You are on page 1of 15

Maney Publishing

Flintknapping Skill, Communal Hunting, and Paleoindian Projectile Point Typology


Author(s): Douglas B. Bamforth
Source: Plains Anthropologist, Vol. 36, No. 137 (November 1991), pp. 309-322
Published by: Maney Publishing on behalf of the Plains Anthropological Society
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25669063 .
Accessed: 02/09/2014 22:33

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Plains Anthropological Society and Maney Publishing are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Plains Anthropologist.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Flintknapping Skill,
Communal Hunting, and Paleoindian
Projectile Point Typology
by
Douglas B. Bamforth

ABSTRACT
Archaeologists reconstruct culture-historical patterns on thePlains during thePaleoindian period
primarily by studying the appearance and technology of projectile points, particularly projectile points
recovered from communal kill sites. TJiispaper discusses some of the implications of this emphasis on
communal kill assemblages for the interpretation of morphological and technological variation in
Paleoindian projectile points. It then considers three temporal and spatial patterns inPaleoindian point
typeswith these issues inmind, focusing on thedistribution offluted and non-fluted points during the
Folsom period, thepattern of variation inCody complex points, and the nature of theFirstview Com
plex.

INTRODUCTION searches for cultural-historical markers, and


current typological practices essentially assume
Although there are many reasons for the
that any and all differences we can see among
level of amateur and professional interest in
Paleoindian one undoubted Paleoindian projectile points are equally
archaeology,
reason is that Paleoindian sites produce really relevant to this search, an assumption which is
incorrect. Like all archaeological research,
nice artifacts, the nicest of which are usually
typological studies require an explicit problem
projectile points. The excellence of Paleoindian
orientation which defines the kinds of data
stone-working, which ismanifest most clearly in
needed to solve the problem: ifwe are interested
projectile points, is regularly cited as an impor
in patterns of artifact use, we need one kind of
tantand distinctive aspect of the early human oc
of North America information; ifwe hope to identify culture/his
cupation (i.e., Goodyear
toricalmarkers, we generally need another. The
1989;Hayden 1982;Kelly andTodd 1988).The
purpose of thispaper is to consider some of the
beauty and technical sophistication of Paleoin
documented patterns of variation inPaleoindian
dian points, along with the fact that they ob
projectile point morphology on the Plains from
viously vary substantially in time and space, have
this perspective.
led archaeologists to study these artifacts inten
sively, and our current understanding of time SOURCES OF VARIATION IN
space systematics on the Plains during the PROJECTILE POINT MORPHOLOGY
Paleoindian period rests almost entirely on The major assumption underlying most
analyses of the temporal and spatial distribution
of distinctive kinds of points. typological studies of Paleoindian projectile
points is that consistent, observable differences
The studies which have documented theun
among groups of points indicate differences in
doubted variation inPaleoindian points, though,
have told us littleabout themeaning of thisvaria community-level standards (Deetz's [1967:45]
"mental templates") governing what points
tion:we know that these artifacts look different,
should look like and how they should be made:
but we know very little about why they look dif
thus, groups of points which share a consistent
ferent. Archaeological typology in general

309

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PLAINS ANTHROPOLOGIST [Vol. 36, No. 137,1991

set of morphological and/or technological terns, outline shape, and cross-sectional sym
characteristics are generally designated as metry, as many archaeologists have noted (Flen
"types". Community-level standards, though, nikenandRaymond1986; Wilmsen
Wheat 1979;
are not the only factorswhich structure variation and Roberts 1978).
inartifact appearance and production, as a num The appearance of any given artifact when
ber of authors (Bradley and Stanford 1987; Flen itreaches the hands of an archaeologist is there
niken and Raymond 1986; Knudson 1973; fore the result of a complex interplay between a
Wheat 1972) have pointed out. For the pur wide range of forces, potentially including cul
poses of this discussion, it is possible to divide tural expectations/mental templates, individual
these other factors into those which affect the preferences and production habits, rawmaterial
appearance of an artifact (in the case at issue constraints, and that artifact's life history.
here, a projectile point) when it isproduced and Typological studies focused on the recognition
those which alter this appearance over the of cultural-historical markers therefore need to
course of itsuseful life. be able to factor these variables out.
Among the most important factors in the Archaeologists interested specifically in
firstgroup are idiosyncratic knapping habits and Paleoindian projectile points have been aware
the level of knapping skill of individual of the effects of use-related changes in artifact
stoneworkers. These factors are likely to be appearance for some time (see, forexample, dis
manifest inmany aspects of point morphology: cussions of thePlainview/Meserve distinction by
for example, good knappers are more likely than Davis [1954]andWormington[1957]),and this
poor knappers to produce symmetrical points awareness has been increasingly explicit inmore
with regular patterns of finishing flaking and in recent typological studies, which virtually always
dividual stone-workers may develop their own attempt to control for the effects of such proces
methods of platform preparation or subtle dif ses as resharpening (i.e., Bradley and Frison
ferences in pressure flaking techniques through 1987;Wheat 1979). The effects of rawmaterial
their own experimentation (Young and Bon differences are less widely acknowledged, but
nichsen 1984). More accomplished knappers are likely tobe minimized in the case of Paleoin
are also more likely to rely on complex, staged dian points by thewell-known emphasis on very
reduction strategies than less accomplished high-quality stone inPaleoindian assemblages.
knappers. Archaeological approaches to Paleoindian
the discussion below will em ex
Although projectile point typology have therefore
or at least some
phasize these two potential sources of variation, plicitly implicitly controlled for
it is important to note that their effects can be of the factors which may blur our ability to dis
modified by other factors. In particular, the cern reliable cultural-historical types. My goal

ability of even themost accomplished flintknap here is to expand on these emphases and focus
per to express his or her individual styleof finish on thelikely
specifically effects fac
of individual
ing flaking, for example, is constrained by the tors on typological variation in Paleoindian
more
quality of the raw material being worked: points.
granular or intractable stone is less likely to
allow for such expression and ismore likely to
PROJECTILE POINT TYPOLOGY
AND COMMUNAL BISON HUNTING
result in less neatly flaked, less symmetrical
It is important to account explicitly for in
products than less granular stone.
Once a tool is produced, other factorsmay dividual differences in flint-knapping skill and
alter its appearance over the course of itsuseful technique in typological studies of Paleoindian
ar
life. In the case of projectile points, themost im projectile points for two reasons. First, the
tifacts which have been emphasized in such
portant of these is likely to be the process of
studies are usually thosewhich show evidence of
resharpening or rejuvenation which occurs after
a point breaks. Points may be resharpened at the highest levels of flint-knapping skill: the
either the tip or the base, altering flaking pat points taken as "typical" of the various divisions

310

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Douglas B. Bamforth] PALEOINDIAN PROJECTILE POINT

of the period are generally those which are the the organization of such hunts might affect the
most symmetrical, themost carefully flaked, and kinds of artifacts recovered from communal kill
themost difficult to produce, and points which sites.
deviate from these are generally considered to For present purposes, it is particularly im
be "atypical" or "non-diagnostic" in some way; portant to consider how projectile points might
such points are sometimes not even illustrated have been produced for a communal hunt. To
inpublished reports. Thus, existing studies tend conduct such a hunt, it is necessary to have suf
to emphasize a limited portion of the full of ficient tools on hand to accomplish all of the
variation in the artifacts we study, a practice taskswhich will be necessary, including killing a
which has the potential to blind us to the impor large number of animals and processing their
tance of this variation. carcasses. Reher and Frison (1980) emphasize
Second, although it is likely thatvariation in this, pointing out that prehistoric communal
theskilllevelsandknappinghabitsof individual hunters tended to "gear up" for thehunt, acquir
flint-knappers "average out" in some sense for ing raw material and producing an adequate
most typological problems, an unusual aspect of stock of weapons and other tools in advance of
Paleoindian archaeology on the Plains has the the kill.
potential tomake such variation a problem for Although advance preparation may not al
us. Virtually all collections of Paleoindian ways have included special trips to quarries for
points which provide large enough samples of raw material (Ingbar and Hofman 1989), there
artifacts for systematic analysis derive from can be no doubt that itwas necessary to have an
communal kills, and there are good reasons for adequate stock of tools prepared before the
supposing that assemblages from such sites are hunt began. The archaeologically visible
extremely likely to show us systematic patterns aspects of this preparation are often limited to
in both individual and community-level varia stone tools, but getting ready for a communal kill
tion. Thisis because communal kill sites often clearly required the production or repair of
represent what is essentially a single moment many other implements and (in at least some
from the past, in contrast to other types of ar times and places) facilities as well. For example,
chaeological sites, which often preserve the when herds were taken in corrals or other
remains of a number of occupations which may humanly-built or modified traps, itwas neces
have involved different social groups visiting a sary to construct or repair them. In addition,
locale over long periods of time. Individual projectile points cannot be used without shafts,
communal kills correspond to the actions of a or at least foreshafts, and, ifthesewere not avail
single group of people over a very short period able in sufficient numbers before a kill, itwould
of time, and artifact assemblages from such kills have been necessary to manufacture them as
therefore represent the tools in use for a single well. Processing meat and hides inbulk required
activity at what is close to a single moment in facilities such as drying racks, which meant that
time. wood had tobe gathered and racks constructed.
Although there can be no doubt that com Conducting the ceremonies which, at least in
munal hunting was an important aspect of the recent times, were an important part of com
Paleoindian adaptation to thePlains, it iscertain munal hunting, would also require advance
that itwas not the onlymeans by which Paleoin preparation of ritual items and facilities.
dian groups provisioned themselves: communal Overall, then, to prepare for a communal
kills were apparently carried out most often kill it must usually have been necessary to
during the winter (Frison 1982; McCartney produce a relatively large number of projectile
1990), and non-communal kills are well-known points at a time when there were many other
(Bamforth 1985; Hester 1972). Communal items to manufacture and tasks to carry out.
hunts therefore represent only one portion of Under these conditions, the most efficient
the overall pattern of Paleoindian activities on means of accomplishing everythingwhich had to
thePlains, making it interesting to consider how be done would have been to allocate tasks to in

311

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PLAINS ANTHROPOLOGIST [Vol. 36, No. 137,1991

dividuals on the basis of their special skills and tion strategies and overall point morphology)
knowledge.If thisiscorrect,itis likelythatthe probably reflect community/cultural standards,
most accomplished flint-knappers available while minor features (such as small variations in
would have produced the stone tools needed, retouch patterns and minor shape differences)
both to ensure that an adequate number of ef probably reflect variation in the habits of in
fectiveweapons would be available when itwas dividual stone-workers.
time to carry out the kill and also to free other Given the importance in all scientific re
individuals for other important tasks. search of studying variables which are relevant
This possibility has two important implica to theproblems we are tryingto solve, the incor
tions. First, itsuggests thatcollections ofprojec poration of both "major" and "minor" features
tilepoints from communal killsmay represent a into Paleoindianprojectile point typologies has
restricted portion of the total variation in the the potential of creating serious interpretive
kindsofprojectilepointsproducedbyanygiven problems: types defined for culture-historical
social group over the course of their annual ac purposes presumably should reflect variation in
tivities.Communal kill collections thusmay be artifactmorphology which is determined at the
dominated by the products of themost skillful community rather than the individual level, and
stone-workers in thatgroup. There may, then,be the variables used to define Paleoindian point
systematic seasonal or other variations between types probably measure both of these.
projectile point assemblages produced for com The difficulty, then, lies in identifyingmajor
munal kills and those produced in other con and minor features archaeologically. Bradley
texts. Second, it implies that thepoints from any and Stanford (1987:412) rely on their extensive
one communal kill are likely to have been experimental experience in drawing thisdistinc
produced by a very small number of individuals, tion,but it is also possible to identifypatterns in
and sometimes by only one individual, as other the archaeological record which may support
researchers have suggested (Hester and Grady their arguments. Particularly, if projectile
1977;Knudson 1973). This impliesthat the points from any one communal kill site do rep
idiosyncratic knapping habits of individual resent thework of a very small number of stone
stone-workers are likely tobe particularly visible workers, it is reasonable to argue that patterns
in communal kill assemblages. which are widespread from site to site (such as
It is therefore important to distinguish outline form) are likely to represent "major"
variation in projectile point form due to dif typological features, while characteristics which
ferences in community standards from variation are found at only one or two sites (such as small
due to differences in the habits and preferences differences in the depth of flake scars) are like
of individual flintknappers. Typological studies ly to represent "minor" typological features.
of Paleoindian points have generally not at SOME ARCHAEOLOGICAL EXAMPLES
tempted to do this.However, Bradley and Stan
ford (1987) have recently outlined a perspective The remainder of this paper considers the
which helps tomake such a distinction possible. implications of these arguments for three
some adaptive
Archaeologists divide Paleoindian points specific topics. First, it suggests
into types on thebasis of two fundamental kinds implications of the distribution of fluted and un
of variation: variation in overall morphology, fluted points during theFolsom period. Second,
particularly in outline form, and variation in it considers the implications of the distinction
production techniques, including overall reduc between major and minor typological features
tion strategies and patterns of finishing flaking. for our understanding of the differences among
Bradley and Stanford (1987:411-412) point out Cody Complex projectile points on the Plains.
that variation in these areas is caused by more Finally, it examines the geographic pattern of
than one factor, and particularly argue that variation inCody projectile points, with a focus
major typological features (such as basic reduc on the concept of the Firstview complex.

312

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Douglas B. Bamforth] PALEOINDIAN PROJECTILE POINT

Table 1: Frequencies of fluted and unfluted points in Folsom period archaeological sites on the Great Plains.

Site_Type_Fluted_Unfluted_Reference_

Lindenmeier 48camp 38 Wilmsen andRoberts 1978


Blackwater Draw camp/small kill 52 27Hester
1972
communal kill
Folsom 19 0
Wormington 1957
communalkill
Linger 16 1943
0Hurst
communal
Zapata kill? 2 0 Wormington 1957
MacHaffie 2 camp 1 Wormington1957,
Forbis and Sperry1952
Sharbauer 7 camp 21 Wendorf et al. 1955
Elida 15 camp 3 Hester 1962,Warnica 1961
Hanson 12 camp 4 FrisonandBradley 1980
Agate Basin 4camp 2 Bradley 1982
communal kill 22 1 Hofman et al. 1988
Lipscomb
Sands
Shifting 19
camp Hofman
59 et al. 1990

To Flute or Not to Flute? number of sites, and recent work byHofman et


al. (1990) suggests strongly that theywere made
Archaeological treatment of Folsom points
and used side by side. Unfluted projectile
perhaps best illustrates the effects of our
on the "best" points and points were, then, a common component of the
typological emphasis
concomitant neglect of patterns of variation. weaponry of hunters on theGreat Plains during
For most archaeologists, the Folsom period is
the Folsom period (cf. Ingbar and Hofman
1989). Similarly, unfluted points were relatively
typifiedby itscharacteristic point, and thispoint common in the Clovis levels at Blackwater
isbest-known from the collection at theLinden
Draw, the type site for the fluted Clovis point
meier site: replication of the "Lindenmeier
Folsom" is the explicit goal ofmany experimen (Hester1972).
I have
argued elsewhere (Bamforth
tal studies of fluted points. This emphasis,
1988:187-188) that fluted and unfluted points
though, obscures the fact that only 48 of the 86 in the skill needed to
differ substantially
finished points recovered from theLindenmeier
produce them: extensive experimentation (i.e.,
site are fluted: 17 are "pseudo-fluted" points, in
Akerman and Fagan 1986; Frison and Bradley
which large portions of the unworked surface of
the flake blank were leftvisible tomimic the ap 1982; Crabtree 1968; Flenniken 1978; Sollberger
1985; Tunnell 1977) indicates unambiguously
pearance of a flute, and another 21 are simply
thatmanufacturing a fluted point may involve
unfluted lanceolate points (Wilmsen and
the use of special equipment to remove the flute
Roberts 1978:105, 159). Similarly, 4 of the 16
and is certainly a complex process involving
finished points from theHanson site and 2 of the
6 finished points from theAgate Basin site are many stages and a high degree of skill inprepar
unfluted (Bradley 1982; Frison and Bradley ing preforms. Although unfluted points may
also be the result of relatively long sequences of
1980).
In fact, a survey of the published sites on the production(Bradleyand Frison 1987;Bradley
and Stanford 1987), finishing a point by pressure
Great Plains which date to the Folsom period
flaking is generally a skill which apprentice
(Table 1) indicates that fully79 of the 270
knappers learn farmore easily than fluting.
projectile points they produced, or nearly 30
Following the arguments above, if points
percent, are unfluted. This total includes
used in communal kills are mainly the products
"Midland" points, which some authors (i.e.,
of themost skillful knappers in a social group,
Gunnerson 1987) have suggested represent an
Folsom period communal kills should be
occupation of the Plains which is distinct from
dominated by fluted rather than unfluted points.
Folsom. However, Midland points are found in
Table 2 summarizes the frequencies of fluted
close association with fluted Folsom points at a

313

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PLAINS ANTHROPOLOGIST [Vol. 36, No. 137,1991

Table 2: Frequencies of fluted and unfluted points in communal kill


and other Folsom period archaeological sites on the Great Plains.

Site
type_Fluted_Unfluted_Total_

communalkill 136 *
37 -
(26.0) (11.0)
other 154 79 233
(164.0) (69.0)
total_191_79_270_
*
observed value
~
expected value

and unfluted points in communal kill sites and point production, and any ritual significance
other sites of Folsom-age (with the latter includ such points may have had may also have led them
ing campsites such as Hanson and Agate Basin to be used preferentially in communal hunts.
and non-communal kills such as Blackwater Regardless of the specific interpretation of
Draw), using the data inTable 1. "Communal the pattern inTable 2, thispattern suggests that
kills" for present purposes are sites which there was more than one context of projectile
produced large quantities of bison bone, often point production inFolsom period society, and
including articulated skeletal units, along with this possibility leads us into a number of inter
projectile points, and few or no other categories esting topics. Gearing-up for recent communal
of stone tools or features (Fawcett 1986). Sites hunts appears to have taken place in the context
which do not fall into this category?that is, sites of a seasonal pattern of population aggregation
with small numbers of bison represented or with and dispersion, with communal hunting carried
numerous features, such as hearths, or a wide out during periods of aggregation. The data in
range of artifacts present?are classified here as Table 2 are at least consistent with such a pat
"other". This table indicates that only one un tern,although theyclearly do not prove that such
fluted point, a pseudo-fluted point from the a pattern existed. However, the possibility that
Lipscomb site, has been recovered from a Fol Folsom hunters had a seasonal or other pattern
som period communal kill, and that this absence of population aggregation which provided the
is not likely to have occurred by chance (chi labor for communal hunts is a potentially criti
= = =
square 15.0, df 1, .001> p, Yule's Q 0.9). cal aspect of Folsom period adaptations. It is,
There are relatively few Folsom period for example, difficult to reconcile with Kelly and
communal kills in the available sample, and it is Todd's (1988) recent arguments that fluted
therefore important to consider this pattern point adaptations in North America involved
cautiously. However, these data do suggest that frequent and unpredictable moves by small
Folsom period hunters participating in com groups of people from communal kill to com
munal huntsmade and used a restricted portion munal kill: such a pattern requires social groups
of the range ofweaponry on which they relied in tomaintain the labor needed for the kill at all
other circumstances, and that this portion in times and also would make itdifficult to plan ag
cluded only those items which were the most gregations of dispersed groups with any success.
labor-intensive and difficult to produce. Fol
How Many Kinds of Cody Points Are There?
lowing the arguments above, thismay reflect the
efficient use of time and labor during the gear A second major topic in Paleoindian ar
ing-up phase prior to a communal hunt, al chaeology is the analysis of differences among a
though there are certainly other possibilities. set of square-based, slightly stemmed, lanceo
For example, Frison and Bradley (1982) have late projectile points which are found
suggested that there may have been important throughout the western and northern portions
ceremonial activities associated with fluted of the Plains and date between approximately
314

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Douglas B. Bamforth] PALEOINDIAN PROJECTILE POINT

Table 3:RawMaterials used byProjectilePointType at theHorner Site


and Frison 1987, Tables 6.1 and
_(after Bradley 6.2)._
TYPE
Alberta/ Alberta/
Cody I Cody II Scottsbluff Eden Other
Material_n %_n %_n %_n %_%_n

Phosphoria 10 28.6 0 0.0 4 33.366.7


10 1 11.1
Porcellanite 13 37.1 3 75.0 1 213.3
8.3 3 33.3
Morrison 7 20.0 0 0.0 1 16.7
8.3 0 0.0
Quartzite
Morrison 1 2.9 1 25.0 1 6.7
3 25.0 2 22.2
Chert
4 Other
11.4 0 0.0 1 6.7
3 25.0 3 33.3

Total
35 4 9
1215

10,000 and 8000 BP. Varieties of these points Considered in light of Bradley and
are often grouped into three complexes with dis Stanford's (1987) distinction between major and
tinctive temporal and/or spatial distributions: minor typological features, this array of
Alberta, dating between 9500 and 9000 BP and categories is extremely interesting. Bradley's
found in themore northern Plains; Cody, dating experimental work indicates quite clearly that
between 9000 and 8500 BP in the same area; and Eden points represent the end-product of a very
datingbetween 10,000and 8000 BP
Firstview, specialized multi-stage production sequence,
and found in the central and southern Plains with theirdistinctive medial ridge and long, nar
(i.e., Agenbroad 1978; Irwin-Williams et al. row outline form resulting from multiple se
1973;JohnsonandHolliday 1981;
Wheat 1972). quences of pressure flaking. This work also
For reasons which will become clear over the indicates that simply terminating this sequence
course of the next two sections, this paper sub after fewer stages of pressure flaking produces
sumes the material in all three of these com many of the other formswhich have been iden
plexes under the general term "Cody". tified inCody period collections; that is, the ex
The general trend in typological studies of istence of a single reduction sequence which was
Cody points over the past 20 years has been to terminated at different stages can account for
name increasing numbers of more and more many of the differences among Cody period
finely distinguished "types". From a basic point types.
dichotomy between Scottsbluff and Eden points In fact, itmay be that decisions regarding
(i.e., Sellards 1952), archaeologists divided the where to terminate this sequence relied, at least
Scottsbluff category into two classes and defined in part, on rawmaterial considerations. For ex
a set of similar but larger and somewhat less fine ample, of themany Cody types defined by Brad
ly flaked points as the Alberta type (Wor ley and Frison (1987) at theHorner Site, only
mington 1957; also see Howard 1943; Eden points show any strong tendency to be as
Satterthwaite 1957). More recently, Wheat sociated with a specific raw material (Table 3):
(1972,1979) has defined three new types (Ker fully two-thirds of these are made from Phos
sey, Firstview, and San Jon) and Bradley and phoria chert. In contrast, Phosphoria chert was
Frison (1987) have recognized two typeswhich used for only one-fourth to one-third of the
are "intermediate" between Alberta and Scot other reasonably numerous classes of points.
tsbluff I; in addition, these authors refer to Many of the differences on which typologi
"Cody intermediate" and "Eden/Scottsbluff" cal distinctions among Cody point types rely are
points. There are, then, a minimum of 11 dif in "minor" typological features: Firstview and
ferent kinds of Cody period projectile points Kersey points, for example, are stemmed by
defined in the literature at present. grinding rather than by flaking, and other types

315

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PLAINS ANTHROPOLOGIST [Vol. 36, No. 137,1991

aredistinguished inthe
bydifferences
primarily seem to form a continuous range of variation in
width and depth of the last round of pressure both technology and morphology, and Wheat's
flaking represented. Similarly, differences in (1972) plots show such continua clearly. The ex
the degree of stemming of Cody points, such as ception to this generalization may be in theAl
thoserecently
emphasizedbyEbell (1988),may berta category, which is finished by percussion
result from the individual preferences of dif rather than pressure flaking and which may date
ferent stoneworkers or simply from the need to somewhat earlier than the other Cody types
haft a standardized set of points made by one (Agenbroad 1978), but there are so few dated
person to a standardized set of shaftsmade by Alberta sites that it is difficult to put much con
someone else. In addition, many types are also fidence in this conclusion.
found only or primarily at one site (i.e., Kersey I hasten to add that such an interpretation
points), and virtually all of the sites which have in no way implies that each typewhich has been
been studied are communal kills. recognized represents a specific prehistoric in
Typological studies of Cody period points, dividual: none of the minor features which
then, rely primarily on communal kill as define the various types seems so esoteric that it
semblages and regularly incorporate minor could not have been developed independently
morphological and technological featureswhich by a number of different people. It simply im
are likely to represent idiosyncratic differences that the occurrence of a similar
plies widespread
among individual flintknappers. Furthermore, point shape and reduction strategy the pattern
is
many aspects of the differences among types which we would expect formajor, or culturally
show patterns which would be expected ifthe as determined, variation, and the much more
semblages from single kills are dominated by the restricted distributions of innumerable tiny
products of very small numbers of flintknappers; variations in shape and technology conform to
that is, there are many minor variations between the pattern we would expect forminor, or in
the assemblages from individual sites. These dividually determined, variation. Following the
patterns suggest that the often bewildering arguments above, the visibility of individual
diversity of distinct categories of points which level variation in this context results from the
have been defined for the Cody period may fact thatmost of theprojectile points which have
reflect the number of differentpeople who made been studied derive from communal kills.
them in the past and not the number of distinct
The Firstview Complex
community/cultural standards which existed.
That is,many of the varieties of Cody-period ViewingCodyperiodprojectilepointsfrom
points which have been identified probably rep thisperspective also illuminates a potentially im
resent individual rather than cultural variation portant spatial pattern on the Plains during the
(cf.Knudson 1973:151). Late Paleoindian period. Although we clearly
Following Bradley and Stanford, then, this do not understand in any detail how prehistoric
implies that the Cody "cultural" pattern in people maintained uniform styles of artifact
cluded production of slightly stemmed, square production over large areas or long periods of
based, lanceolate points using a fairly time, it seems safe to infer that the existence of
standardized multi-stage reduction strategy, such styles indicates some kind of contact or in
and that individual stone-workers interpreted teraction. The geographic extent of a given
thisbasic pattern in terms of theirown individual style, then, provides a rough indication of the
skills and preferences. These skills and spatial scale of this interaction. It is therefore
preferences are simply visible to us in this con useful to examine a major distinction which has
text because of the emphasis on communal kill been drawn between Northern and Southern
sites in Paleoindian archaeology. A number of Plains projectile points during theLate Paleoin
authors (i.e., Bradley and Frison 1987; Bradley dian period.
and Stanford 1987; Ebell 1988), in fact,quite ex Wheat (1972) distinguishes a series of point
plicitlynote that
manyCody periodpointtypes types from theSouthern and Central Plains from
316

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Douglas B. Bamforth] PALEOINDIAN PROJECTILE POINT

Table 4: Characteristics of Projectile Points from the Plainview and Milnesand Sites.

Plainview Milnesand

Flake Blank Visible


no 4 22.2 4 40.0
one face 9 50.0 2 20.0
both faces 5 27.8 4 40.0

Number of Finished Edge Surfaces


1 0 0.0 2 20.0
2 0 0.0 2 20.0
3 2 103 1 10.0
4 17 89.5 5 50.0

the classic Cody types of the more northern For present purposes, I define Plainview
areas, and refers to the former as the Firstview points as the points recovered from the Plain
Complex. This complex includes two stemmed, view type site and Milnesand points as thepoints
Cody-style types (Firstview and San Jon), along recovered from theMilnesand type site. The
with Milnesand and Plainview points (also see sample of material I consider here includes all
Johnson and Holliday 1981). As the previous Plainview(n = 21) and 11of
of thepointsfrom
section discusses, the key distinctions between thepoints recovered during the initial investiga
Firstview and San Jon points and other types of tion of theMilnesand site 1, all of which are
stemmed, lanceolate points made during Cody presently stored at the Texas Archaeological
times lie inwhat seem tobe minor features; these Research Laboratory. The typological affilia
types can, arguably, be subsumed within the tions of these points have been discussed exten
general range of Cody technological and mor sively(Johnsonand Holliday 1980;Knudson
phological variation. Milnesand and Plainview 1983;Krieger1947;Sellards1955;Sellardset al.
points, however, show a number of important 1947; Warnica and Williamson 1968; Wheat
differences from theCody material, particularly 1972).
in the fact that they are unstemmed, a charac One hallmark of the Cody reduction
teristicwhich Wheat (1972) stresses. Further strategy is the extremely regular pattern of
more, points which can clearly be placed into finishing flaking, which, in most cases, com
these two unstemmed types are largely or com pletely obscures evidence of previous stages of
pletely restricted to the Southern Plains flaking. At Northwestern Plains Cody sites, for
(Johnson and Holliday 1980; Wheat 1972), in example, points which show traces of the flake
contrast to the stemmed Cody material, which blanks fromwhich theywere made are extremely
occurs fromTexas toCanada. rare or entirely absent (i.e., Bradley and Frison
The absence of any kind of stemming on 1987; Ebell 1988; Satterthwaite 1957). In con
Plainview and Milnesand points indicates that trast, a very large proportion of both the Plain
they lack the first of the two major typological view and theMilnesand points (Table 4) show
features defining Cody points. As I noted above, traces of their original flake blanks, and many of
following Bradley and Frison (1987), the other them show extensive traces; many of these tools
major typological feature defining Cody points are essentially marginally-worked flakes, al
is the complex, multi-stage reduction strategy though themarginal flaking is often quite well
used to produce them. It is difficult to argue that executed (i.e., Knudsen 1973, Figure D-4).
Plainview and Milnesand points are uniformly, That is,neither of the two principal identify
or even usually, the end products of such a ing characteristics of Cody points can be seen in
strategy. either the Plainview or theMilnesand material,
while both of these characteristics can be seen

317

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PLAINS ANTHROPOLOGIST [Vol. 36, No. 137,1991

inboth theFirstview and San Jon types. Includ to look at either of these possibilities, or any
ing all four of these types together in a single others, in any detail. However, they both sug
complex which is seen as distinct from more gest thatunderstanding variation within and be
northern Cody materials, then, is somewhat tween the unstemmed point types should be a
problematic. An alternative view implies that major focus of research.
identifiable Cody material can be found Although most definitions of the Plainview
throughout thewestern Plains, but that a distinct type rely primarily on the most completely
class of unstemmed and, often,minimally staged worked points from the site (i.e., Krieger 1947;
points ismore or less confined to the Southern Wormington 1957), variability within the collec
Plains. The chronological relationships be tion iswell-documented. For example, although
tween the Cody and more purely Southern such authors as Wormington (1957:265)
Plains types are, however, not clear. Johnson describe Plainview points as "lanceolate... with
andHolliday (1980) indicatethattheradiocar parallel or slightly convex sides and concave
bon dates on Plainview points cluster around bases", the type collection includes two points
10,000 BP, somewhat earlier thanmost accept with clearly concave edges, producing a slightly
able dates for Cody points. Milnesand points flared base (Knudson 1973, Fig. D-5, D-6).
were recovered in association with Cody-style Technologically, Knudsen (1973) argues
points from deposits dated between 10,000 and that there are three distinct kinds of points in the
8500 BP at Blackwater Draw (Haynes 1975; Sel Plainview collection, each made on flake blanks
lards 1952), but there are only very limited of differingthickness
which requirediffering
provenience data on these artifacts (Hester degrees of reduction: thicker blanks must be
1972),making thespecificage of thetwopoint more extensively worked and therefore show
types at the site impossible to determine. more complete flaking. However, it is also pos
The interpretation of this pattern in human sible to view these "varieties" as divisions of a
terms is also not clear. A traditional view might continuum, with knappers accepting a range of
identify two distinct "traditions" of projectile blank sizes and working each blank as much as
point production, one found throughoutmost of necessary to produce a useable point. It is likely
the Plains and one confined to the Southern that a similar continuum underlies some of the
Plains. Such a view might then lead us into technological variation in the Milnesand
problems of ethnicity and artifact style and the material as well, as both collections show similar
conditions under which mobile hunter proportions of incompletely flaked points
gatherers might exploit overlapping territories (Table 4).
while still retaining distinctive technological pat However, there are also interesting dif
terns. However, we could also infer that com ferences between the two collections. First, the
munity standards of projectile point production margins of the points fromMilnesand generally
were simply more variable on the Southern show less carefulfinishingflakingthan those
Plains than elsewhere during the Cody period; from Plainview; while 89.5 percent of the Plain
while most or all flintknappers in other areas view points show careful regularization of the
seem to have held a single "mental template" in edges by removing tinyflakes from the ridges left
common, those in the south seem sometimes to on both edges of both faces during the last round
have worked towards this template and some of pressure flaking,only 50.0 percent of theMil
times to have neglected it.Given thispossibility, nesand points show such removals (Table 4). In
we might consider the conditions affecting the addition, all but one of thePlainview points show
degree of standardization of artifact appearance similar flake scar patterns on both faces, with
and production in different societies. this pattern usually being parallel collateral
Given the small number of documented (Table 5). In contrast, 50.0 percent of theMil
communal kills on the Southern Plains and the nesand points show different scar patterns on
poor chronological control over some of those different faces, often combining parallel col
which are known, we are clearly not in a position

318

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Douglas B. Bamforth] PALEOINDIAN PROJECTILE POINT

Table 5: Correspondence Between Flake Scar Patterns on Opposite Faces of Single Points from Plainview and Milnesand.

PLAINVIEW Face 1
Oblique_Collateral_Irregular_Total

Face 2:

Oblique 0 10 1
Collateral 0 19 19 0
Irregular 0 0 0 0
Total 020 0 20

MILNESAND Face 1
Oblique_Collateral_Irregular_Total

Face 2:

Oblique 0 2 2 0
Collateral 60 15
Irregular 0 3 3 0
Total 1 10 11 0

lateral on one face with oblique or irregular scar descriptions of the Plainview type.Whether the
patterns on the other. collections from the two sites represent a single
In addition to these distinctions, ithas been "tradition" of production or not, it is quite clear
recognized formany years that the basal con that they differ considerably from one another
figurations of Plainview and Milnesand points in stylistically and technologically interesting
differ from one another, with Plainview points ways, and that each collection includes a fairly
showing a concave base thinned by the removal variable set of implements.
of a small number of relatively long flakes, and We currently have very few conceptual tools
Milnesand points showing straight bases for understanding variation such as this, in part
finished with many short, steep flakes, resulting because we very rarely attempt to identify it.
inwhat has been described as a "wedge-shaped" One possible tool is in the hypothesis that some
base. This generalization holds strongly for the aspects of Paleoindian projectile point produc
points fromPlainview, all ofwhich show at least tionmay be linked to the environmental and so
slightly concave bases and only one of which cial conditions which allow the development of
shows a basal configuration which might be part-time craft specialists (Bamforth 1988). If
described as wedge-shaped. However, theMil this is correct, the greater variability in point
nesand material is, again, fairly variable; all of styles on the Southern Plains suggests a degree
the 7 points at TARL with intact bases show es of geographical variation inPaleoindian adapta
sentially straight bases, four (59.1 percent) do tions,with more southern groups existing under
not show a stronglywedge-shaped base while conditions which hindered reliance on a small
the remaining three (40.9 percent) do. number of highly skilled stoneworkers. If a
Overall, then, thepoints fromPlainview and greater proportion of individuals within a
Milnesand differ from the classic Cody pattern society make their own tools, for example, we
in a more "opportunistic" selection of blanks might expect to see more variation in form,
and accommodation of reduction strategies to reduction strategies, and skill levels. We clear
blank types. Furthermore, the points fromMil ly do not presently have sufficient data to
nesand show less extensive finishing flaking and evaluate hypotheses such as this,but we cannot
less standardized flaking patterns and basal even consider them ifwe continue to rely ex
design than those fromPlainview, and thePlain clusively on traditional approaches to point
view collection shows interesting variations in typology.
outline form that are not incorporated into the

319

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PLAINS ANTHROPOLOGIST [Vol. 36, No. 137,1991

CONCLUSIONS theMilnesand accession number at TARL, 23


ofwhichwere reportedby Sellards (1955).Of
The fundamental points of this paper are
these 30, sixwere not actually from the bone bed
that studies of Paleoindian projectile points,
itself, nine were returned to the landowner,
first, have emphasized a limited portion of the
three are either on display or still inplaster jack
total range of the variation in point form and
ets, and one was lost (D. Creel, pers. comm.
technology and, second, have often failed to con
sider the relevance of the attributes they ex 1989).
amine to the problems they generally hope to REFERENCES CITED
solve. These problems both limit the range of
Agenbroad, L.
questions we can study and obscure important 1978 TheHudson-MengSite:An AlbertaBisonKill in
patterns of variation, often by creating a nearly
theNebraska High Plains. University Press of
America, Washington, D.C.
impenetrable thicket of "types".
Like other artifacts, Paleoindian projectile Akerman, K., and J. Fagan

points often fall into obvious categories: Cody


1986 FlutingtheLindenmeierFolsom:A Simpleand
Economical Solution to the Problem, and its
points really do not look likeAgate Basin points, Implications for Other Fluted Point Tech
and archaeologists have recognized this. nologies. Lithic Technology 15:1-7.

However, we have gone beyond this recognition Bamforth, D. B.


inmaking that all variation in
the assumption 1985 The Technological Organization of Paleoin
and technology can dian Small-Group Bison Hunting on the Llano
projectile point morphology
Estacado. Plains Anthropologist 23:1-16.
be organized into such categories, which we call
"types". This is not the case, and assuming that
on theGreat
1988 Ecology and Human Organization
Plains. Plenum Press, New York.
it isblinds us to the existence and significance of
other kinds of variation, such as themore or less Bradley, B.
1982 Flaked Stone Technology and Typology. In The
continuous pattern of differences among Cody
Agate Basin Site: A Record of Paleoindian Oc
points. cupation of theNorthwestern High Plains, edited
As is the case for all other aspects of ar by G. Frison and D. Stanford, 181-208.
Academic Press, New York.
chaeological research, typological studies must
be guided by clear statements about thepurpose Bradley, B., and G. Frison
forwhich artifacts are being classified, careful 1987 Projectile Points and Specialized Bifaces from
the Horner Site. In The Horner Site: The Type
consideration of the kinds of data which should
Site of theCody Cultural Complex, edited by G.
be gathered to accomplish thatpurpose, and sys Frison and L. Todd, 199-232. Academic Press,
tematic analysis of the patterning of those data. New York.

a rich
Projectile points can provide us with Bradley, B., and D. Stanford
source of insight into Paleoindian ways of life, 1987 The Claypool Study. In Tlie Horner Site: The
edited
but only ifwe study themwith care and rigor. Type Site of tlieCody Cultural Complex,
405-434. Academic
by G. Frison and L. Todd,
Press, New York.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Research Crabtree, D.
Darrell Creel of the Texas Archaeological
1968 A Stoneworker's Approach to Analyzing and
Laboratory of theUniversity of Texas atAustin allowed me
Replicating theLindenmeier Folsom. Tebiwa 9:
to examine the Plainview and Milnesand collections; I am
3-39.
version of this
grateful for his time and assistance. The final
paper has also benefited from comments on earlier drafts Davis, E. M.
by Eric Ingbar and an anonymous reviewer, although 1954 The Culture History of the Central Great Plains
iswholly my Prior to the Introduction of Pottery. Ph.D. diss.,
responsibility for its strengths and weaknesses
own. of Anthropology, Harvard
Department
University, Cambridge.
NOTE
Deetz, J.
The 11 points fromMilnesand include all of 1967 Invitation to Archaeology. Natural History
Press, Garden City.
those at TARL which are currently available for
examination. A total of 30 points are listed under

320

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Douglas B. Bamforth] PALEOINDIAN PROJECTILE POINT

Ebell, S. Haynes, C.
1988 The Dunn Site. Plains Anthropologist 33: 505 1975 Pleistocene and Recent Stratigraphy. In Late
530. Pleistocene Environments of the Llano Es
tacado, edited by F. Wendorf and J.Hester, 57
Fawcett, W. 96. Fort Burgwin Research Center, Ranchos de
1986 Variation on the Lithic from
Assemblages Taos, New Mexico.
Bison Kills and Associated Camp and Process
29:9-28. J.
ingAreas. Wyoming Archaeologist Hester,
1962 A FolsomLithicComplex fromtheElida Site,
Flenniken, J. Roosevelt 69:
County, New Mexico. El Palacio
1978 Reevaluation of the Lindenmeier Folsom: A 92-113.
Replication Experiment in Lithic Technology.
American Antiquity 43:474-480. 1972 BlackwaterLocalityNo. 1:A Stratified
Early
Man Site inEastern New Mexico. Fort Burgwin
Flenniken, J., and A. Raymond Research Center, Ranchos de Taos, New
1986 Morphological Projectile Point Typology: Mexico.
Replication Experimentation and Technologi
cal Analysis. American Antiquity 51: 603-614. Hester, J., and J. Grady
1977 Paleoindian Social Patterns on the Llano Es
Forbis, R., and J. Sperry tacado. Museum Journal 17: 78-96.
1952 An Early Man Site inMontana. American An
J.,D. Amick, and R. Rose
tiquity 18:127-132. Hofman,
1990 Shifting Sands: A Folsom-Midland As
Frison, G. semblage from a Campsite inWestern Texas.
1978 Prehistoric Hunters of the High Plains. Plains Anthropologist 35: 221-254.
Academic Press, New York.
Hofman, J., L. Todd, and C. Schultz
1982 Paleoindian Winter Subsistence Strategies on 1988 The Lipscomb Bison Quarry. Continuing In
theHigh Plains. InPlains Indian Studies: A Col Site on
vestigation at a Folsom Kill-Butchery
lection ofEssays inHonor of John C. Ewers and the Southern Plains. Paper presented at the
Waldo Wedel, edited by D. Ubelaker and D. 46th Annual Plains Conference, Wichita, Kan
Stanford, 193-201. Smithsonian Contributions sas.
toAnthropology, No. 30, Smithsonian Institu
tion,Washington, D.C. Howard, E.
1943 The Finley Site: Discovery of Yuma Points in
Frison, G., and B. Bradley situ, Near Eden, Wyoming. American Antiquity
1980 Folsom Tools and Teclmology at theHanson 8: 224-234.
Site. University of New Mexico Press, Albu
querque. Hurst, C.
1943 A Folsom Site in a Mountain Valley of
1982 Fluting Folsom Points. In The Agate Basin Site: Colorado. American Antiquity 8:250-253.
A Record of Paleoindian Occupation of the
Northwestern High Plains, edited by G. Frison Ingbar, E., and J.Hofman
and D. Stanford, 209-212. Academic Press, New 1989 Folsom Fluting Fallacies. Paper presented at
York. the 47th Annual Plains Conference, Sioux Falls,
South Dakota.
Frison, G., and D. Stanford, eds.
1982 The Agate Basin Site: A Record of Paleoindian Irwin-Williams, C, H. Irwin, G. Agogino, and C. Haynes
Occupation of the Northwestern Plains. 1973 Hell Gap: Paleoindian Occupation on theHigh
Academic Press, New York. Plains. Plains Anthropologist 19: 40-53.

Goodyear, A. Johnson, E., and V. Holliday


1989 A Hypothesis for the Use of Cryptocrystalline 1980 A Plainview Kill-Butchering Locale on the
Raw Materials Among Paleoindian Groups of Llano Estacado?The Lubbock Lake Site.
North America. In Eastern Paleoindian Lithic Plains Antliropologist 25: 89-111.
Resource Use, edited by C. Ellis and J.Lothrop,
1-10.Westview Press, Boulder, Colorado. 1981 Late Paleoindian Activity at the Lubbock Lake
Site. Plains Anthropologist 26:173-193.
Gunnerson, J.
1987 Archaeology of theHigh Plains. Bureau of Land Kelly, R., and L. Todd
Denver. 1988 Coming into the Country: Early Paleoindian
Management,
Hunting Mobility. American Antiquity 53: 231
Hadyen, B. 244.
1982 Interaction Parameters and the Demise of
Paleoindian Plains Knudson, R.
Craftsmanship.
27:109-123. 1973 Organizational Variability inLate Paleoindian
Anthropologist
Assemblages. Ph.D. diss., Dept. of Anthropol
ogy,Washington State University, Pullman.

321

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PLAINS ANTHROPOLOGIST [Vol. 36,No. 137,1991]

Krieger, A. Warnica, J.
1947 Artifacts from the Plainview Bison Bed. Bul 1961 The Elida Site: Evidenc of a Folsom Occupa
letinof theGeological SocietyofAmerica 58: tion in the Roosevelt County, Eastern New
938-954. Mexico. Bulletin of the Texas Archaeological
Socitety 30:209-215.
McCartney, P.
1990 Alternative Hunting Strategies in Plains Warnica, J., and T. Williamson
Paleoindian Adaptations. In Hunters of the 1968 The Milnesand Site-Revisited. Transactions
Recent Past, edited by L. Davis and B. Reeves, of theRegionalArchaeologicalSymposium
for
111-121. Unwin Hyman, Ltd., London. Southeastern New Mexico and Western Texas 3:
21-22.
Reher, C, and G. Frison
1980 The Vore Site,48CK302:A Stratified
Buffalo Wendorf, F., A. Krieger, C. Albritton, and T. Stewart

Jump in theWyoming Black Hills. Plains 1955 TheMidland Discovery.Universityof Texas


Antliropologist 25(88, pt. 2), Memoir 16. Press, Austin.

Satterthwaite, L. Wheat, J. B.
1957 Stone Artifacts at and Near theFinley Site, Near 1972 The Olsen-Chubbock Site: A Paleoindian
Eden, Wyoming. University Museum, Univer Bison Kill. Society for American Archaeology
sity of Pennsylvania. 37(1,pt. 2),Memoir 26.
Sellards, E. 1979 The Jurgens Site. Plains Anthropologist 24(84,
1952 of Texas pt. 2), Memoir 15.
Early Man in America. University
Press, Austin.
Wilmsen, E., and F. Roberts
1955 Fossil Bison and Associated Artifacts fromMil 1978 Lindenmeier, 1934-1974: Concluding Report on
nesand, New Mexico. American Antiquity 20: Investigations. Smithsonian Contributions in
336-344. Anthropology No. 24, Smithsonian Institution,
Washington, D.C.
Sellards, E., G. Evans, and G. Meade
1947 Fossil Bison and Associated Artifacts from Wormington, H.
Plainview, Texas. Bulletin of the Geological 1957 Ancient Man in North America. Denver
Museum of Natural History, Popular Series No.
Society ofAmerica 58: 927-938.
4, Denver.
Sollberger, J.
1985 A Technique for Fluting Folsom. Lithic Tech Young, D., and R. Bonnichsen
nology 14: 41-50. 1984 Understanding Stone Tools: A Cognitive Ap
proach. Center for the Study of Early Man. The
D.
Thomas, University of Maine, Orono.
1986 Points on Points: A Reply to Flenniken and
Raymond. American Antiquity 51: 619-628. Department ofAnthropology
Tunnell, C. Campus Box 233
1977 FlutedPoint Production as Revealed by Lithic University of Colorado at Boulder
from the Adair-Steadman Site in
Specimens Boulder, Colorado 80309-0233
Northwest Texas. Museum Journal 17:140-168.

March 1991

322

This content downloaded from 162.38.186.136 on Tue, 2 Sep 2014 22:33:57 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like