You are on page 1of 10

INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD, SEPTEMBER–OCTOBER 2012,

VOL. 47, NO. 5, 609–617

Short Report
Social–communicative effects of the Picture Exchange Communication
System (PECS) in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Anna Lerna†, Dalila Esposito†, Massimiliano Conson‡, Luigi Russo† and Angelo Massagli†
†Scientific Institute I.R.C.C.S. “Eugenio Medea" Regional Branch of Ostuni, Brindisi Department of Neurorehabilitation 2,
Child Psychiatry, Brindisi, Italy
‡Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Second University of Naples, Caserta, Italy

(Received February 2012; accepted May 2012)

Abstract
Background: The Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) is a common treatment choice for non-
verbal children with autism. However, little empirical evidence is available on the usefulness of PECS in treating
social–communication impairments in autism.
Aims: To test the effects of PECS on social–communicative skills in children with autism, concurrently taking
into account standardized psychometric data, standardized functional assessment of adaptive behaviour, and
information on social–communicative variables coded in an unstructured setting.
Methods & Procedures: Eighteen preschool children (mean age = 38.78 months) were assigned to two intervention
approaches, i.e. PECS and Conventional Language Therapy (CLT). Both PECS (Phases I–IV) and CLT were
delivered three times per week, in 30-min sessions, for 6 months. Outcome measures were the following: Autism
Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) domain scores for Communication and Reciprocal Social Interaction;
Language and Personal–Social subscales of the Griffiths’ Mental Developmental Scales (GMDS); Communication
and Social Abilities domains of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS); and several social–communicative
variables coded in an unstructured setting.
Outcomes & Results: Results demonstrated that the two groups did not differ at Time 1 (pre-treatment assessment),
whereas at Time 2 (post-test) the PECS group showed a significant improvement with respect to the CLT group
on the VABS social domain score and on almost all the social–communicative abilities coded in the unstructured
setting (i.e. joint attention, request, initiation, cooperative play, but not eye contact).
Conclusions & Implications: These findings showed that PECS intervention (Phases I–IV) can improve social–
communicative skills in children with autism. This improvement is especially evident in standardized measures of
adaptive behaviour and measures derived from the observation of children in an unstructured setting.

Keywords: Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS), autism, intervention, social and communicative
skills.

What is already known on this subject


The Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) is an augmentative communication system frequently used
with non-verbal children with autism. PECS can facilitate functional communication, improve severe impairments
of functional speech, decrease the behaviors problem. Only a small number of studies suggest positive effects in the
social domain.
What this study adds
This paper shows the possibility of using PECS for improving socio-communicative skills in autistic children.
An experimentally controlled group investigation was conducted using data collected with formalized measures of
communication and social functioning and with behaviour observation of participants in an unstructured setting.
Outcomes measures after PECS training suggest that this intervention (based on the “physical exchange” within an
interactive context) can represent an effective tool for promoting improvement of social-communicative skills.

Address correspondence to: Angelo Massagli, Department of Neurorehabilitation 2, Child Psychiatry Scientific Institute I.R.C.C.S.
‘Eugenio Medea’, Regional Branch of Ostuni–Brindisi, Di Summa Square, I-72100 Brindisi, Italy; e-mails: angelo.massagli@os.lnf.it and
angelo.massagli@tin.it
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online  c 2012 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists
DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00172.x
610 Anna Lerna et al.
Introduction impairments of functional speech (Flippin et al. 2010),
Communication and social impairments appearing only a small number of studies suggest positive effects
early in life and persisting into adulthood are core in the social domain (Preston and Carter 2009).
features of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD; American In a study on three individuals with autism,
Psychiatric Association (APA) 2000, Howlin et al. Charlop-Christy et al. (2002) demonstrated positive
2004). In the past, rehabilitation interventions aimed treatment-related increases in speech and different
at improving language and communication in children social–communicative behaviours, especially initiations,
with ASD mainly focused on verbal production through requests and joint attention. Accordingly, in a single case
response training, while neglecting that, unlike non- study Kravits et al. (2002) reported some increase in
verbal training, this method did not influence communi- duration of social interaction with peers and provided a
cation skills (Howlin 1998). At present, successful demonstration of the effectiveness of PECS in increasing
rehabilitation strategies of non-verbal autism are centred spontaneous communication skills.
on functional spontaneous communication. This has In a randomized control study, Yoder and Stone
favoured implementation of Alternative Augmentative (2006) compared the effect of PECS with that of
Communication (AAC) systems devoted to the develop- another communicative intervention (i.e. Responsive
ment of non-verbal communication by using signs, Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching—RPMT),
pictures and symbols (Howlin 1998, Howlin et al. 2007, on initiating joint attention, requesting and turn-taking,
Mirenda 2003, Schuler et al. 1997). Various forms and found that RPMT facilitated the frequency of
of AAC have been devised. These include the use of generalized turn-taking and generalized initiating joint
manual signs, voice output communication devices and attention more than the PECS did, whereas the PECS
various picture-based systems (for a recent meta-analysis had a superior effect to generalized request in children
on AAC in ASD, see Ganz et al. 2011). The Picture with initially low rates of initiating joint attention. Carr
Exchange Communication System (PECS), originally and Felce (2007) focused on spontaneous communica-
developed by Bondy and Frost (1994), is a picture- tive initiation following PECS training and found
based AAC system that was specifically designed for significant increases in initiation and dyadic interac-
non-verbal children with ASD (for recent reviews, see tion between children with autism and their teachers.
Flippin et al. 2010, and Ganz et al. 2011). PECS Analogously, in another randomized control study
uses basic behavioural principles, particularly reinforce- Howlin et al. (2007) investigated potential effects
ment techniques, to teach children to use functional of PECS training on spontaneous communication of
communication in a social interaction context (Bondy children with ASD in a school setting and found that
and Frost 1994, 1998). rates of communicative initiations and PECS usage were
The PECS protocol includes six instructional phases significantly increased immediately following interven-
(Bondy and Frost 1998): Phases I and II include teaching tion (also Gordon et al. 2011). The authors also
the student to exchange pictures for preferred items examined ADOS-G (Lord et al. 2000) domain scores
and activities (Phase II expands on Phase I by teaching for communication and reciprocal social interaction,
generalization across distance, communicative partners and only observed a specific decrease in the severity
and a variety of items and pictures); in Phase III score for reciprocal social interaction at the 10-month
students learn to discriminate between preferred and follow-up.
non-preferred pictures and eventually between a variety This review shows that only three group studies
of preferred items; in Phase IV students are taught are available on social–communicative effects of PECS
to make requests using complete sentences; Phase V training in ASD. In two studies a specific analysis
involves instruction in answering questions; and in the was performed on behavioural measures derived
final phase, Phase VI, students learn to make a variety of from observation of participants in an unstructured
comments. Thus, the first four phases focus on teaching setting, without providing a systematic assessment with
the child to use a symbol to make a request, while the standardized tests (Carr and Felce 2007, Yoder and
final two phases expand the range of communicative Stone 2006). In the third study, a less deep behavioural
functions used by the child. observation has been combined with standardized tests
Despite the fact that PECS is a popular specifically assessing language and only one measure of
communication-training programme for non-verbal social–communicative abilities (i.e. ADOS Communi-
children with autism (Flippin et al. 2010, Howlin et al. cation and Reciprocal Social Interaction domains;
2007), only a few empirical studies are available on its Howlin et al. 2007). Recently, Preston and Carter (2009)
efficacy in treating social–communication impairments claimed that investigations on social–communicative
in autism. More precisely, while several investiga- effects of PECS training should concurrently take
tions indicated that PECS can provide a viable means into account standardized psychometric data, standard-
to facilitate communication and to improve severe ized functional assessment of adaptive behaviour, and
Social–communicative effects of PECS in ASD 611
information on social–communicative variables coded Table 1. Means and standard deviations (SD) of demographic
in an unstructured setting. On this basis, in the present variables, GMDS Non-verbal IQ and ADOS Total score,
separately for the two groups
study we conducted an experimentally controlled group
investigation to test the effects of the first four PECS PECS CLT
phases on social–communicative behaviours of children Variable Mean SD Mean SD
with ASD. To this aim, we collected psychometric
data and functional measures of adaptive behaviour Chronological age (months) 38.78 7.43 41.11 8.67
Mother’s education (years) 13.56 3.00 11.89 2.21
by means of standardized tasks, and evaluated social– Father’s education (years) 11.67 3.84 10.44 3.17
communicative skills in an unstructured setting through GMDS Non-verbal IQ 73.56 18.15 68.56 16.97
observation of children’s free-play interactions with an ADOS Total score 16.78 4.06 16.11 3.86
adult.

Methods tion, whereas the control group received a Conventional


Language Therapy (CLT). The two groups were defined
Participants by territory, that is a 30–50-mile limit was set as a
The participants were all recruited from ‘Eugenio viable distance for the parents to travel to reach the
Medea’ Scientific Institute, Regional Branch of Ostuni ‘Eugenio Medea’ Institute where PECS training was
(BR), Italy, an institute for diagnosis and rehabilita- implemented. Therefore, all the children in this area
tion of developmental disorders. For inclusion in the who met the inclusion criteria for participation were
study each child was required: (1) to have a diagnosis of included in the intervention group, whereas children
autism; (2) to be aged between 18 and 60 months; (3) to outside the 30–50-mile limit who met inclusion criteria
have little or no functional language (spontaneous use were recruited for the control group. The control group
of fewer than five words according to parent report, received CLT training in other rehabilitation structures
defined on the basis of clinical observation and the accredited with the Italian National Health System
MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory; placed within a short distance from their home.
Fenson et al. 1993), (4) not to be using PECS or All pre-test measurements were given to partici-
other ACC systems, and (5) to have no evidence of pants before the intervention sessions were initiated.
severe sensory, motor deficits or of known co-morbid Table 1 presents descriptive information of the sample,
medical conditions. Diagnosis of autism was based on separately for the two groups.
the following criteria: (a) a formal clinical diagnosis
made by a child psychiatrist and a clinical psychol- Procedure
ogist according to the criteria of the DSM-IV; and
(b) results from the Autism Diagnostic Observation At entry into the study (Time 1), children received four
Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al. 1999) administered by pre-treatment assessments (see the Outcome Measures
trained and certified examiners. section below). The treatment phase lasted 6 months,
Thirty children were screened for participation in and at the end of this period the four pre-treatment
the study between September 2010 and February 2011, measures were repeated (Time 2). All assessment
and 18 children fitted the inclusion criteria, so that procedures at both time points were conducted by
the total sample size was 18. All children were from examiners who were blind to the purpose of the study
southern Italy; 17 male and one female. The mean and to group assignment of the participants. Evaluation
non-verbal IQ (based on level performance of Griffiths’ took place in a building that was different from the
Mental Developmental Scales) was 71.1 (SD = 17.2) therapy building, and used materials and activities that
and ranged from 44 to 98 points. Mean production were not used in therapy sessions.
score on the MacArthur Communicative Development
Inventory was 1.72 (SD = 2.26), and total score on Outcome measures
the ADOS was 16.52 (SD = 3.76). The mean formal
educational level of the primary parent was 12.7 years The efficacy of treatment was assessed via a battery
(SD = 2.7) for mother and 11.1 (SD = 3.5) for father. of standardized assessments of social–communicative
Participants were assigned either to the experimen- abilities. In addition, several behavioural measures were
tal or to the control group after parents had provided derived from observation of a free-play session with an
written informed consent. However, parents were examiner. The following measures were included.
unaware of the precise goals of the study and of the
specific children’s skills that were targets of the interven- • Griffiths’ Mental Developmental Scales (GMDS;
tion. The experimental group received PECS interven- Griffiths 1984): language subscale, assessing
612 Anna Lerna et al.
Table 2. Dependent measures and operational definitions for behaviours observed in the UFPE

Dependent measure Operational definition


Cooperative play Child engaged in the same activity (e.g. basketball, puzzles, board games) with the therapist for at least 10 s
Eye contact Child looked into therapist’s eyes for 2 s
Joint attention (a) Observing same object for 5 s
(b) Referential look between therapist and an object
(c) Established common attention focus with therapist
Requests Labelling object while pointing or reaching for it, handing the therapist PECS cards, verbal requests
Initiation Independent approach to therapist with vocal or non-vocal attempt to engage in conversation or play (e.g. pulling on
therapist’s shirt to get attention, play, handing therapist a toy)

receptive and expressive language, and personal– (version XT.7; Noldus Information Technology 2007)
social subscale, assessing proficiency in the software package to transform a standard computer
activities of daily living, level of independence and keyboard into a point and state events recorder. The
interaction with other children, were employed as point events were behaviours that only take an instant
outcome measures. in time, or whose duration was not important, i.e. joint
• Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale (ADOS; Lord attention, requests and initiation; these variables were
et al. 1999). Communication and Reciprocal coded in terms of frequency. The state events were
Social Interaction domain scores were used to behaviours that take a period of time and therefore
assess change over time. have a duration, i.e. cooperative play, eye contact; these
• Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition variables were coded in seconds. The duration of the
(VABS; Sparrow et al. 2005). VABS were used to session was exactly 15 min for all children; therefore
gather parent report of child communication and no correction for the length of the observation was
social abilities at home and in the community. necessary.
• Unstructured Free-Play with Examiner (UFPE). Reliability of coded variables was analysed on
Following previous studies on social– independently coded and randomly selected samples.
communicative effects of PECS (Charlop-Christy Two independent observers were trained to use the
et al. 2002, Yoder and Stone 2006) a 15-min coding system developed for the purposes of this study.
session of UFPE was planned in order to collect Each of the observers had a Master’s degree in clinical
data on specific variables, i.e. cooperative play, eye or developmental psychology and received training by
contact, joint attention, requests and initiation. an experienced clinician using video samples of children
The operational definition of these variables was not involved in the study. The observers (coders) were
based on Charlop-Christy et al.’s (2002) criteria blind to the purposes of the study and the treatment
(table 2). UFPE was conducted in a room with assignment. Inter-rater reliability was determined by
a variety of age appropriate toys or objects (i.e. double coding of 20% of the observations and was very
bubbles, balloons, stuffed and rubber animals, good (Kappa = 0.87).
female baby doll, building blocks, wind up car,
electronic toys, a small table and two chairs)
accessible to the children. The PECS book was Treatments
present in pre- and post-treatment and both PECS and CLT were implemented within a psycho-
groups had the possibility to use it. The examiner education rehabilitation programme based on the
played with the same or similar toy as the child TEACCH methodology (Mesibov et al. 2004).
by imitating the child’s play. If the child did not All children followed a structured teaching for a
attend to any toy for 10 s, then the examiner total 12 h a week; children were taught skills
selected an interesting object and used it in a way including attention, basic discrimination, language and
that was suitable to the cognitive level of the child. spontaneous communication, daily living, socialization,
Examiners verbally commented on the child’s or play, fine and gross motor control and pre-academics
their own actions, but they were instructed not to (Watson et al. 1989). A semi-structured setting was
use any type of communication prompt, such as also implemented, using incidental teaching techniques,
time delays, questions or gestural prompts. to enhance generalization, increase motivation, develop
social skills and to decrease problem behaviours.
Social–communicative behaviours coded during the Treatments sessions took place in a therapeutic
UFPE session were derived from videotaped records room equipped with small desks, chairs, familiar and
of the free-play session using the Observer Video Pro unfamiliar objects and toys; children were offered 30-
Social–communicative effects of PECS in ASD 613
Table 3. Brief description of the PECS phases implemented in the study

Phase Description
I: Picture exchange The child is taught to approach a communicative partner, give him or her a picture card, and receive a
preferred item
II: Increasing distance The child is taught to retrieve a picture from his PECS board, which had been moved away from him,
walk to a communicative partner and persist in handing the picture to the communicative partner,
who had moved across the room
III: Picture discrimination The child is taught to discriminate among multiple pictures on the PECS board. At the end of this
phase the child can choose from six or more preferred and non-preferred items
IV: Sentence structure The child creates a ‘sentence’ on the sentence strip by combining the ‘I want card’ and the card of a
desired item, seeks out the communicative partner, and gives him or her the sentence strip

min individual therapy sessions (PECS or CLT) three reinforcements. In Italy it represents one of the
times a week for 6 months (i.e. 72 sessions). most widely employed methods to treat language and
Experts leading both PECS and CLT were special- communicative disorders in developmental disabili-
ists in speech–language pathology with extensive ties in accordance with the guidelines of the Italian
expertise with ASD. As specifically regards PECS, National Health System. CLT employs a variety of
the trainer attended two workshops about PECS largely operant approaches to language intervention
basic and advanced training provided by an expert implemented in accordance with discrete trial training
consultant of Pyramid Educational Consultants UK, formats (Goldstein 2002, Lovaas 1981, 2002): the child
following the highly prescribed format of the training is taught to attend to adults and respond to simple
manual (Frost and Bondy 2002), and received a instructions (receptive language training); she/he has to
certificate of attendance at the conclusion of each first imitate manual, oral-motor and vocal behaviour,
workshop. and then to imitate speech. Association learning is
Phases I–IV of PECS were implemented according used for teaching increasingly sophisticated expressive
to the standard procedures devised by Bondy and Frost language skills, and motivation is favoured through use
(1998); table 3 provides a brief description of each the of various external rewards. CLT uses didactic, adult-
four PECS phases. Before beginning of the training, directed instructions in a one-to-one interaction: each
parents were required to record their children’s favourite session begins by presenting the child with his own
food and toys on a card in order to make picture cards preferred items. If the child shows motivation to obtain
to be included in the communication book, according the item (e.g. reaching for it), the therapist waits for
to the standard procedure. a few seconds to provide the opportunity for the child
The percentage of correct requests per session and to say the word independently. If the child does not
the number of sessions per criterion were measured produce the word, the therapist repeats the name of
for each of the four PECS phases. Observations were the object three times with time delay. When the child
carried out independently by two autism specialists, spontaneously names or imitates the name, or produces
speech-and-language therapists, one of which was the a word approximation, she/he receives the desired
PECS trainer. There was 100% agreement regarding the item.
PECS level between the two assessors. The criterion for
advancement within a phase was set at 80% independent
on symbol exchanges made during PECS instruction. Results
Training within each phase of PECS was continued until Standardized test assessment
all children were able to demonstrate independently a
correct response score of 80% or higher for a minimum We investigated group differences before treatment. To
of three consecutive sessions, in accordance with the this aim, a MANOVA was performed with demographic
criteria outlined in the PECS training manual (Bondy characteristics and mean standard scores on formalized
and Frost 1994). The mean number of PECS sessions tests at Time 1 (tables 1 and 4) as dependent variables
for each phase was 6.3 (SD = 2.3); within these sessions, and group as independent variable. Results showed
children were provided with a mean number of 91 that there were no significant group differences in
(SD = 38.9) individual trials. demographic data and Time 1 formalized tests scores (all
Conventional Language Therapy (CLT) is a p > 0.05), thus demonstrating that the two groups were
language training based on a systematic, step-by- well matched with respect to pre-treatment measures
step teaching technique using prompts and useful and that no covariate was needed to analyse group
differences on Time 2 measures.
614 Anna Lerna et al.
Table 4. Means and standard deviations (SD) of standard scores on formalized outcome measures at Times 1 and 2 separately for the
two groups

Time 1 Time 2
PECS CLT PECS CLT
Variable Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
GMDS
Language 35.00 7.98 36.00 13.18 38.44 14.70 36.67 17.43
Social 61.78 8.00 53.56 12.03 63.00 8.41 55.22 11.17
ADOS
Communication 6.44 1.74 6.22 2.39 5.89 3.44 6.11 2.09
Reciprocal Social Interaction 10.11 3.02 10.22 1.99 9.56 2.40 9.89 1.76
VABS
Communication 56.33 13.20 55.67 10.79 64.89 8.64 60.56 17.08
Social 64.56 6.95 60.00 3.46 82.67 19.91 60.78 3.67

Table 5. Means and standard deviations (SD) of behavioural outcome measures coded during free-play at Times 1 and 2 separately
for the two groups

Time 1 Time 2
PECS CLT PECS CLT
Variable Measure Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Cooperative play Duration 40.56 30.06 32.56 36.41 209.78 131.72 74.89 63.73
Eye contact Duration 4.00 4.18 9.11 16.21 19.22 21.22 26.00 39.46
Joint attention Frequency 1.00 1.12 1.11 2.03 4.11 3.86 0.44 0.53
Requests Frequency 1.78 2.22 3.44 2.35 6.44 4.72 2.67 2.12
Initiation Frequency 0.22 0.44 0.67 1.41 3.67 4.00 0 0

A MANOVA was performed with mean standard (table 5) showed significant group differences in
scores on formalized outcome measures at Time 2 cooperative play, F (1, 16) = 7.648, p = 0.014,
(table 4) as dependent variables and group as η2 p = 0.323, joint attention, F (1, 16) = 7.993,
independent variable. Results showed significant group p = 0.012, η2 p = 0.333, requests, F (1, 16) = 4.797,
differences on VABS—Social, F (1, 16) = 10.519, p = 0.044, η2 p = 0.2312, and initiation, F (1,
p = 0.005, η2 p = 0.397, but not on all the other Time 16) = 7.562, p = 0.014, η2 p = 0.321, but not in eye
2 measures (all p > 0.05). contact (p > 0.05).
A within-group comparison between Times 1 and A within group comparison between Times 1 and
2 scores was performed separately in PECS and CLT 2 scores was performed separately in PECS and CLT
group. As regards the PECS group, paired t-tests showed group. As regards the PECS group, paired t-tests showed
a significant improvement of scores on both VABS— a general improvement in all the five coded variables
Communication (t(8) = –2.484, p = 0.038) and (cooperative play, t(8) = –3.941, p = 0.004, eye contact,
VABS—Social (t(8) = –3.331, p = 0.010) domains, but t(8) = –2.369, p = 0.045, joint attention, t(8) = –
not on the other measures (all p > 0.050). The same 2.865, p = 0.021, requests, t(8) = –2.682, p = 0.028,
analysis performed on the CLT group did not reveal and initiation, t(8) = –2.486, p = 0.038). The same
significant difference on any of the considered measures analysis performed on the CLT group did not reveal
(all p > 0.050). significant difference on any of the considered measures
(all p > 0.05).

Unstructured free-play session


Discussion
A MANOVA was performed with behavioural
outcome measures coded during free-play at Time 1 The present study investigated the effects of PECS on
(table 5) as dependent variables and group as indepen- social–communicative abilities of children with ASD by
dent variable. Results showed that at Time 1 there collecting data from formalized measures of communi-
were no significant group differences in the behavioural cation and social functioning and from observation
measures coded during free-play (all p > 0.05). In of participants’ behaviour in an unstructured setting.
contrast, the MANOVA performed on Time 2 measures A group of children with ASD receiving PECS were
Social–communicative effects of PECS in ASD 615
compared with another group of children receiving CLT. important consideration is that the children with autism
The two groups were well matched on demographic and are less motivated by social effects of communication,
pre-treatment outcome measures, thus allowing to relate such as praise, shared attention or dyadic interaction,
post-treatment differences to the specificity of group than they are to its concrete effects, such as obtaining a
intervention. Between group comparisons performed on desired item (e.g. Bondy and Frost 1998). Motivation
formalized tests scores at Time 2 showed that children to initiate social–communicative skills could occur in
receiving PECS gained significantly higher scores than response to internal signals of interest or positive affect,
the CLT group on the VABS social domain. The so there is reason to believe that children can learn
between group comparison on social–communicative new communicative behaviours, such as requesting or
measures coded during free-play revealed that frequency turn taking (Yoder and Stone 2006), and later generalize
of joint attention, requests and initiation, and duration them to social–communicative skills such as initiating
of cooperative play but not of eye contact were signifi- joint attention. Starting from the concept of physical
cantly higher in the PECS than the CLT group. These exchange within an interactive context (Bondy and Frost
results were confirmed by within-group comparisons 1994), we speculated that motivation to interact with
between Times 1 and 2 showing in the PECS group, a partner could be first triggered in the children by the
but not in the CLT group, significant improvements on activation of the desire to reach for a concrete target by
VABS communication and social domains, and in all the means of a social interaction and, only subsequently, by
measures coded during free-play. These results provide the desire to share an experience related to objects with
support to the existence of a close relationship between a social partner (e.g. to carry out a game).
communication and social skills (Charlop-Christy et al. It remains to be elucidated why in the PECS group
2002). only one social–communicative variable coded in free-
Several lines of evidence suggest that improvement play, i.e. eye contact, did not improve from pre- to
in the frequency of requests might trigger an increase of post-treatment. Although at first sight this finding
joint attention (Tomasello et al. 2007). In a recent meta- seems at odds with improvement of the PECS group
analysis reviewing data on PECS effects on communi- in joint attention, it could be explained by taking
cation and speech in children with ASD, Flippin into account the distinction between two different
et al. (2010) underlined that one of the main potential kinds of joint attention. The child’s ability to follow
pre-treatment identifiers of a child with autism who gaze direction, head turns and/or pointing gesture of
is likely to respond to PECS intervention is limited another individual may be referred to as responding joint
joint attention. Accordingly, Yoder and Stone (2006) attention, whereas the child’s ability in using eye contact
suggested the PECS may be a communication interven- and/or deictic gestures (e.g. pointing or showing) to
tion more effective in children with initially low rates of initiate spontaneously coordinated attention with a
joint attention. The authors performed an exploratory social partner may be referred to as initiating joint
analysis of pre-treatment variables and found that initial attention (Mundy 2003). In this respect, the variable we
rates of joint attention were a strong predictor of labelled here as joint attention corresponded to respond-
whether a child made greater communication (i.e. ing joint attention, whereas eye contact corresponded to
requesting) gains. Joint attention also emerged as a initiating joint attention. These two aspects of joint
potentially influential pre-treatment characteristic in a attention can dissociate between each other, because
single-subject study by Charlop-Christy et al. (2002) they reflect different processes of social and emotional
which documented that the largest amelioration was functioning (Mundy et al. 2000). Actually, initiat-
observed in the youngest participant, who took part ing joint attention reflects the tendency to initiate
to the study with no spontaneous speech, weak joint spontaneously a social interaction, whereas respond-
attention, eye contact and toy play. Although analysis ing joint attention is a measure of the tendency to
of pre-treatment characteristics of children undergoing respond to another person’s signal to shift attention.
PECS intervention was outside the aims of the present It has been demonstrated that initiating joint attention
study, the findings are in line with data from literature can be affected by executive and motivation defects more
reviewed above and strengthen the idea that PECS is a than responding joint attention (Mundy et al. 2000). In
valuable tool for enhancing social–communicative skills particular, initiating joint attention seems to involve the
of children with ASD. It is possible to suggest that tendency to initiate spontaneously episodes of sharing
the ‘physical exchange’ (i.e. the communicative act to affective experience related to objects or events with
receive a concrete item) within an interactive context a social partner (Mundy et al. 1992); in this sense,
(to establish a physical contact with a partner prior to a difficulty with initiating joint attention represents a
emitting a referential communicative; Bondy and Frost core symptom of autism (Mundy 2003). Results from
1994) is a key feature of PECS intervention favouring the present study showing a dissociation between PECS-
children’s communication and social interaction. An related effects on eye contact and joint attention closely
616 Anna Lerna et al.
fit the distinction between these two types of joint cation and reciprocal social interaction. However, a
attention, revealing that PECS intervention can specifi- specific decrease in the severity score for reciprocal social
cally influence responding joint attention. interaction was reported at the 10-month follow-up.
One strength of the present study is represented by Therefore, it could be useful in future studies to repeat
our assessing children in both structured and unstruc- these assessment after a longer interval of time within a
tured settings. This allowed us to verify whether a longitudinal protocol.
best outcome measure is available to detect PECS- Limitations of the present study include the lack
related changes in the child’s behaviour and to verify of randomization of treatments and the relatively
the presence of possible relationships between different sample size which, however, although encourage future
measures. We found that while almost all variables randomized trials with larger sample sizes, do not
coded in free-play reflected the positive effects of PECS undermine reliability of the results. Moreover, we did
treatment, among the formalized measures only the not collect observational data on dyadic interactions
VABS social domain score demonstrated an advantage between the children and the adults in their everyday
for the PECS group. A specific consideration should environment. While this is a post-hoc consideration
be made on the difference between communicative and prompted by the fact that the results revealed a major
social abilities assessed with the VABS, and communica- improvement of children’s social–communicative skills
tive and social disabilities measured with the ADOS. The in an ecological context, it would be useful to investigate
ADOS quantifies social and communicative behaviours further this aspect following data of recent research (Carr
that are relevant to the diagnosis of autism; the and Felce 2007, Howlin et al. 2007). For instance, Carr
‘algorithm’ for the specific domains of the ADOS and Felce (2007) demonstrated significant PECS-related
has been commonly used for characterization of the improvements in the dyadic patterns of communication
level of autistic symptomatology (Lord et al. 1999, between children and teachers in the classroom. Finally,
Klin et al. 2007). The VABS in contrast represents a another aspect that merits comment regards the effect
measure of personal and social self-sufficiency obtained of PECS on language abilities. Although the present
through a semi-structured interview with the person’s study did not demonstrate any increase in children’s
parents or caregivers, and it examines the adaptive language abilities, this negative result could be related
functioning in everyday life. The data underlining a to the nature of measures employed here; we mainly
dissociation between abilities (VABS) and disabilities focused interest on social–communicative functioning
(ADOS) in the communication and social domains rather than on language abilities (for instance, in free-
are fully consistent with these differences between the play no language assessment was performed).
two measures (Klin et al. 2007). Therefore, we did In conclusion, the present results showed a
not find any relationship between higher levels of significant effect of PECS (Phases I–IV) on social–
adaptive skills and lower levels of symptomatology; thus, communicative abilities of children with ASD which
adaptive skills (abilities) and symptomatology (disabil- underwent extensive assessment including formalized
ities) might have to be conceived, at least partially, tests and observation during free-play. Outcomes
as independent domains of intervention. As Klin measures demonstrated that the first four phases of
et al. (2007) suggested, factors mediating the acquisi- the PECS intervention can represent an effective
tion of adaptive skills may be different from factors tool for promoting improvement of several social–
mediating the expression of autistic symptomatology. communicative skills.
Related to this difference between abilities and disabil-
ities assesses by VABS and ADOS, respectively, is the References
tentative explanation of another aspect of the present
AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION (APA), 2000, Diagnostic and
findings, namely that the severity of symptomatology Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn, text revn
measured by ADOS remained stable after 6 months, in (Washington, DC: APA).
contrast to the improvement of socio-communicative BONDY, A. S. and FROST, L. A., 1994, PECS: The Picture Exchange
abilities after PECS training measured by VABS. One Communication System Training Manual (Cherry Hill, NJ:
could suggest that VABS are sensitive to day-by-day Pyramid Educational Consultants).
BONDY, A. S. and FROST, L. A., 1998, The Picture Exchange
improvement over a short-term period of 6 months, Communication System. Seminars in Speech and Language,
while the ADOS detects more global changes over a 19, 373–389.
longer period. This would be consistent with Howlin CARR, D. and FELCE, J., 2007, The effects of PECS teaching to phase
et al.’s (2007) results showing that rates of communica- III on the communicative interactions between children with
tive initiations and PECS usage were significantly autism and their teachers. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 37, 724–737.
increased after PECS training, whereas the authors CHARLOP-CHRISTY, M. H., CARPENTER, M., LEBLANC, L. and
did not find significant changes immediately following KELLET, K., 2002, Using the picture exchange communi-
treatment on ADOS-G domain scores for communi- cation system (PECS) with children with autism: assessment
Social–communicative effects of PECS in ASD 617
of PECS acquisition, speech, social–communicative behavior, LORD, C., RISI, S., LAMBRECHT, L., COOK, E. H. Jr., LEVENTHAL,
and problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, B. L., DILAVORE, P. C., PICKLES, A., RUTTER, M., 2000,
35, 213–231. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic:
FENSON, L., DALE, P. S., REZNICK, J. S. THAL, D., BATES, E., a standard measure of social and communication deficits
HARTUNG, J. P., PETICK, S. and REILLY, J. S., 1993, The associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism
MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.
Guide and Technical Manual (San Diego, CA: Singular). LORD, C., RUTTER, M., DILAVORE, P. C. and RISI, S., 1999, Autism
FLIPPIN, M., RESZKA, S. and WATSON, L. R., 2010, Effectiveness Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Los Angeles, CA: Western
of the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) Psychological Services).
on communication and speech for children with Autism LOVAAS, O. I., 1981, Teaching Developmentally Disabled Children
Spectrum Disorders: a meta-analysis. American Journal of (Baltimore, MD: University Park Press).
Speech–Language Pathology, 19, 178–195. LOVAAS, O. I., 2002, Teaching Individuals with Developmental Delays:
FROST, L. A. and BONDY, A. S., 2002, PECS: The Picture Exchange Basic Intervention Techniques: Autism (Austin, TX: PRO-ED).
Communication System Training Manual, 2nd edn (Cherry MESIBOV, G. B., SHEA, V. and SCHOPLER, E., 2004, The TEACCH
Hill, NJ: Pyramid Educational Consultants). Approach to Autism Spectrum Disorders (Berlin: Springer).
GANZ, J. B., EARLES-VOLLRATH, T. L., HEATH, A. K., PARKER, R. I., MIRENDA, P., 2003, Towards functional augmentative and alterna-
RISPOLI, M. J. and DURAN, J. B., 2011, A meta-analysis of tive communication for students with autism: manual signs,
single case research studies on aided augmentative and alterna- graphic symbols, and voice output communication aids.
tive communication systems with individuals with Autism Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 203–
Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental 216.
Disorders, 42, 60–74. MUNDY, P., 2003, Annotation: the neural basis of social impairments
GOLDSTEIN, H., 2002, Communication intervention for children in autism: the role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and
with autism: a review of treatment efficacy. Journal of Autism anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child Psychology and
and Developmental Disorders, 32, 373–396. Psychiatry, 44, 793–809.
GORDON, K., PASCO, G., MCELDUFF, F., WADE, A., HOWLIN, P. MUNDY, P., CARD, J. and FOX, N., 2000, EEG correlates of the
and CHARMAN, T., 2011, A communication-based interven- development of infant joint attention skills. Developmental
tion for nonverbal children with autism: what changes? Who Psychobiology, 36, 325–338.
benefits? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, MUNDY, P., KASARI, C. and SIGMAN, M., 1992, Joint attention,
447–457. affective sharing, and intersubjectivity. Infant Behavior and
GRIFFITHS, R., 1984, Griffiths’ Mental Developmental Scales (GMDS). Development, 15, 377–381.
The 1996 Revision by M. Huntley (Oxford: Hogrefe, The Test NOLDUS INFORMATION TECHNOLOGY, 2007, The Observer XT, 2007
Agency). (Wageningen: Noldus Information Technology).
HOWLIN, P., 1998, Treating Children with Autism and Asperger PRESTON, D. and CARTER, M., 2009, A review of the efficacy of
Syndrome: A Guide for Parents and Professional (Chichester: the Picture Exchange Communication System intervention.
Wiley). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1471–
HOWLIN, P., GOODE, S., HUTTON, J. and RUTTER, M., 2004, Adult 1486.
outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology SCHULER, A. L., PRIZANT, B. M. and WETHERBY, A. M.,
and Psychiatry, 45, 212–229. 1997, Enhancing language and communication develop-
HOWLIN, P., GORDON, R. K., PASCO, G., WADE, A. and CHARMAN, ment: prelinguistic approaches. In D. J. Cohen and F. R.
T., 2007, The effectiveness of Picture Exchange Communi- Volkmar (eds), Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Develop-
cation System (PECS) training for teachers of children with mental Disorders (New York, NY: Wiley), pp. 539–571.
autism: a pragmatic, group randomised controlled trial. SPARROW, S. S., CICCHETTI, D. V. and BALLA, D. A., 2005, Vineland
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 473–481. Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition (Vineland—II) Survey
KLIN, A., SAULNIER, C. A., SPARROW, S. S., CICCHETTI, D. V., Forms Manual (Circle Pines, MN: AGS).
VOLKMAR, F. R. and LORD, C., 2007, Social and communi- TOMASELLO, M., CARPENTER, M. and LISZKOWSKI, U., 2007, A
cation abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individ- new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–
uals with Autism Spectrum Disorders: the Vineland and the 722.
ADOS. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 37, 748– WATSON, L. R., LORD, C., SCHAFFER, B. and SCHOPLER, E., 1989,
759. Teaching Spontaneous Communication to Autistic and Develop-
KRAVITS, T. R., KAMPS, D. M., KEMMERER, K. and POTUCEK, mentally Handicapped Children (New York, NY: Invington).
J., 2002, Brief Report: Increasing communication skills for YODER, P. and STONE, W. L., 2006, Randomized comparison of two
an elementary-aged student with autism using the Picture communication interventions for preschoolers with Autism
Exchange Communication System. Journal of Autism and Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Counseling and Clinical
Developmental Disorders, 32, 225–230. Psychology, 74, 426–435.
Copyright of International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders is the property of Wiley-
Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

You might also like