisite for autobiographical
encoding and storage. Povinelli (1995; see also Suddendorf & Corballis, 1997 for
related ideas) also hypothesized the importance of self understanding for autobio-
graphical memory, but believed that children do not have a ‘proper’ self until they are
capable of understanding delayed self-recognition: I am the same person today that I
was yesterday and last year. Povinelli, Landau, and Perilloux (1996) developed an inge-
nious task to measure delayed self-recognition (DSR) that is analogous to MSR but
with a built-in delay. Children are marked surreptitiously with a sticker and after a
delay are shown a video or photo of their marked selves. Children who will reach up
to obtain the sticker after a brief delay purportedly understand the connection between
a past and present self and are capable of autobiographical memory. Povinelli (1995)
predicted that a proper self comes into play at around 4 years of age, after sufficient
representational awareness has been achieved. Conway and Pleydell-Pearce (2000), in
their cognitive-motivational theory of adults’ autobiographical memory, concurred
with these theorists that the main task of autobiographical memory is self grounding.
Conway and Pleydell-Pearce instead focus on the incompatibility of goals for the self
in the first five years of life (e.g., basic attachment goals) and adults’ self goals (e.g.,
identity goals) as the primary reason for infantile amnesia. The mismatch between
self goals at encoding and retrieval is theorized to result in an inability to reconstruct
memories from earliest childhood.
Like Povinelli (1995), Perner and Ruffman’s (1995) theory emphasized children’s
growing understanding of mind at around age 4 years as a critical prerequisite for
autobiographical memory. For Perner and Ruffman (1995), however, the critical
accomplishment occurs as children gain a better understanding of the link between
what they know and how they know it (origins of knowledge understanding). With
this understanding, children become capable of true recollection. As they remember
a past event, they can re-experience the event. Only then is their memory truly episodic
or autobiographical. Perner (in press, a) later refined his reason for infantile amnesia
not to be a problem with understanding the origins of knowledge per se, but a problem
for children in understanding how their re-experiences of an event related to the origi-
nal event. In other words, children under age 4 years do not have the phenomenol-
ogical capacity for recollection. Thus any ‘memory’ they display for the event might
in fact be reported from a conversation or be a simple association, not a true memory
of the event as directly experienced. As in Howe and Courage’s (1993) theory, the
critical framework missing is the capacity for encoding and later retrieving an event
as personally experienced; however, the ability that enables this framework is not
present until much later in Perner’s theory than in Howe and Courage’s theory.

(ii) Multiple influence theories. Another group of theorists has also focused on pre-
sumed cognitive or social-cognitive deficits in the early preschool years that do not
allow autobiographical memories to be encoded and retrieved, but have located these
critical abilities in the larger social and linguistic realm for the child. Pillemer and
White (1989), for instance, noted that children younger than about age 4 are not able
to understand fully the causal-temporal sequence of events that would enable fluent
retrieval from an adult’s perspective. For example, young preschoolers do not under-
stand how steps of an event relate causally to each other, or in general how events are
temporally ordered in relation to each other. Fivush (1988) proposed that young
preschoolers do not yet have a sense of self-in-time; they are unable to order self-
relevant events into a chronological and evaluative life story. In addition, Welch-Ross
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
Autobiographical Memory Development 127
(1995) emphasized the initial importance for autobiographical memory of an implicit
understanding of the link between experience and knowing from about age 3 years
(e.g., seeing the contents of a box leads to knowledge of the contents in the box).
Nelson (1996) argued that a necessary cognitive development for autobiographical
memory is children’s ability to use language as a representational system at about age
4 years. These explanations have some commonalties with Povinelli’s (1995) notion
of an enduring self and Perner’s (in press, a) ideas about connections between past
events and re-experiencing events. The multiple influence theorists propose some
social, cognitive, or social-cognitive framework contributes to autobiographical
memory (e.g., Pillemer & White, 1989) or even enables autobiographical memory
(Welch-Ross, 1995).
All of the multiple influence theorists, however, point to the additional powerful
influence of social interaction in creating autobiographical memories, even to the point
of claiming that children learn these critical cognitive capacities in part through social
interaction with others and through reminiscing in particular (Welch-Ross, 1997).
Highly elaborative parents who provide a great deal of information about shared past
events and who evaluate the past with their children are providing a model for
children to encode and retain events in a richer manner themselves. Nelson (1993)
theorized that children learn the significance of past events, indeed the function of
autobiographical memory, through talking about the past with significant others. Once
these social and cognitive contributions have come together, children are capable of
beginning to retain events autobiographically and to create a life story. Critically, these
accomplishments take place through the medium of language in the context of social
interaction (Fivush, 1998; Welch-Ross, in press). One main function of these interac-
tions is to help children place events in an evaluative perspective: what do they mean
for the child’s sense of self and for their interactions with others (Fivush, in press)?
Children are not just learning what to remember or how to remember, but why they
should remember.
The multiple influence theorists have also begun to take the larger social and emo-
tional context for the child, in the form of the attachment relationship with the primary
caregiver, into account (Farrar, Fasig, & Welch-Ross, 1997; Fivush & Reese, in press;
Nelson, 1999; Pillemer, 1998). If one function of autobiographical memory is to main-
tain social and emotional bonds, then mothers’ styles of reminiscing and children’s
participation in reminiscing should be linked with the mother-child attachment
relationship. Mothers with securely attached children may be more elaborative and
evaluative when reminiscing with their children. Securely attached children may in
turn be more responsive than insecurely attached children in conversations about past
events they shared with mothers. Ultimately, securely attached children may develop
a richer and more evaluative personal memory system.
This review now turns to a consideration of these theories in light of recent evi-
dence. I will necessarily have to constrain my review to those studies that tested the
role of children’s independent social, cognitive, or linguistic capacities for autobio-
graphical memory.

Role of Self Understanding


Only one published study to date has addressed Howe and Courage’s (1993, 1997)
hypotheses about the importance of visual self-recognition ability for autobiographi-
cal memory. Harley and Reese (1999) measured children’s self-recognition with the
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
128 Elaine Reese
mirror test at age 1-1/2 years as a predictor of children’s memory reports with mothers
and experimenters at ages 2 and 2-1/2 years. Children’s independent language, non-
verbal memory ability and maternal reminiscing style were also assessed. Children’s
memory reports with mothers and experimenters did vary as a function of their earlier
self-recognition skill, with early recognizers reporting more with mothers and with
experimenters over time. Further analyses with this sample, however, showed that the
initial importance of self-recognition was later mediated by maternal reminiscing style
when children were 2 years old (Reese, in press). Children who recognized themselves
earlier in the mirror test had mothers who responded with increased elaborations in
past event conversations with children at age 2 years, and maternal reminiscing style
more directly predicted children’s later memory reports. Mothers may notice at some
level children’s increased cognitive abilities and respond by increasing their level of
elaboration. Thus, there is a link between children’s self-recognition skill and their
autobiographical memory, but it appears to be an indirect one that is mediated by social
factors. In addition, self-recognition was only one of several important predictors of
autobiographical memory in this study, lending credence to a multiple influence
perspective.
Other research has focused on the role of an extended self-in-time as the primary
contributor to autobiographical memory. Povinelli et al. (1996) operationalized chil-
dren’s extended self with the delayed self-recognition measure (DSR) outlined previ-
ously. In their first series of experiments using this procedure, Povinelli et al. (1996)
found that no 2-year-olds reached up to retrieve the sticker upon seeing the video after
a brief delay, 25% of 3-year-olds reached for the sticker, and 75% of 4-year-olds
did so. Essentially the same results were obtained when children viewed a photograph
of the marking event after a brief delay, with 13% of young 3-year-olds, 60% of older
3-year-olds, and 80% of young 4-year-olds reaching up to retrieve the sticker. Povinelli
and Simon (1998) further demonstrated that 4- and 5-year-old children differentiated
between brief and extended delays for the marking procedure. These older children
reached up for the sticker after a brief delay of several minutes, but when shown
a video of themselves being marked from the previous week, they did not reach up
for the sticker. Three-year-olds, on the other hand, did not differentiate between brief
and extended delays for marking, reaching for the sticker at equally low rates in
both conditions. Povinelli and Simon concluded that 4- and 5-year-old children
have a concept of the self as enduring in time; thus, they understand that transient
aspects of self, such as a sticker on one’s head, endure for a short but not a long period.
Younger three-year-olds, on the other hand, are unable to connect the past with the
present self after even a very brief delay, although these same children are able to
detect the marked self in a live condition (Povinelli et al., 1996; Experiment 3).
According to Povinelli (1995), only when children have achieved a proper self that
is extended in time can they be considered capable of having autobiographical
memory, because only then can they connect events that happened to a past self with
the present self.
Povinelli and colleagues have demonstrated convincingly the developmental phe-
nomenon by which younger preschoolers are not able to link past and present events.
They have not yet, however, linked this developmental accomplishment with inde-
pendent measures of children’s recall of past events. Skene and Moore (in press) have
taken steps in this direction by showing a relation between children’s DSR perfor-
mance and their episodic memory. Skene and Moore’s episodic memory measure
consisted of children’s immediate recall of a sticker game. Recalling the stickers’ order
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
Autobiographical Memory Development 129
and the context (i.e., what color cup was covering the sticker) were posited to tap into
episodic rather than semantic memory because children had to rely on recollection.
Delayed self recognition was positively correlated with both aspects of episodic
memory for 4-year-olds, even with children’s general memory for the type of stickers
partialled out. As Skene and Moore admit, however, their memory task may not be
autobiographical in the sense that it is not likely to comprise a specific past event
of personal relevance for the participant; nor does it comprise long-term or verbal
recall.
Welch-Ross (in press) reported the first evidence to support a relation between DSR
performance and children’s verbal recall. Children’s memory reporting performance
varied as a function of DSR, with 3-year-olds who reached for the sticker recalling
proportionally more about significant past events with their mothers than children who
did not reach for the sticker. The effect of delayed self understanding interacted with
mothers’ reminiscing style, such that only among children of highly elaborative
mothers did a difference exist between reachers and non-reachers in their verbal
memory scores. Welch-Ross interpreted the DSR task as demonstrating children’s
understanding of causal connections between the past and present, not necessarily indi-
cating an extended self-in-time. Further work by Povinelli, Landry, Theall, Clark, and
Castille (1999) supported the contention that 3-year-old children’s difficulty with the
delayed self task is not specific to self-recognition, but is part of a general difficulty
with causally connecting past and present events. Similar to the results with live self-
recognition (e.g., Reese, in press), the role of delayed self or causal understanding per
se appears to interact with social factors, such as the amount of elaborative conversa-
tion the child receives about past events from the mother. This pattern of findings is
in line with Pillemer and White’s (1989) and Fivush’s (1988) theories that the effects
of these cognitive advances are mediated by the way adults introduce children to the
function of autobiographical memory.
It is also possible that the results to date have revealed only a limited role for self
understanding in autobiographical memory because the measures of self understand-
ing are only partially capturing the ability. Even the DSR procedure does not fully
capture the sense of an extended self-in-time as conceptualized by Povinelli (1995),
Fivush (1988; in press), and Nelson (in press, a); instead, it appears to measure chil-
dren’s causal understanding more generally. Pillemer, Picariello and Pruett (1994) have
also demonstrated the role of children’s causal-temporal understanding in their auto-
biographical memory. Children who were 3-1/2 years old at the time of a significant
event (a fire alarm at their preschool) were not able to construct a coherent causal
chain of events two weeks after the experience. For instance, many of these children
stated that they were already outside the building when the fire alarm went off. In con-
trast, almost all of the children who were 4-1/2 years old at the time of the same event
said that they were inside the preschool when the alarm went off. Only one 3-1/2 year
old, but nearly half of the 4-1/2 year olds, spontaneously mentioned the cause of the
fire alarm. Notably, 57% of the older children displayed some memory for the event
7 years later, compared to only 18% of the children who were 3-1/2 at the time of the
event. Thus, a marked change in children’s understanding of causal and temporal
sequencing of events at around age 4 years may be contributing to an increase in chil-
dren’s autobiographical memory.
In sum, the DSR task may be tapping into a larger change in children’s causal-
temporal understanding that occurs at about age 4 years. This development would
affect children’s ability to construct a self-in-time, but it does not necessarily involve
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
130 Elaine Reese
an evaluation of the significance of a past event with respect to a current concept of
self. Even less research has addressed the link between autobiographical memory and
evaluative components of self. Welch-Ross, Fasig, and Farrar (1999) found a concur-
rent relation between 4-year-olds’ self understanding, as measured by the Children’s
Self—View Questionnaire (Eder, 1990), and their autobiographical memory. Children
with a more consistent, organized sense of themselves had more evaluative conversa-
tions with their mothers about past events. These results could mean that children need
a coherent sense of self before they can contribute to and benefit from evaluative con-
versations of the past. There was some indication in these data that only children with
an organized self view were able to benefit from an elaborative maternal style. The
results may also mean that mothers’ evaluative perspective for past events contributes
to children’s organized self-concept, or that mothers respond to their perception of
children’s organized self-concept by discussing emotions more freely in past event
talk. Longitudinal studies need to be conducted to determine the direction of this
relationship.
Self understanding does relate to autobiographical memory even with the current
measures, but often only indirectly. These results point to the conclusion that self
understanding cannot be considered a solely endogenous characteristic of the child as
implied in Howe and Courage (1993) and Povinelli (1995). Instead, social factors, such
as parental reminiscing style, interact with children’s self understanding to produce
autobiographical memory. There are probably several reasons for this interaction. First,
parents’ talk about personally relevant past events may be an important source of infor-
mation for children’s self understanding. Second, children may not be able to benefit
from increased self understanding in their retention of autobiographical memories
unless they have the added elaborative and evaluative support from their parents in
understanding past events. These conclusions are necessarily speculative due to the
limited amount of research conducted in this area so far. Few extensions have been
made to children’s independent autobiographical memory, and in only one sample was
the role of self understanding studied longitudinally (Harley & Reese, 1999; Reese,
in press).

Role of Understanding of Mind


A related body of research has addressed the role of children’s understanding of mind
in their ability to encode and display autobiographical memories. Children’s under-
standing of their own and others’ mental states (theory of mind) is hypothesized to be
important in at least two ways for autobiographical memory (Welch-Ross, 1997). First,
children’s understanding of the origins of knowledge may contribute to their autobio-
graphical memory skills. Specifically, children begin to understand that visual access
equates to knowledge at around 3 years of age (Pillow, 1989). Around age 4 years,
children start to understand more specifically what someone might know depending
on what they saw (Taylor, 1988). Children also become capable of connecting what
they know to the sense modality of the original experience (e.g., either they know
because they saw an event or because they were told of an event; Gopnik & Graf,
1988). These origins of knowledge skills are predicted to be related to children’s auto-
biographical memory in that they aid the child in distinguishing between their own
knowledge and others’ knowledge, how they obtained that knowledge, and between
personally experienced events and events about which they have been told (Perner &
Ruffman, 1995).
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
Autobiographical Memory Development 131
A second aspect of theory of mind that is predicted to be related to children’s
autobiographical memory is the ability to consider two conflicting representations
simultaneously (Perner, in press b; Welch-Ross, 1997). This skill is typically measured
with a false belief (Gopnik & Astington, 1988; Wimmer & Perner, 1983) or appear-
ance-reality task (Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1986). In false belief tasks, children are
required to predict what another will do based on a false belief, or they are required
to remember their own previous false belief. In appearance-reality tasks, they must
resolve the discrepancy between what something appears to be and what it really
is. These cognitive skills may be related to autobiographical memory by aiding
children’s understanding of the subjective nature of memory and of the potential con-
flict between their own representations of an event and another person’s representa-
tions of an event.
Perner and Ruffman (1995) focused on the way that autobiographical memory
depends on children’s understanding of the origins of knowledge. Children aged 3-1/2
to 5-1/2 years of age participated in a range of tasks assessing how they know what
they know in addition to a memory measure. The memory measure in this study was
children’s free and cued recall of a set of 12 pictures of familiar items. Free recall was
posited as a measure of episodic memory and was thus expected to be uniquely cor-
related with children’s origins of knowledge performance. Perner and Ruffman’s argu-
ment is that only when children are capable of understanding informational access can
they be aware of having experienced an event and therefore possess an ‘episodic trace’
of the event. Correlations between children’s free recall and their origins of knowl-
edge performance were significant even after their cued recall performance and recep-
tive vocabulary had been partialled out. Perner and Ruffman interpreted these findings
as strong support for the idea that episodic memory (or recollective memory) is de-
pendent upon the development of experiential knowing. In an intriguing new study,
Perner and Gornik (1999; cited in Perner, in press a) tested 3-1/2- to 6-year-old chil-
dren’s memory for directly experienced and indirectly experienced events. In a direct-
experience condition, children saw pictures on the fronts of cards as they placed them
in a box. In an indirect-experience condition, children looked at the back of each
card as they put it into a box, but later saw the pictures on the front of the cards in a
video. Children over 5 years of age showed better memory for directly than indirectly
experienced events. Four-year-olds showed equivalent recall of direct and indirect
experiences. Moreover, children’s understanding of which sense modality leads to
knowledge (e.g., one should use sight to figure out the color of an object) was the best
independent predictor of the difference between their memory for directly vs. indi-
rectly experienced events. Perner’s (in press, a) interpretation of these results is that
children who are unable to connect their knowledge with the sense modality in which
it was gained are unable to make use of a feeling of recollection in aiding their episodic
memory.
Strengths of Perner and colleagues’ studies include the variety of tests used to
measure children’s informational access and the inclusion of an independent language
measure (Perner and Ruffman, 1995). Limitations of these studies include their con-
current design. Could children’s episodic memory performance instead be aiding their
informational access? An additional limitation is the sole use of picture recall as a
measure of episodic memory. Perner and Ruffman (1995) acknowledged that we can
not be sure when children start to experience a sense of recollection during free recall.
An additional test of the hypothesis would measure relations between children’s under-
standing of mind and their verbal memory for naturally occurring events.
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
132 Elaine Reese
Welch-Ross (1997) tested concurrent relations between 3-1/2 to 4-1/2 year old chil-
dren’s theory of mind and their verbal reports of past events with mothers. Two aspects
of children’s theory of mind were assessed: their origins of knowledge and conflict-
ing representation skills. These two skills were both predicted to enhance children’s
autobiographical memory, but for different reasons. Like Perner and Ruffman (1995),
Welch-Ross (1997) hypothesized that children’s ability to link knowledge and experi-
ence would enable the recollective component of autobiographical memory. Children’s
skill with conflicting representations was instead expected to facilitate their coordi-
nation of their own event representation with their partners’. Due to their increased
ability to coordinate past and present representations of themselves, these same chil-
dren would also be expected to participate more in and benefit more from past event
conversations with adults. In sum, children with a better theory of mind were pre-
dicted to report more unique event information in response to mothers’ elaborative
questions compared to children with a less developed understanding of mind.
Children with a better theory of mind in this study indeed provided more unique
memory responses and fewer empty responses to mothers’ elaborative questions com-
pared to children with a poorer theory of mind, even with age and syntactic ability
partialled out. Strengths of this study are the inclusion of measures of conflicting
representation in addition to origins of knowledge, and an extension of the relation
between theory of mind and autobiographical memory to children’s verbal recall for
naturally occurring events. Limitations of this study are a concurrent research design
and a possibly inadequate measure of children’s language ability. Children’s syntactic
skill in this study was measured in the past event conversations in terms of the mean
length of children’s longest utterances (MLUL). Children’s MLUL may not be the best
indicator of their language skill by the preschool years. Indeed, children’s MLUL did
not correlate with the theory of mind measures in this study, in contrast to many
demonstrations of the strong relation between children’s language and their theory of
mind (e.g., Astington & Jenkins, 1999). The relation needs to be tested against an in-
dependent measure of children’s language skill to be sure that it isn’t due solely to the
correlation between theory of mind and language. We also still need to know the direc-
tionality of the relation between children’s theory of mind and their autobiographical
memory. Could enhanced autobiographical memory skills instead be contributing to
children’s understanding of mind?
The research conducted on this topic to date has undoubtedly revealed a link
between children’s understanding of mind and their episodic and autobiographical
memory. Yet several crucial details remain unknown. Perner (in press, a) proposed that
the critical aspect of children’s theory of mind for autobiographical memory is their
ability to link what they know to their original experience, whereas Welch-Ross (1997,
in press) additionally suggested that success with conflicting representations should
also contribute to autobiographical memory. Welch-Ross’s (1997) data support the
role of both aspects of theory of mind to autobiographical memory, but a replication
of this effect is in order, as well as further specification as to how the two aspects of
theory of mind might relate to autobiographical memory in different ways. Moreover,
we do not yet know how the link between theory of mind and autobiographical
memory might interact with other cognitive factors, such as children’s self under-
standing or causal-temporal understanding, or with social factors, such as the style of
reminiscing to which children are exposed. Researchers are beginning to acknowledge
that theory of mind is not a solely endogenous skill, but grows out of children’s social
interactions with others (e.g., Dunn, 1988; Perner, Ruffman, & Leekam, 1994;
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
Autobiographical Memory Development 133
Ruffman, Perner, & Parkin, 2000). Perhaps one especially important context for learn-
ing about theory of mind is when reminiscing with others about shared past events
(Welch-Ross, 1997; Welch-Ross, in press). Such conversations might bring up dis-
crepancies between event representations and promote negotiations between parent
and child about the objective and subjective details of events. Longitudinal studies are
needed to test the relation between social interaction and theory of mind, as well as
the relation between theory of mind and autobiographical memory. Finally, theory of
mind has not been demonstrated definitively as a prerequisite to autobiographical
memory. For instance, in Welch-Ross (1997), children with lower and higher theory
of mind skills both provided unique memory information with their mothers, but chil-
dren with higher theory of mind skills did so to a greater degree. To demonstrate that
theory of mind is a prerequisite to autobiographical memory, researchers would need
to show a qualitative shift in children’s memory capacities concurrent with or soon
after children start passing relevant theory of mind tasks. It’s hard to know what such
a qualitative shift would look like, but possible candidates are the presence of
unprompted past event discussions contingent upon theory of mind performance, or
a statistical discontinuity in the quality or quantity of children’s memory responding
before and after passing theory of mind tasks.

Role of Social and Linguistic Factors


The role of social interaction in autobiographical memory, and in particular parents’
style of talking about the past with children, is now well-established (Fivush &
Fromhoff, 1988; Hudson, 1990; McCabe & Peterson, 1991; Reese & Fivush, 1993;
Reese, Haden, & Fivush, 1993; for a review see Reese & Farrant, in press). Parents
who use a highly elaborative style of talking about the past with their children, in
which they ask many questions containing new information and confirm children’s
responses profusely, have children who later display a more elaborative style of rem-
iniscing both with parents and independently (Farrant & Reese, 2000; Hudson, 1993).
Other parents use a less elaborative style of talking about the past that consists of
repeating the same question until children produce a specific answer. This style of
reminiscing is associated with children providing sparser memory reports later with
parents and with experimenters. In the first experimental investigation of maternal
reminiscing style, Peterson, Jesso and McCabe (1999) trained mothers to talk about
past events in an elaborative way with their 3-1/2 year old children. One year later,
children from the training group showed higher vocabulary scores compared to chil-
dren from a control group, and by age 5-1/2 years, children from the training group
gave more complex past event narratives with an experimenter than children in a
control group.
Recent research in this area has also focused on the contextual and child charac-
teristics that might shape parents’ naturally occurring style of reminiscing. Haden,
Haine and Fivush (1997) demonstrated that mothers’ provision of evaluations in past
event narratives (intensifiers, emotional and mental state terms) with children at 40
months predicted children’s independent past event narratives at 70 months, even after
controlling for children’s earlier narrative skill. Fathers’ early provision of narrative
structure did not predict their children’s later independent narrative skill. Primary
caregiver mothers most likely play a greater role than secondary caregiver fathers in
facilitating children’s reminiscing, partially as a function of sharing more experiences,
but also possibly as a function of their greater frequency of reminiscing. Mothers’
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
134 Elaine Reese
reminiscing style is consistent across time (Reese et al., 1993), across shared and
unshared past events (Reese & Brown, 2000), and across children in the same family
(Haden, 1998). Maternal reminiscing style, however, does not generalize to other con-
versational contexts such as free play (Haden & Fivush, 1996). Therefore, maternal
reminiscing style is a stable characteristic of the mother but may be specific to a
memory context.
Mothers’ reminiscing style is responsive, however, to child characteristics. The bulk
of this research has been conducted on the role of children’s gender in maternal rem-
iniscing style. Reese and Fivush (1993) and Reese, Haden, and Fivush (1996) found
that parents were more elaborative and evaluative with daughters than with sons when
discussing past events. In turn, girls produced more memory information in the inter-
views than boys from as early as 3-1/2 years of age. These results are intriguing given
that women date childhood memories back to younger ages than men (e.g., Mullen,
1994). These effects of child gender appear to be most robust when parents are
discussing emotional events. Parents, especially mothers, are more elaborative with
daughters than sons about emotional events, particularly regarding sadness (e.g.,
Adams, Kuebli, Boyle, & Fivush, 1995; Fivush, Brotman, Buckner, & Goodman,
2000).
Children’s skill levels also shape maternal reminiscing style. Most mothers become
more elaborative as their children get older and are providing more information (Reese
et al., 1993). As mentioned previously, mothers also become more elaborative in
response to children’s increased self-recognition skills (Reese, in press) and in
response to children’s theory of mind skills (Welch-Ross, 1997). Moreover, mothers
are more elaborative with children who have more advanced expressive language skills
(Farrant & Reese, 2000). Nevertheless, maternal reminiscing style cannot be reduced
to a simple ‘child-effects’ explanation. Two lines of evidence support this contention.
First, maternal reminiscing style still predicted children’s verbal memory even when
controlling for child characteristics such as their self-recognition skill, language skill,
attachment security, and interest in participating in memory conversations (Reese, in
press). Second, the experimental manipulation of maternal reminiscing style indicated
a causal role in children’s later past event narratives, regardless of children’s gender
or initial skill levels (Peterson et al., 1999).
If indeed maternal reminiscing style is casually related to children’s autobiographi-
cal memory, what then is the mechanism of this effect? Recent research on the effect
of maternal style at the time of encoding helps to clarify these effects. Tessler and
Nelson (1994) found that 3-year-old children only mentioned items in a subsequent
memory report that had been jointly discussed by mother and child during the event.
Similarly, Haden, Ornstein, Eckerman and Didow (in press) demonstrated that objects
that were jointly handled and discussed by both mother and child were more likely
to be mentioned in a child’s later memory report than objects discussed only by
the mother. Fivush (1998) speculated that the elaborative style may also work in a
more general way to enhance children’s later memory reports. The rich narrative struc-
ture that elaborative mothers provide for children helps them organize their memo-
ries, both at the time of encoding and retrieval. Moreover, an elaborative style is
probably most effective when the mother is following in on the child’s attentional focus
in the event or conversation. In addition, the evaluative perspective that elaborative
mothers provide may help children understand the significance of the event for the life
story.

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002


Autobiographical Memory Development 135

Role of Attachment
With the recent emphasis on the evaluative framework that mothers can potentially
provide in these conversations, research has also focused on the early attachment
relationship between mother and child as a potential contributor to both maternal
reminiscing style and children’s autobiographical memory. Attachment security is
established in the first year of the child’s life (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978;
Bowlby, 1969). Securely attached dyads interact with greater harmony, nonverbally
and verbally, than insecurely attached dyads (e.g., Matas, Arend, & Sroufe, 1978;
Meins, 1997). By the preschool years, securely attached children and their mothers
should be able to communicate more openly about a variety of topics because of the
child’s trust in the mother’s emotional and physical availability (Bowlby, 1988;
Bretherton & Munholland, 1999; Etzion-Carasso & Oppenheim, 2000). Indeed, one
method of assessing children’s attachment security is to examine their narratives about
hypothetical attachment-relevant events (e.g., Bretherton, Ridgeway, & Cassidy, 1990;
Waters, Rodrigues, & Ridgeway, 1998). The focus of the attachment and reminiscing
research is somewhat different. Rather than viewing reminiscing as a narrative window
to attachment security (Koren-Karie, Oppenheim, Haimovich, & Etzion-Carasso, in
press), this research has instead examined reminiscing as a separate social-emotional
interaction that arises in part from the attachment relationship but may also contribute
to attachment security. For instance, the maternal sensitivity that leads to children’s
attachment security may also lead to an elaborative maternal reminiscing style. In turn,
these elaborative conversations about significant events shared with the primary
attachment figure may contribute to the child’s internal working model of the attach-
ment relationship, and ultimately to the child’s internal working model of self (Reese
& Farrant, in press). For these reasons, mothers of securely attached children are pre-
dicted to display a highly elaborative style, which is more sensitive in response to the
child’s agenda in past event conversations (Fivush & Reese, in press; Nelson, 1999;
Pillemer, 1998; Reese & Farrant, in press). In turn, securely attached children are pre-
dicted to give richer memory reports later on.
Farrar, Fasig, and Welch-Ross (1997) looked specifically at the emotional content
of past event narratives between mothers and 4-year-old children as a function of chil-
dren’s attachment status. Mothers and insecurely attached daughters initiated talk
about negative emotions more often than mothers and securely attached daughters.
Mothers and securely attached daughters talked in more depth about both positive and
negative emotions, once initiated, than mothers and insecurely attached daughters. This
finding supports the notion that mothers adopt a more elaborative and evaluative rem-
iniscing style with securely attached children, at least for girls. Fivush and Vasudeva
(in press) expanded on these findings by addressing the link between 4-year-old chil-
dren’s attachment security and mothers’ elaborative reminiscing style for primarily
positive events. Similar to Farrar et al., mothers were more elaborative with securely
attached children, although the relation held for both boys and girls. Farrant and Reese
(2001) sought to examine the developing relation between mother-child reminiscing
and children’s attachment security from when children were 1-1/2 years old. Mothers
of securely attached children were more elaborative and less repetitive with their chil-
dren, especially in response to children giving an empty response, such as ‘I don’t
know.’ Securely attached children were somewhat more likely to offer information in
the conversations. Most importantly, mothers and securely attached children showed

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002


136 Elaine Reese
long-term links between their reminiscing styles over time, whereas mothers and inse-
curely attached children showed no long-term relations with each other’s reminiscing
styles. These findings suggest that memory socialization is taking place to a greater
extent in dyads with securely attached children (see Reese & Farrant, in press, for
further discussion).
Attachment security does appear to play a role in maternal reminiscing style and
in children’s autobiographical memory, but a great deal more research needs to be
conducted in this area. Most studies to date have used maternal sorts of the Attach-
ment Q-set to measure attachment security (Waters, 1987/1995) as the attachment
measure. In one notable exception, Etzion-Carasso and Oppenheim (2000) did find a
link between children’s attachment security as measured in the Strange Situation
(Ainsworth et al., 1978) and their reunion conversations with mothers at age 4-1/2
years. Focusing specifically on reminiscing style for attachment-related narratives is
another possible direction (see Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985; Oppenheim & Waters,
1995). The research so far suggests that children’s attachment security is related to
mother-child reminiscing about both positive and negative events, but the link may be
particularly strong for reminiscing about separation events. Further specification in
theory and method is also in order to ascertain when maternal elaborations are a sen-
sitive strategy. Koren-Karie et al. (in press) point out that maternal elaboration on a
negative event is not necessarily a sensitive strategy if mothers are not also helping
children to resolve the negative emotion, nor can elaboration that focuses on the
mother’s rather than the child’s subjective states be considered sensitive. It is also desir-
able to further specify the mechanism by which attachment security relates to mater-
nal reminiscing style. One possibility is that adult attachment orientation leads to a
more elaborative reminiscing style for mothers (Fivush & Reese, in press; Pillemer,
1998). Mothers who are classified as secure/autonomous on the Adult Attachment
Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996) are predicted to have a more open and
elaborative style when reminiscing with their children about past events, resulting in
intergenerational transmission of both reminiscing style and attachment security
(Reese & Farrant, in press).

Interactions Between Social, Cognitive, and Linguistic Factors


With such a host of potential contributors to autobiographical memory, a critical task
for researchers is to attempt to identify the independent and interacting predictors of
autobiographical memory skill. Some studies have controlled for the role of language
development, but only one study to date has examined the simultaneous contribution
of social, cognitive, and linguistic factors to autobiographical memory. Reese (in
press) tested the independent contributions during infancy of self-recognition in the
mirror task, productive language skill, attachment security, initial interest in partici-
pating in memory conversations, and maternal reminiscing to children’s later autobi-
ographical memory skill. All of these variables except for self-recognition contributed
independently to children’s memory reports with their mothers by age 2- to 2-1/2 years.
Self-recognition was also related to children’s later memory reports, but only indi-
rectly through a link with maternal reminiscing style. Children who had high language
skills, were securely attached, showed initial interest in participating in memory con-
versations, and had highly elaborative mothers reported the greatest amount of
memory information with mothers at age 2- to 2-1/2 years. Children’s memory reports
with their mothers at age 2-1/2 years then generalized to their independent memory
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
Autobiographical Memory Development 137
reports with experimenters at age 3-1/2 years. Thus, social, linguistic, and to a lesser
extent cognitive factors played a role in initiating children into the autobiographical
memory system, which then progressed in a fairly continuous fashion to age 3-1/2
years. This longitudinal sample is currently being followed past the 3-1/2 year age
mark to determine the additional role of children’s theory of mind and extended self
development to autobiographical memory. It is possible that these cognitive develop-
ments will result in qualitative changes to children’s verbal memory reports, but given
the results of the concurrent studies conducted thus far, these effects will most likely
interact with maternal reminiscing style.

Conclusions
We know a great deal more now about the conditions affecting children’s autobio-
graphical memory than we did 10 or even 5 years ago, when the burst in theorizing
about infantile amnesia occurred. Still, the data are far behind the theories. A picture
of autobiographical memory that is influenced by children’s social interactions, their
language skill, and to a lesser extent their cognitive capabilities is beginning to form,
but the influence of these factors in relation to each other over time is still largely
unknown.
There may be enough data at present, however, to warrant a preliminary revision of
the initial theories of the process. In particular, we must take a more global view of
the entire autobiographical memory system, instead of proposing that one or another
development is necessary or primary (see Reese, in press). This new theory of auto-
biographical memory has much in common with a dynamic systems approach to
development (see Thelen & Smith, 1998 for a review; see Howe & Rabinowitz, 1994
and Nelson, in press b for applications to cognitive development). With a dynamic
systems approach, the task is to characterize the process of development in an attempt
to discover how change in the system as a whole is taking place as a result of at times
minute changes at a particular age or level. Change may be relatively continuous
during certain periods of development, but then a small disruption to one aspect of
the system may result in a more dramatic qualitative shift. A dynamic systems
approach would enable us to integrate biological, social, linguistic, and cognitive influ-
ences on autobiographical memory and would encompass different levels of explana-
tion. In the process, a dynamic systems approach would help us to escape from the
tired old dichotomies of whether change is due to endogenous or exogenous factors,
or whether the nature of change is essentially continuous or discontinuous. For
example, the relatively continuous changes in autobiographical memory wrought by
children’s self understanding, expressive language, attachment security, and maternal
reminiscing style may come together in early childhood to result in a qualitative shift
in autobiographical memory development past age 3-1/2 years, especially as children
develop theory of mind and extended self understanding.
Thelen and Smith (1994; 1998, pp. 601–602) have outlined the steps to take in a
dynamic systems approach to conducting research. The first step is to define the ‘col-
lective variable of interest’: in the case of children’s autobiographical memory, their
memory reports with various conversational partners. Step 2 in the process is to isolate
the ‘preferred state’ of this outcome variable at various ages and under various con-
ditions. We know quite a bit about the general course of verbal memory development,
but we could still address more attention to the question of what qualitative changes
in verbal memory development would look like. Would we know qualitative change
© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002
138 Elaine Reese
if we saw it, or are we only viewing it in retrospect, from the point of view of adults
recalling their earliest memories? Step 3 is to conduct intensive longitudinal studies
of the variable of interest, especially over periods of rapid transition. Only a few such
longitudinal studies have been conducted to date (e.g., McCabe & Peterson, 1991;
Farrant & Reese, 2000; Haden et al., in press; Reese, Haden, & Fivush, 1993), but no
single study has spanned the gap from 1-1/2 to 5 years of age. This gap must be filled
if we are to map out periods of continuity and discontinuity in autobiographical
memory in the same children over time and identify ‘transition points,’ which con-
stitutes Step 4. Step 5 is to then discover ‘potential control parameters’ affecting
autobiographical memory. This review has focused on various social, cognitive, and
linguistic factors affecting children’s verbal memory reports. Step 6, finally, is to test
our hypotheses about these parameters experimentally through the use of training and
enrichment studies. Researchers have made some progress on all of these fronts, but
in my view we should know quite a bit more about the process of change and the con-
ditions for change in autobiographical memory before we attempt large-scale experi-
mental studies. In other words, there’s still a long(itudinal) way to go. The sometimes
herculean effort involved in such intensive longitudinal studies would be worth it,
however, to gain a fuller picture of this complex and intriguing skill.

References
Adams, S., Kuebli, J., Boyle, P., & Fivush, R. (1995). Gender differences in parent-child con-
versations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles, 33, 309–323.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Astington, J. W. & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language
and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.
Barr, R. & Hayne, H. (2000). Age-related changes in imitation: Implications for memory devel-
opment. In C. Rovee-Collier, L. P. Lipsitt, & H. Hayne (Eds.), Progress in Infancy Research,
Vol. 1 (pp. 21–68). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. London:
Routledge.
Bretherton, I. & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working models in attachment relation-
ships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment:
Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 89–111). NY: The Guilford Press.
Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D., & Cassidy, J. (1990). Assessing internal working models of the
attachment relationship: An attachment story completion task for 3-year-olds. In M. Green-
berg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
research and intervention (pp. 273–308). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Ceci, S. J. & Bruck, M. (1993). Suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and
synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 403–439.
Conway, M. A. & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memo-
ries in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.
Dudycha, G. J. & Dudycha, M. M. (1941). Childhood memories: A review of the literature.
Psychological Bulletin, 38, 668–682.
Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell.
Eder, R. (1990). Uncovering young children’s psychological selves: Individual and develop-
mental differences. Child Development, 61, 849–863.
Etzion-Carasso, A. & Oppenheim, D. (2000). Open mother-preschooler communication: Rela-
tions with early secure attachment. Attachment and Human Development, 347–370.
Farrant, K. & Reese, E. (2000). Maternal reminiscing style and dyadic quality: Stepping stones
in children’s autobiographical memory development. Journal of Cognition and Development,
1, 193–225.

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002


Autobiographical Memory Development 139
Farrant, K. & Reese, E. (2001). Attachment security and early mother-child reminiscing: A
developmental exploration. Unpublished manuscript.
Farrar, J. M., Fasig, L. G., & Welch-Ross, M. K. (1997). Attachment and emotion in autobio-
graphical memory development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 389–408.
Fivush, R. (1988). The functions of event memory: Some comments on Nelson and Barsalou.
In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional
approaches to the study of memory (pp. 277–282). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fivush, R. (1998). The stories we tell: How language shapes autobiography. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 12, 483–487.
Fivush, R. (in press). Owning experience: Developing subjective perspective in autobio-
graphical narratives. In C. Moore & K. Skene (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental issues.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Fivush, R., Brotman, M., Buckner, J. P., & Goodman, S. (2000). Gender differences in parent-
child emotion narratives. Sex Roles, 42, 233–254.
Fivush, R. & Fromhoff, F. A. (1988). Style and structure in mother-child conversations about
the past. Discourse Processes, 11, 337–355.
Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Adam, S. (1995). Structure and coherence of preschoolers’ per-
sonal narratives over time: Implications for childhood amnesia. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 60, 32–56.
Fivush, R. & Reese, E. (in press). Reminiscing and relating: The development of parent-child
talk about the past. In J. Webster & B. Haight (Eds.), Critical advances in reminiscence work.
NY: Springer.
Fivush, R. & Vasudeva, A. (in press). Remembering to relate: Socioemotional correlates of
mother-child reminiscing. Journal of Cognition and Development.
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the
appearance-reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
ment, 51, Serial No. 212.
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult Attachment Interview Protocol (3rd ed.).
Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley.
Gopnik, A. & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and
its relation to the understanding of the false-belief and the appearance-reality distinction.
Child Development, 58, 26–37.
Gopnik, A. & Graf, P. (1988). Knowing how you know: Young children’s ability to identify and
remember the sources of their beliefs. Child Development, 59, 1366–1371.
Haden, C. A. (1998). Reminiscing with different children: Relating maternal stylistic consis-
tency and sibling similarity in talk about the past. Developmental Psychology, 34, 99–114.
Haden, C. A. & Fivush, R. (1996). Contextual variation in maternal conversational styles.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 200–227.
Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative structure in parent-child
reminiscing across the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 33, 295–307.
Haden, C. A., Ornstein, P. A., Eckerman, C. O., & Didow, S. M. (in press). Mother-child
conversational interactions as events unfold: Linkages to subsequent remembering. Child
Development.
Harley, K. & Reese, E. (1999). Origins of autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 35, 1338–1348.
Howe, M. L. (1997). Children’s memory for traumatic experiences. Learning and Individual
Differences, 12, 917–927.
Howe, M. L. & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of infantile amnesia. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 113, 305–326.
Howe, M. L. & Courage, M. L. (1997). The emergence and early development of autobio-
graphical memory. Psychological Review, 104, 499–523.
Howe, M. L. & Rabinowitz, M. (1994). Dynamic modeling, chaos, and cognitive development.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 184–199.
Hudson, J. A. (1990). The emergence of autobiographical memory in mother-child conversa-
tion. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp.
166–196). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hudson, J. A. (1993). Reminiscing with mothers and others: Autobiographical memory in
young two-year-olds. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 3, 1–32.

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002


140 Elaine Reese
Hyman, I. E. & Faries, J. M. (1992). The functions of autobiographical memory. In M. A.
Conway, D. C. Rubin, H. Spinnler, & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.), Theoretical perspectives on
autobiographical memory (pp. 207–221). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., Haimovich, Z., & Etzion-Carasso, A. (in press). Dialogues
of seven-year-olds with their mothers about emotional events; Development of a typology.
In R. N. Emde, D. P. Wolf, & D. Oppenheim, Affective meaning making in narratives: Studies
with young children.
Lewis, M. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of the self. New
York: Plenum Press.
Linde, C. (1993). Life stories: The creation of coherence. NY: Oxford University Press.
Main, M., Kaplan, K., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood: A
move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of
attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society fo rResearch in Child Develop-
ment, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209), 66–104.
Matas, L., Arend, R. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year:
The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Devleopment,
49, 547–556.
McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the story: A longitudinal study of parental styles
in eliciting narratives and developing narrative skill. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.),
Developing narrative structure (pp. 217–253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Meins, E. (1997). Security of attachment and the social development of cognition. Hove: Psy-
chology Press Ltd.
Mullen, M. K. (1994). Earliest recollections of childhood: A demographic analysis. Cognition,
52, 55–79.
Nelson, C. A. (1995). The ontogeny of human memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective.
Developmental Psychology, 31, 723–738.
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psycho-
logical Science, 4, 1–8.
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind.
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Nelson, K. (1999). Event representations, narrative development and internal working models.
Attachment and Human Development, 1, 239–252.
Nelson, K. (in press, a). Language and the self: From the experiencing I to the continuing me.
In C. Moore & K. Skene (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental issues. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nelson, K. (in press, b). Narrative and the emergence of a consciousness of self. In T. McVay,
O. Flanagan, & G. Fireman (Eds.), Narrative and Consciousness: Literature, Psychology,
and the Brain. NY: Oxford University Press.
Oppenheim, D. & Waters, E. (1995). Narrative processes and attachment representations: Issues
of development and assessment. In E. Waters, B. E. Vaughn, G. Posada, & K. Kondo-Ikemura
(Eds.), Caregiving, cultural, and cognitive perspectives on secure-base behavior and working
models: New growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of Society for
Research in Child Development, Vol. 60, 197–215.
Perner, J. (in press, a). Episodic memory: Essential distinctions and developmental implica-
tions. In C. Moore & K. Skene (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental issues. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Perner, J. (in press, b). Memory and theory of mind. In E. Tulving and F. I. M. Craik (Eds.),
The Oxford Handbook of Memory. New York, NY: University of Oxford Press.
Perner, J. & Ruffman, T. (1995). Episodic memory or autonoetic consciousness: Developmen-
tal evidence and a theory of childhood amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
59, 516–548.
Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. B. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it
from your sibs. Child Development, 64, 1228–1238.
Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An inter-
vention study. Journal of Child Language, 26, 46–67.
Pillemer, D. (1998). What is remembered about early childhood events? Clinical Psychology
Review, 18, 895–913.

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002


Autobiographical Memory Development 141
Pillemer, D. B., Picariello, M. L., & Pruett, J. C. (1994). Very long-term memories of a salient
preschool event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 95–106.
Pillemer, D. B. & White, S. H. (1989). Childhood events recalled by children and adults. In H.
W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 21, pp. 297–340). San
Diego: Academic Press.
Pillow, B. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 116–129.
Povinelli, D. J. (1995). The unduplicated self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Advances in psychology, Vol.
112. The self in infancy: Theory and research (pp. 161–192). Amsterdam: North Holland-
Elsevier.
Povinelli, D. J., Landau, K. R., & Perilloux, H. K. (1996). Self-recognition in young children
using delayed versus live feedback: Evidence of a developmental asynchrony. Child Devel-
opment, 67, 1504–1554.
Povinelli, D. J., Landry, A. M., Theall, L. A., Clark, B. R., & Castille, C. M. (1999). Develop-
ment of young children’s understanding that the recent past is causally bound to the present.
Developmental Psychology, 35, 1426–1439.
Povinelli, D. J. & Simon, B. B. (1998). Young children’s understanding of briefly versus
extremely delayed images of the self: Emergence of the autobiographical stance. Develop-
mental Psychology, 34, 188–194.
Reese, E. (1999). What children say when they talk about the past. Narrative Inquiry, 9, 1–27.
Reese, E. (in press). A model of the origins of autobiographical memory. In H. Hayne & J.
Fagen (Eds.), Progress in Infancy Research, Volume 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Reese, E. & Brown, N. (2000). Reminiscing and recounting in the preschool years. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 14, 1–17.
Reese, E. & Farrant, K. (in press). Social origins of reminiscing. In R. Fivush & C. A. Haden
(Eds.), Connecting culture and memory: The construction of an autobiographical self.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Reese, E. & Fivush, R. (1993). Parental styles of talking about the past. Developmental
Psychology, 29, 596–606.
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversations about the past: Rela-
tionships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403–430.
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1996). Mothers, fathers, daughters, sons: Gender dif-
ferences in autobiographical remembering. Research on Language and Social Interaction,
29, 27–56.
Rovee-Collier, C. (1997). Dissociations in infant memory: Rethinking the development of
implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Review, 104, 467–498.
Ruffman, T., Perner, J., & Parkin, L. (2000). How parenting style affects false belief under-
standing. Social Development, 8, 395–411.
Sachs, J. (1983). Topic selection in parent-child discourse. Discourse Processes, 2, 145–153.
Skene, K. & Moore, C. (in press). Binding the self in time. In C. Moore & K. Skene (Eds.),
The self in time: Developmental issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Singer, J. A. & Salovey, P. (1993). The remembered self: Emotion and memory in personality.
New York, NY: The Free Press.
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human
mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167.
Taylor, M. (1988). The development of children’s understanding of the seeing-knowing dis-
tinction. In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of mind
(pp. 207–225). Melbourne: Cambridge University Press.
Tessler, M. & Nelson, K. (1994). Making memories: The influence of joint encoding on later
recall by young children. Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 307–326.
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cogni-
tion and action. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Hand-
book of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th Ed, pp.
563–634). New York: Wiley.
Waters, E. (1987/1995). The Attachment Q-set (Version 3.0). In E. Waters, B. Vaughn, G.
Posada, & K. Kondo-Ikemura (Eds.), Caregiving, cultural, and cognitive perspectives on

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002


142 Elaine Reese
secure-base behavior and working models: New growing points of attachment theory and
research, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Developmetn (Vol. 60 2–3, Serial
No. 244, pp. 234–246).
Waters, H. S., Rodrigues, L. M., & Ridgeway, D. (1998). Cognitive underpinnings of narrative
attachment assessment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 211–234.
Welch-Ross, M. K. (1995). An integrative model of the development of autobiographical
memory. Developmental Review, 15, 338–365.
Welch-Ross, M. K. (1997). Mother-child participation in conversation about the past: Rela-
tionships to preschoolers’ theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 33, 618–629.
Welch-Ross, M. K. (in press). Personalizing the temporally extended self: Evaluative self-
awareness and the development of autobiographical memory. In C. Moore & K. Skene (Eds.),
The self in time: Developmental issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Welch-Ross, M. K., Fasig, L. G., & Farrar, M. J. (1999). Predictors of preschoolers’ self-
knowledge: Reference to emotion and mental states in mother-child conversation about past
events. Cognitive Development, 14, 401–422.
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The
frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–354.
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining func-
tion of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13,
103–128.

Acknowledgements
I would like to thank Melissa Welch-Ross for discussing the ideas in this review with me, and
William Friedman, Catherine Haden, and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments
on the manuscript.

© Blackwell Publishers Ltd 2002 Social Development, 11, 1, 2002

You might also like