You are on page 1of 24

Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, Vol. 3, No.

4, 2000

Child Outcomes of Nonabusive and Customary Physical


Punishment by Parents: An Updated Literature Review

Robert E. Larzelere1

This article updates the only previous systematic literature review of child outcomes of
nonabusive and customary physical punishment by parents. The outcomes differ by methodo-
logic, child, and subcultural factors as well as by how the physical punishment was used. All
six studies that used clinical samples (including four randomized clinical studies) and all
three sequential-analysis studies found beneficial outcomes, such as reduced noncompliance
and fighting, primarily when nonabusive spanking was used to back up milder disciplinary
tactics in 2- to 6-year olds. Five of eight longitudinal studies that controlled for initial
child misbehavior found predominantly detrimental outcomes of spanking. However, those
detrimental outcomes were primarily due to overly frequent use of physical punishment.
Furthermore, apparently detrimental outcomes have been found for every alternative disci-
plinary tactic when investigated with similar analyses. Such detrimental associations of fre-
quent use of any disciplinary tactic may be due to residual confounding from initial child
misbehavior. Specific findings suggest discriminations between effective and counterproduc-
tive physical punishment with young children. More research is needed to clarify the role
of spanking and alternative disciplinary tactics in control system aspects of parental discipline.
KEY WORDS: physical punishment; parental discipline.

Few social scientific topics involve such sharp ‘‘probably efficacious’’ (Lonigan, Elbert, &
contradictions as does parental spanking. Consider Johnson, 1998).
the following examples: ● Social scientists generally make sensitivity to
cultural differences a top priority. Yet African-
● About 94% of American 3- and 4-year olds have American families are often denigrated for
been spanked by a parent at least once during spanking, even though most relevant research
the past year (Straus & Stewart, 1999). Yet most
finds that moderate spanking has benign or
clinical child psychologists would never suggest beneficial outcomes in African Americans
parental spanking, and one-third consider such (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997, and 11 com-
a suggestion always unethical (Hyman, 1997;
mentaries; Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997;
Schenck, Lyman, & Bodin, 2000). Whaley, 2000).
● Several countries have banned all parental ● A large number of social scientists consider the
spanking, and others are considering such a ban. mildest spanking to be more harmful than alter-
Yet if spanking were being evaluated as a psy- native disciplinary tactics. Yet the only system-
chological intervention, it would qualify as
atic review of nonabusive physical punishment
found little evidence of differential harmfulness
1
Psychology Department, Munroe–Meyer Institute, University of in direct comparisons with alternative disciplin-
Nebraska Medical Center, and Girls and Boys Town. Corre-
ary tactics (Larzelere, 1996).
spondence concerning this article should be sent to Robert E.
Larzelere, Psychology Department, Munroe–Meyer Institute,
685450 Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE 68198-5450. Email: Never before have social scientists advocated a
rlarzelere@unmc.edu. total ban on a practice this widespread. Does the

199
1096-4037/00/1200-0199$18.00/0  2000 Plenum Publishing Corporation
200 Larzelere

available scientific evidence support a total ban on cent studies.2 These procedures yielded 269 poten-
parental spanking, or does the evidence suggest an tial studies.
appropriate but limited role for nonabusive spanking The selection criteria for relevant articles were
in parental discipline? These are major questions un- as follows: First, a study had to be published in a
derlying this review of the relevant scientific liter- peer-reviewed professional journal in the English
ature. language. Exceptions to this criterion were made for
It is widely known that several methodologic sources in the Gershoff (1999) review and for recent
problems are pervasive in much research on parental unpublished studies that otherwise qualified. Forty-
spanking. First, most research is cross-sectional and one (41) of the 269 potential studies were ruled out
correlational, which cannot establish causation. Sec- because they included no original data on parental
ond, much research emphasizes overly severe forms physical punishment. Second, studies had to have a
of physical punishment. The controversy is about child outcome variable for which beneficial versus
nonabusive physical punishment, not overly severe detrimental outcomes were reasonably unambigu-
forms of physical punishment. Third, many studies ous. This eliminated 40 additional studies. This crite-
incorporate spanking as one item in a broader mea- rion excluded studies in which the only child outcome
sure of punitiveness. Such studies cannot determine concerned the likelihood of using nonabusive physi-
the unique effects of spanking. Thus these three kinds cal punishment as a parent. Studies in which child
of studies were excluded from this literature review. outcomes concerned the child’s subsequent use of
It would have been ideal to focus exclusively on overly severe or frequent physical punishment were
studies of explicitly nonabusive spanking. Because included, however.
such studies are so few, this review also considers Third, a study had to include at least one mea-
studies of customary spanking. These are studies that sure of nonabusive or customary physical punishment
measure physical punishment without emphasizing by parents. This excluded measures dominated by
the severity of its use. severity or abusiveness (eliminating 37 more studies)
This review updates the only previous systematic and measures dominated by nonspanking tactics
review of child outcomes of nonabusive or customary (e.g., washing a child’s mouth out with soap, yelling,
physical punishment by parents (Larzelere, 1996) and restraint, nonphysical punishment, which eliminated
its unpublished predecessor (Lyons, Anderson, & 39 additional studies). Fourth, the referent period for
Larson, 1993). It uses a qualitative ‘‘box-score’’ re- parental physical punishment had to precede the time
view for two reasons. First, a qualitative review clari- period for the child outcome measure (eliminating
fies whether outcomes vary by the causal conclusive- 32 cross-sectional studies). Finally, the average age
ness of the designs, which is essential for conclusions of the child when spanked had to be younger than
about the causal effect of physical punishment. As 13 years (eliminating 42 otherwise eligible studies).
noted by Miller and Pollock (1994), meta-analyses This criterion also excluded retrospective studies un-
of causally inconclusive studies yield causally incon- less their survey specified a referent age for spanking
clusive summaries. Second, a qualitative review pro- that averaged younger than 13 years.3 Table I summa-
vides more details about the qualifying studies so that rizes the 38 studies that met all five criteria.
others can more easily critique the conclusions of Because two reviews of physical punishment
this review. This is particularly important, given the came to rather different conclusions (Gershoff, 1999;
controversial nature of the topic. Larzelere, 1996), it might be helpful to clarify which
studies from Gershoff’s (1999) review met the criteria

METHOD
2
Diana Baumrind, Kirby Deater-Deckard, Joan Durrant, Chris
Ellison, Leonard Eron, James Garbarino, Elizabeth Thompson
Several strategies were used to locate articles Gershoff, Anthony Graziano, George Holden, Irwin Hyman, John
for this review. Articles from 1995 to February 2000 Knutson, Joan McCord, Vonnie McLoyd, Jerry Patterson, Kathy
that addressed corporal punishment were identified Ritchie, Mark Roberts, Rebecca Socolar, John Steley, Murray
using PsychLit, Medline, and references in those arti- Straus, Claudio Violato, Gail Wallerstein.
3
A prospective study that asked similar questions retrospectively
cles. In addition, all sources in two reviews of physical
at age 25 found that the retrospective data correlated most
punishment’s outcomes were considered as potential strongly with earlier maternal reports of physical punishment at
references (Gershoff, 1999; Larzelere, 1996). Finally, ages 12 to 14 (Stattin, Janson, Klackenberg-Larsson, & Magnus-
21 leading research investigators were asked for re- son, 1995).
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 201

for this review and which criteria were not met by teristics. The final part of the Results section then
other studies in that review. Although the Gershoff compares the outcomes of physical punishment with
(1999) review is still being revised for publication, it those of alternative disciplinary techniques in these
has been cited as ‘‘in press’’ for at least 2 years (e.g., 38 studies.
Straus & Mouradian, 1998, citing an earlier version
as Thompson, in press).
Only 16 of the 92 studies in Gershoff (1999) met Research Design: Causal Conclusiveness
the criteria for this review. Gershoff’s (1999) other
studies were excluded because of (i) the severity of The child outcomes of physical punishment var-
the measure of physical punishment (17 studies), (ii) ied dramatically by the type of research design (see
an overly broad measure of punishment (17 studies), Table I). All 6 clinical studies (including 4 random-
(iii) a cross-sectional design (22 studies), (iv) a focus ized clinical trials) found predominantly beneficial
on physical punishment of teenagers (19 studies), or child outcomes, as did the 3 studies using sequential
(v) no unambiguous child outcome variable (1 study). analyses. The beneficial outcomes included reduced
Thus, the present review focuses more specifically on noncompliance and fighting and, in one study, en-
causally relevant studies of nonabusive or customary hanced parental affection. However, 5 of the 8 con-
physical punishment of young children than does the trolled longitudinal studies found a broader range of
Gershoff (1999) review. predominantly detrimental child outcomes of physi-
For the purposes of summarizing the data, a cal punishment, and the remaining 3 studies found
finding was counted as a beneficial outcome if physi- neutral outcomes or a mixture of both beneficial and
cal punishment predicted a desirable outcome in the detrimental outcomes. Almost half (47%) of the 15
child (e.g., improved compliance) at the 0.05 signifi- uncontrolled longitudinal studies found predomi-
cance level. A finding was counted as a detrimental nantly detrimental child outcomes, and the rest (53%)
outcome if physical punishment significantly pre- found neutral outcomes. Two (2) of the 6 retrospec-
dicted an undesirable outcome in the child (e.g., tive studies found beneficial outcomes, 1 found detri-
lower self-esteem, more delinquency). A study was mental outcomes, and the remaining 3 found neu-
summarized as finding predominantly beneficial or tral outcomes.
predominantly detrimental outcomes if (i) its only Of the 12 retrospective studies in Larzelere’s
relevant significant outcome was in the specified di- (1996) earlier review that did not qualify for this
rection, (ii) one of its two or three relevant analyses updated review, two-thirds found predominantly det-
was significant and in the specified direction, or (iii) rimental outcomes, and the other one-third found
at least two of its relevant analyses were significant neutral outcomes. Thus detrimental outcomes have
and in the specified direction. Otherwise, a study was previously been most common in retrospective stud-
summarized as showing a neutral outcome. ies, especially when the referent child age is either
unspecified or averaged 13 years old or more.
In general, the stronger the causal conclusive-
RESULTS ness of the design, the more likely a study was to
find beneficial child outcomes. There were, however,
Overall, the 38 qualifying studies (Table I) di- important exceptions to that overall tendency. Con-
vided almost equally into predominantly beneficial trolled longitudinal studies were the most causally
child outcomes (32%), predominantly detrimental conclusive studies that found mostly detrimental out-
outcomes (34%), and neutral or mixed outcomes comes. Those studies differed from the randomized
(34%). The following sections consider how various clinical trials on many other dimensions. The clinical
study characteristics discriminate beneficial out- studies tended to focus on using a 2-swat spank to
comes from detrimental outcomes. Relevant study enforce short-term compliance with time out in clini-
characteristics include methodologic characteristics, cally referred 2- to 6-year olds. The controlled longi-
type of outcome, child characteristics, how the physi- tudinal studies were based on maternal reports of
cal punishment was used, and the cultural context. spanking frequency in older, nonclinical samples, us-
Because different characteristics are often con- ing the following long-term outcomes: antisocial be-
founded with each other, a subsequent section havior, fighting, hostility, emotional problems, com-
highlights the 11 studies that are most relevant for petence, and self-esteem. (A later section returns to
untangling common confounds among these charac- these studies to begin untangling these confounds.)
Table I. Studies of Child Outcomes of Nonabusive or Customary Physical Punishment by Parents
N Type Physical Other Child Physical
and of punishment discipline outcome punishment
a
Authors Age genderb sample (parent)c,d responses measures associatione Summary of findingsa,d

Randomized clinical trials

Preschoolers
Bean & 2–6 24 B,G Clinic-referred, 2-swat spank back-up Child release from Compliance with par- ⫹ Percentage compliance improved more with spank back-up
Roberts disruptive for time out (TO) TO, non-time- ent commands than in either of other two conditions. Fewer TOs than
(1981) problems (M) out control Child Release group.
Day & 2–5 16 B,G Clinic-referred, 2-swat spank back-up Room time out Compliance with par- ⫹ Spank back-up and room time out back-up equally effec-
Roberts disruptive for TO (M) back-up for ent commands tive in increasing compliance.
1983 chair TO (bar-
rier)
Roberts 2–6 18 B,G Clinic-referred, 2-swat spank back-up Room time out Compliance to parent ⫹ The two back-ups equally effective in reducing escapes
(1988) disruptive for TO (M) back-up for TO commands, escapes from time out and in increasing compliance. Room
(barrier) from time out chair, time-out back-up had fewer verbalizations in first chair
verbalizations time out.
Roberts & 2–6 36 B,G Clinic-referred, 2-swat spank back-up Room TO, child Compliance, TO es- ⫹ Spank and room TO yielded fewer escapes from TO and
Powers disruptive for TO (M) release, hold as capes, number of fewer TOs than hold or child release back-ups. Fifteen
(1990) TO backups chair TOs needed children needed alternative back-up (switch to spank in
12 cases), which increased compliance.

Other clinical studies

Preschoolers
Roberts 2–6 32 B,G Clinic-referred, 2-swat spank back-up None Escapes from time out ⫹ Reduced mean number of escapes from time out from
(1982) disruptive for TO (M) first to second time out attempt.
School age
Bernal et al. 8 1B Clinic-referred, Good, hard whack Ignore Compliance, aggression, ⫹ Increased compliance, decreased aggression, increased
(1968) disruptive on rear (M) liking of son liking for son.

Longitudinal studies controlling for initial misbehavior

Preschoolers
Baumrind 3–5 (T1), 87–135 Children in pre- Frequency of physi- Verbal punish- General competence, ⫺/0 After controlling for initial level of externalizing problems
& Owens 7–11 (T2), B,G schools cal punishment ment cognitive compe- and cooperation, T1 spanking predicted lower general
(2000) 12–16 (T3) from ratings of in- tence, social agency, competence at T2 and T3, lower cognitive competence
terviews and home communal compe- at T2, and lower communal competence at T2. Spanking
observations (B) tence, total problems, frequency at T2 predicted lower general competence at
externalizing prob- T3 with the same control. Then, after dropping the 5% or
lems, internalizing 7% most frequent/severe cases of physical punishment,
problems only one of those remained significant (T1 spanking and
T2 general competence). This became nonsignificant after
controlling also for either positive parenting or malad-
justed parenting. Using equivalent analyses, T1 verbal
punishment predicted detrimental outcomes in five T3
outcomes, four of which held up after dropping ex-
treme verbal punishment cases.
Ellison et al. 2–4 (T1), 467 B,G Nationally repre- Frequency of spank- None Antisocial behavior, ⫹/⫺ In conservative Protestant families, children who were
(1998) 9–11 (T2) sentative ing in previous hostility, emotional spanked only at T1 had fewer T2 problems than chil-
sample week at T1 and T2 problems dren who were not spanked at either time. In other
(mother report) families, there were increased T2 problems for children
(B) spanked at both times, but no effects for those spanked
at T1 but not at T2.
Gunnoe & 4–11 (T1), 1112 B,G Nationally repre- Frequency of spank- None Antisocial behavior, ⫹/⫺ Spanking frequency predicted a decrease of subsequent
Mariner 9–16 (T2) sentative ing in previous youth-reported fight- fighting 5 years later for (1) African Americans, (2) 4-to
(1997) sample week (mother re- ing at school 7-year olds, and (3) girls. Spanking frequency predicted
port) (B) an increased frequency of fighting 5 years later for (1)
European Americans and (2) 8- to 11-year olds. It also
predicted higher mother-reported antisocial behavior 5
years later, replicating Straus et al. (1997). All analyses
controlled for T1 bullying.
Larzelere 2–3 (T1), 4 38 B,G Volunteers in re- Slap hand or spank Reasoning, non- Delay until next recur- ⫹/0 Backing up reasoning with either kind of punishment en-
et al. (T2) sponse to news- when used in com- physical punish- rence of fighting or hanced the subsequent effectiveness of reasoning by it-
(1998b) paper article bination with rea- ment, combina- disobedience fol- self in delaying the next recurrence of either misbehavior.
soning (mother re- tions of the lowing subsequent This effect occurred for a nonphysical back-up in 8 of 8
port) (M) above, other reasoning by itself, analyses and for physical punishment in 4 of 8 analyses.
disruptive behavior Children whose mothers used reasoning by itself fre-
at T2 quently and also backed up reasoning with nonphysical
punishment showed the largest decrease in T2 disrup-
tive behavior. The same effect for physical punishment
as a back-up was not significant. Reasoning without pun-
ishment predicted more T2 disruptive behavior.
School age
Adams 6–12 (T1), 367 B,G National sample Spanked twice or None Academic achievement ⫺/0 Frequent spanking group had lower global self-esteem in
(1995) 10–16 (T2) with some more in previous in reading and math, boys and for all children. No significant differences on
oversamplingf week or spanked behavior problems, any of the other four outcome variables for boys. No-
once plus other en- global self-esteem, thing was significant for girls. Controlled for ethnicity;
dorsement of scholastic self-esteem nurturance; cognitive enrichment; poverty; self-esteem
spanking (mother and behavior problems; and other variables.
report) (M)
Larzelere 6–9 (T1), 981 B,G National sample Spanking frequency Grounding, send- Antisocial behavior ⫺/0 The frequency of spanking or of any other the four alter-
& Smith 8–11 (T2) with some in previous week ing to room, re- natives each predicted a higher rate of antisocial behav-
(2000) oversamplingf (mother report) moving allow- ior 2 years later, controlling for the original level of anti-
(M) ance, removing social behavior. Predictions of T2 antisocial from each
privileges in pre- disciplinary tactic became nonsignificant when T1 exter-
vious week nalizing problems were measured more thoroughly. Con-
trolled for five other variables and interactions.
McLeod 4 or more 1866 B,G National sample Spanking frequency None Antisocial behavior ⫺/0 Spanking frequency at Time 2 predicted antisocial be-
et al. (T1), 6 or with some in previous week havior at Time 2 in European Americans but not in
(1994) more (T2) oversamplingf (mother report) African Americans, controlling for Time 1 antisocial
(M) behavior. T1 antisocial behavior predicted increased
spanking in both races.
Straus 6–9 (T1), 807 B,G National sample Spanking frequency None Antisocial behavior ⫺ Spanking predicted more antisocial behavior 2 years later,
et al. 8–11 (T) with some in previous week controlling for initial level of antisocial behavior as well
(1997) oversamplingf (mother report) as five other demographic and parenting-quality mea-
(M) sures and their interactions with spanking, due mostly to
those spanked 3⫹ times weekly. Correlations similar for
four other cohorts, including 3- to 5-year olds and chil-
dren 10 or older.

Sequential analyses

Preschoolers
Larzelere 2–3 38 B,G Volunteers in re- Slap hand or spank Reasoning, non- Delay until next recur- ⫹ Either type of punishment without reasoning delayed the
& Merenda sponse to news- (mother report) physical punish- rence of disobedience next disobedience recurrence the most at high levels of
(1994) paper article (M) ment, combina- or fighting child distress. Reasoning delayed disobedience recur-
tions of above, rences the most at intermediate levels of child distress.
and other Either type of punishment with reasoning at an inter-
mediate child distress was the best at delaying disobedi-
ence recurrences overall.
Larzelere 2–3 38 B,G Volunteers Slap hand or spank Reasoning, non- Delay until next recur- ⫹ The combination of either physical or nonphysical punish-
et al. (mother report) physical punish- rence of disobedience ment with reasoning was associated with a longer delay
(1996) (M) ment, combina- or fighting until the next misbehavior recurrence than following
tions of above, reasoning alone, punishment alone, or other discipline
and other responses.
Table I. (Continued)
N Type Physical Other Child Physical
and of punishment discipline outcome punishment
a
Authors Age genderb sample (parent)c,d responses measures associatione Summary of findingsa,d
Ritchie 3 90 B,G Recruited from Slap hand or spank Ignore, power as- Ability to stop a non- ⫹ Time out and spank were the only tactics that mothers (1)
(1999) birth records (mother report) sert physically, compliant tactic sug- were more likely to use for defiance and (2) that was
(M) power assert gested with (1) not followed by an inflated probability of defiance. This
verbally, time greater likelihood of pattern occurred for other disciplinary tactics only for
out, threaten, re- using the tactic in re- ignore in response to whining and for privilege removal
move privilege, sponse to a given in response to simple refusals. Compliance occurred at
reasoning, offer child tactic (e.g., de- higher rates following only ignore and power assert
alternative fiance) when (2) the physically.
disciplinary tactic was
not followed by a sig-
nificantly increased
probability of the
same child tactic;
compliance
Prospective longitudinal studies
Preschoolers
Crowne 5 (T1), 18 83 B,G Kindergarten Frequency of spank- Deprivation of Realistic aspirations 0 Mother’s spanking frequency associated with realistic as-
et al. (T2) ing by each parent, privileges, with- (vs. overcautious or pirations more than with unrealistic aspirations (females).
(1969) extent of physical drawal of love unrealistic), unusual Mother’s spanking frequency ⫹r with number of unusual
punishment (B) shifts in aspirations shifts in aspirations. Love withdrawal ⫹r with overly cau-
tious aspirations (males).
Deater- 1–5 (T1), 585 B,G Kindergarten stu- No vs. mild to harsh Harsh discipline, Externalizing problems ⫺/0 Nonabusive mild to harsh physical punishment predicted
Deckard 5.5–12 (T2 dents physical disci- ranging from (teacher rating) lower externalizing problems in African Americans com-
& Dodge to T8) pline, distin- nonrestrictive pared to children of nonspankers, whereas it predicted
(1997) guished from physi- and positive to higher externalizing problems in European Americans
cal abuse (M) strict, severe, of- (significance not given). The harsh discipline variable
ten physical predicted subsequent externalizing problems in Euro-
pean Americans, but not in African Americans, unless
African-American parents were nonnurturing.
Deater- 4 (T1), 4.5– 566 B,G Kindergarten Mother’s spontane- None Externalizing problems ⫺/0 Physical punishment ⫹r with all 3 measures of acting out
Deckard 7.5 (T2) ous report of PP (teacher report), ag- for European Americans, physical punishment ⫺r with
et al. as responses to 5 gression and conflict all three measures for African Americans, but nonsig-
(1996) misbehavior with teacher (peer nificantly so.
vignettes (M) report)
Grinder 5 (T1), 11–12 140 B,G Kindergarten Extent of physical Isolation, love Resistance to tempta- 0 Physical punishment and other disciplinary responses
(1962) (T2) punishment (M) withdrawal, tion to cheat for were each unrelated to resistance to temptation.
privilege depri- prize in lab game
vation
Johannesson 9–24 mos. 212 B,G Birth cohort Extent of spanking None Teacher’s rating of ten- 0 Spanking was not associated with subsequent school ag-
(1974) (T1), 10–13 over 3 years from ‘‘never’’ to dency to start a quar- gression.
(T2) ‘‘constantly’’ (M) rel and have conflict
with peers
McClelland 5 (T1), 31–32 78 B,G Kindergarten Extent of physical Deprivation of Need for achievement, 0 Physical punishment ⫺r with need for power for males
& Pilon (T2) punishment (M) privileges need for affiliation, only; deprivation of privileges unrelated to all adult
(1983) need for power needs.
McCord & 6 (T1), 32 953 B,G Children in Afri- Mother’s report as to None reported Criminal violence, de- 0/⫺ Frequent spanking predicted a greater probability of the
Ensminger (T2) can-American whether their child pression, alcoholism three adulthood problems in only 1 of 6 analyses (only
(1997) inner city was spanked twice increased alcoholism for men; none of three outcomes
a week or more were increased for women).
(M)
Michels M ⫽ 5.3 342 B,G Kindergarten Parents’ report that Attention, time Disruptive classroom ⫺ Children whose parents thought physical punishment
et al. (T1), M ⫽ children physical punish- out, depriva- behavior (e.g., hit- worked best were more disruptive in the classroom
(1993) 5.9 (T2) ment worked best. tion of privi- ting, defiance, disrup- than those who thought alternatives worked best or
Frequency of physi- leges, reason, tiveness) as reported who considered discipline not a problem or who said
cal punishment. bribe/threaten, by the teacher nothing worked. Those physically punished weekly or
(B) formal behav- more were more disruptive than those hit less often.
ior management Among those not physically punished, frequency of
system nonphysical punishment was not associated with disrup-
tive behavior.
Sears 5 (T1), 12 160 B,G Kindergarten Extent of physical Deprivation of Five types of aggression 0 Of 10 correlations with later aggession, physical punish-
(1961) (T2) punishment (M) privileges, love (e.g., antisocial, pro- ment ⫹r only with prosocial agression in girls. The other
withdrawal jected) discipline responses also had one more ⫹r than ⫺r.
Sears 5 (T1), 12 160 B,G Kindergarten Extent of physical None Self-esteem 0 Physical punishment not associated with subsequent self-
(1970) (T2) punishment (M) esteem.
Smith & 12 & 36 mos. 715 B,G Low-birth- Mother reported 2 or None IQ 0/⫺ Girls in the high physical punishment group at both 12
Brooks- (T1), 36 weight more physical pun- and 36 months were lower on IQ than were those not in
Gunn mos. (T2) children ishments in past that group at both ages. The other 11 pairwise compari-
(1997) week or seen hit- sons were not significant.
ting or scolding
(M)
Strassberg 4 (T1), 4.5 273 B,G Kindergarten Spanking frequency Parental violence Observed bullying and ⫺ Spanked children were higher than the 11 nonspanked/
et al. (T2) during past year, reactive, instrumen- nonabused children on total aggression and reactive ag-
(1994) including spank- tal, and total aggres- gression, but not on bullying or instrumental aggression.
ing with object (B) sion at school Spanking frequency not related to any aggression mea-
sures.
Straus & 1–4 (T1), 3–6 960 B,G National sample Frequency of spank- None Cognitive achievement ⫺ Spanking frequency predicted poorer cognitive achieve-
Paschall (T2), 5–9 with some ing during previ- in reading and math ment at T3, controlling for cognitive measures at T1,
(1998) (T3) oversamplingf ous week at T1 age, ethnicity, gender, mother’s education, father pres-
and T2 plus ob- ence, cognitive stimulation, emotional support, etc. Re-
served hitting (M) portedly replicated without 1-year olds in the sample.
Yarrow M ⫽ 4.2 58 B,G Nursery school 9-point scale from lit- Isolation, reason- Aggression and depen- ⫺/0 Physical punishment was associated with more boys’ depen-
et al. (T1), M ⫽ children tle to very often ing, privilege re- dency as rated by dency (desired attention and closeness) toward teacher,
(1968) 4.4 (T2) used, rated from in- moval, love two teachers in the but not associated with aggression in either gender (ex-
terview about six withdrawal, iso- nursery school cept physical punishment for sibling aggression predicted
extreme examples lation, scolding, lower aggression in total sample and tended toward sig-
of disobedience diverting atten- nificance for boys). Other disciplinary tactics also gener-
(M) tion, modeling ally unrelated to outcomes.
School age
Paschall 5–9 (T1), 711 B,G National sample Frequency of spank- None Cognitive achievement ⫺ Spanking frequency predicted poorer cognitive achieve-
& Straus 7–11 (T2), with some ing during previ- in reading and math ment at T3, controlling for cognitive achievement at T1
(1998) 9–13 (T3) over samplingf ous week at T1 plus controls for age, ethnicity, gender, mother’s educa-
and T2 plus ob- tion, father presence, cognitive stimulation, emotional
served hitting (M) support, etc.

Retrospective studies

Preschoolers
Rhue & 3–5 & 6–10 53 B,G Top and bottom Severity, frequency None Fantasy proneness ⫺ Fantasy-prone sample reported more frequency and seve-
Lynn (T1), col- 4% of college of early physical rity of physical punishment, were more likely to be hit
(1987) lege age students on punishment; hit when behaving, and were less likely to know why they
(T2) fantasy prone- when behaving; were physically punished.
ness ⫹ middle knew why they
group were physically
punished (B)
School-age
Alibrando 7(T1), M ⫽ 96 B,G College students Percentage of punish- None analyzed Rebelliousness (vs. con- ⫹/0 Percentage of punishment that was physical predicted
(1987) 22 (T2) ment that was formity), neuroti- lower rebelliousness, controlling for parental accep-
physical, fre- cism, introversion tance or parent or child orientation for physical punish-
quency of spank- (vs. extroversion) ment. Low rebelliousness could be reframed as ex-
ing or physical pun- treme conformity. Physical punishment was unrelated
ishment (B) to neuroticism and introversion.
Table I. (Continued)
N Type Physical Other Child Physical
and of punishment discipline outcome punishment
Authors Agea genderb sample (parent)c,d responses measures associatione Summary of findingsa,d

Holmes & 6–13 (T1), 200 B,G Alcoholics, de- Spanking and slap- Punched, hit with Differences in three 0 Groups similar in percentage that had experienced spank-
Robins 18–49 (T2) pressives, con- ping used fre- belt or stick, groups (alcoholics or ing frequently or as usual mode of punishment. Con-
(1988) trols quently or as usual confinement, depressives vs. con- trols less likely to have been hit with object frequently
mode of discipline scolding, isola- trols) or beaten, less likely to fear parental discipline, or to
(B) tion, beatings be punished in public. Their mothers were more likely
to carry out their threats and share discipline responsi-
bilities with their husbands.
Larzelere Before 13 157 B,G Home economics Frequency of spank- Alternative pun- Self-esteem 0 Spanking unrelated to self-esteem, as was the alternative
et al. (T1), M ⫽ college stu- ing (before 13) (B) ishment factor punishment factor. Parent-oriented motivation for
(1989) 21 (T2) dents (time out, re- spanking predicted low self-esteem.
moving privi-
leges, and resti-
tution)
MacIntyre & Before 13 240 B,G College students Major punishment None Approval of aggression 0 Major physical punishment styles prior to age 13 unrelated
Cantrell (T1), M ⫽ style: physical pun- toward objects or to approval of aggression toward either objects or peo-
(1995) 24 (T2) ishment (No, with people ple.
explanation, with
verbal assault, or
spontaneously) (B)
Tennant 0–14 (T1), 5044 B U.S. Army sol- Punishment: spank- Noncontact pun- Usage of three drugs ⫹ Drug nonusers were more likely to report spanking than
et al. M ⫽ 23 diers in ing (Yes or no? or ishment (e.g., and alcohol (Never, were frequent drug users, for three drugs. Ampheta-
(1975) (T2) Germany most common?) stand in cor- occasional, or fre- mine nonusers were less likely to report noncontact
(B) ner), punish- quent) punishment than frequent users. No association with
ment frequency alcohol usage. Punishment frequency predicted more
frequent use of two drugs.
a
T1 ⫽ First data collection period (Time 1), T2 ⫽ second data collection period (Time 2), T3 ⫽ third data collection period (Time 3)
b
B ⫽ boys, G ⫽ girls.
c
M ⫽ mother, B ⫽ both mother and father.
d
TO ⫽ time out, ⫹r ⫽ positively associated (i.e., correlated), ⫺r ⫽ negatively associated.
e
⫹ ⫽ Predominantly beneficial child outcomes of physical punishment; ⫺ ⫽ predominantly detrimental child outcomes; 0 ⫽ generally neutral child outcomes; ⫹/⫺ ⫽ two or more
beneficial child outcomes and two or more detrimental child outcomes; 0/⫺ ⫽ generally neutral outcomes, but one outcome was detrimental out of four or more relevant analyses; ⫺/
0 ⫽ predominantly detrimental outcomes with important exceptions (e.g., disappears with controls, does not generalize across genders or ethnic groups); ⫹/0 ⫽ predominantly beneficial
outcomes with important exceptions.
f
Oversampling of young, single mothers; lower socioeconomic status; and ethnic minorities.
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 207

Uncontrolled longitudinal studies and retrospec- confounded with several other dimensions, addressed
tive studies found detrimental outcomes more often in a later section.
than beneficial outcomes. However, neither of those
designs can rule out the possibility that higher levels
of child misbehavior caused both the increased physi- Substantive Issues
cal punishment and the subsequent detrimental child
outcomes (Straus, Sugarman, & Giles-Sims, 1997). The findings in the studies in Table I varied by
Because a primary purpose of this review is to clarify several substantive characteristics as well as by meth-
the causal effects of physical punishment, the remain- odologic dimensions. These included the type of out-
der of the results section generally considers only the come, the age and clinical status of the child, how
17 studies that are most causally relevant. Thus the the physical punishment was used, and the larger
following sections exclude uncontrolled longitudinal cultural context.
and retrospective studies except for supplementary
information.
The remaining sections sometimes include more Type of Child Outcome
than 17 findings. Studies contribute more than one
finding when their results differ by the dimension The apparent effects of physical punishment
summarized or when they have both beneficial and were generally beneficial in reducing noncompliance
detrimental outcomes. For example, a study that in- and fighting and in enhancing parental warmth and
vestigated three types of child outcomes (i.e., behav- milder disciplinary tactics. However, the apparent
ior problems, mental health, and competence) would effects of physical punishment were generally detri-
be counted three times when considering how the mental in increasing externalizing behavior problems
results vary by the type of outcome. and mental health problems, and in reducing compe-
tencies.
All the studies that investigated noncompliance
Methodologic Issues found that physical punishment reduced it. This in-
cluded all 6 clinical studies, all 3 sequential studies,
This section summarizes how the results of these and the sequential analysis part of 1 controlled longi-
17 causally relevant studies varied by two methodo- tudinal study (Larzelere, Sather, Schneider, Lar-
logic variables: the type of data and whether the son, & Pike, 1998b).
outcome was short or long term. Four of five findings on fighting indicated that
Beneficial child outcomes were most likely when it was reduced by previous physical punishment. This
the outcome measure was observational (6 of 6 stud- included the single-case clinical study (Bernal, Du-
ies) or a specific daily maternal report (3 of 3 studies). ryee, Pruett, & Burns, 1968), two sequential-analysis
Detrimental child outcomes were most likely when studies (Larzelere & Merenda, 1994; Larzelere,
all the measures in a study were based on the same Schneider, Larson, & Pike, 1996), and four of seven
person’s global reports (5 of 7 results) or on different significant findings in a controlled longitudinal study
persons’ global reports (3 of 4 results). Gunnoe and (Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997). Only Gunnoe and Mari-
Mariner (1997) found mixed beneficial and detrimen- ner (1997) found that physical punishment increased
tal outcomes when different sources were used for subsequent fighting, and then only in three of seven
the spanking and outcome variable, but a uniformly significant findings (e.g., for 8- to 11-year olds and
detrimental outcome when basing all measures on European-Americans as a subgroup).
maternal reports. Overall, beneficial outcomes were A few findings suggest that physical punishment
more likely for more objective, specific outcome mea- can enhance parental nurturance and the effective-
sures. ness of milder disciplinary tactics. The single-case
On the second methodologic issue, beneficial study in Table I (Bernal et al., 1968) found that spank-
child outcomes were more likely for outcomes mea- ing enhanced the mother’s liking for her clinically
sured less than 6 months after the use of physical disruptive 8-year-old son. All 6 clinical studies and
punishment (9 of 10 findings). Detrimental outcomes Larzelere et al. (1998b) found that spanking was ef-
were more likely for outcomes that were 6 months or fective in enhancing the subsequent effectiveness of
more after the physical punishment (7 of 10 findings). milder disciplinary tactics, such as time out or reason-
The short- vs. long-term status of the outcomes was ing. This enhanced effectiveness of milder disciplin-
208 Larzelere

ary tactics should in turn prevent disciplinary prob- tences. Baumrind and Owens (2000) found that
lems from eroding parental nurturance toward the spanking frequency at ages 3 to 5 predicted lower
child. Further research is needed, however, to clarify subsequent competencies in cognitive, communal,
that speculation. and general areas. All but one of these effects became
In contrast to these generally beneficial out- nonsignificant after those who used spanking most
comes, physical punishment tended to predict higher frequently and severely were dropped from the anal-
rates of externalizing problems, mental health prob- yses (5–7% of the sample).4 Adams (1995) found no
lems, and lower competencies in the 17 causally rele- effect on academic achievement.
vant studies in Table I. Five (5) of 10 findings indi-
cated that physical punishment significantly increased
subsequent externalizing problems (Ellison, Mu- Child Characteristics
sick, & Holden, 1998; Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997; Lar-
zelere & Smith, 2000; McLeod, Kruttschnitt, & Dorn- The outcomes of physical punishment also var-
feld, 1994; Straus et al., 1997). These detrimental ied by the child’s age and whether the child was
outcomes were neutralized or reversed in African clinically disruptive. Studies of children averaging 6
Americans (Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997; McLeod et al., or younger in Table I generally found beneficial out-
1994; but see also Straus et al., 1997) and conservative comes, whereas studies of older children generally
Protestants (Ellison et al., 1998), two subcultures that found detrimental outcomes. In children with mean
view spanking more normatively. ages under 6, 11 of 12 studies (92%) found predomi-
The most consistent causally relevant evidence nantly beneficial outcomes of physical punishment,
of detrimental outcomes of physical punishment has whereas the remaining study (8%) found predomi-
been a subsequent increase in a 6-item antisocial mea- nantly detrimental outcomes. In children averaging
sure from controlled longitudinal studies of the Na- from 7.5 to 10 years old, 6 studies (86%) found pre-
tional Longitudinal Study of Youth (NLSY: Larzel- dominantly detrimental outcomes and only 1 study
ere & Smith, 2000; McLeod et al., 1994; Straus et al., (14%) found beneficial outcomes. In the previous
1997). It is therefore important to note that (i) similar review, all 4 studies of the physical punishment of
increases in antisocial behavior were associated with teenagers found detrimental outcomes (Larzelere,
each of four other disciplinary tactics included in the 1996). Since that review, the first controlled longitudi-
NLSY survey (Larzelere & Smith, 2000) and that (ii) nal study of spanking of teenagers found increased
all of these apparently detrimental outcomes became rates of dating-partner abuse associated with such
nonsignificant when the measure of initial child exter- spanking, p ⫽ 0.06 with complete statistical control
nalizing problems was expanded to a 16-item mea- for initial delinquency (Simons, Lin, & Gordon, 1998;
sure (Larzelere & Smith, 2000). The other four disci- R. Simons, personal communication, 1999).
plinary tactics in the NLSY survey were removing The results also varied by whether the child had
privileges, removing allowance, sending to room, and clinical levels of disruptive behavior. All 6 clinical
grounding (Larzelere & Smith, 2000). studies found beneficial outcomes of spanking,
Two of four findings indicated that physical pun- whereas the other 13 findings were evenly split be-
ishment predicted increased mental health problems. tween predominantly detrimental outcomes (54%)
This included lower self-esteem (Adams, 1995) and and predominantly beneficial outcomes (46%). The
increased hostility and emotional problems in part effect of spanking frequency in Straus et al. (1997)
of the sample in Ellison et al. (1998). Both detrimental depended on initial level of antisocial behavior in 3
findings involved high spanking frequencies; twice a of the 5 cohorts (Straus, personal communication,
week for Adams’ 6- to 12-year olds or weekly spank- 1996). In those 3 cohorts, spanking decreased subse-
ing at ages 9 to 11 in Ellison et al. (1998). For conser- quent antisocial behavior in the initially most antiso-
vative Protestants, however, children who were cial group, but it increased antisocial behavior in the
spanked at ages 2 to 4 but not at ages 9 to 11 had least antisocial group.
lower hostility and emotional problems than children
4
not spanked at either age (Ellison et al., 1998). Baum- In their publication, Baumrind and Owens (2000) are planning
to control for some parenting characteristics before controlling
rind and Owens (2000) found neutral outcomes on
for initial externalizing/cooperative behavior. The summary here
internalizing problems. applies to their data when controlling only for initial externalizing/
Finally, one of two studies found that physical cooperative behavior with and without the most frequent and
punishment predicted lower subsequent compe- severe users of physical punishment.
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 209

How Physical Punishment Was Used Bates, 1994). None of these studies found detrimental
outcomes for nonabusive or customary physical pun-
The outcomes of physical punishment in the 17 ishment, except for Strassberg et al. (1994). In that
strongest studies in Table I also varied by how physi- study, the 11 children (4%) who were not spanked
cal punishment was used. Child outcomes tended to during the year preceding kindergarten were less ag-
be beneficial when physical punishment was used gressive during kindergarten than the 96% who had
nonabusively, not too frequently, primarily as a back- been spanked. All other studies in Table I that ex-
up to milder disciplinary tactics, and flexibly. cluded abusive parents found beneficial or neutral
Not one of the 17 causally relevant studies found outcomes of nonabusive physical punishment.
predominantly detrimental outcomes if they did any- Detrimental outcomes were associated with
thing to rule out parents who used physical punish- overly frequent physical punishment as well as overly
ment too severely. To be included in this review, the severe physical punishment. The 8 controlled longitu-
studies could not emphasize severity in their measure dinal studies all investigated spanking frequency or
of physical punishment, but only 2 of the controlled an approximation of it. Linear associations between
longitudinal studies made any attempt to rule out spanking frequency and subsequent outcomes may
abusive physical punishment. The 6 clinical studies be significant due solely to the most frequent spank-
and 2 of the 3 sequential analysis studies made some ers. Most (5 of 8) of the controlled longitudinal stud-
attempt to exclude overly severe physical punish- ies found detrimental child outcomes, but they all
ment. Nine (9) of the 10 findings that ruled out abuse tested linear associations of spanking frequency.
were predominantly beneficial, and the remaining Three (3) of the controlled longitudinal studies pro-
study had generally neutral findings after dropping vided information on various spanking frequencies.
the 5% to 7% of their sample that used physical pun- They indicated that the detrimental outcomes of
ishment most frequently and severely (Baumrind & physical punishment did not become significantly dif-
Owens, 2000). Of the studies that did not rule out ferent from those not spanked until the spanking
abuse, most findings (7 of 11) indicated detrimental frequency reached one to three times a week (6- to
outcomes, 2 indicated beneficial outcomes, and 2 12-year olds: Adams, 1995; 3- to 5-year olds: Baum-
showed neutral outcomes. rind & Owens, 2000; 6- to 9-year olds: Straus et al.,
Studies that emphasized the severity of physical 1997).
punishment found detrimental outcomes, but they In contrast, spanking had predominantly bene-
have been excluded from this review (e.g., Weiss, ficial outcomes when it was used conditionally, pri-
Dodge, & Bates, 1992). Straus and Mouradian (1998) marily to back up milder disciplinary tactics. All 9
addressed this issue in an important cross-sectional studies that emphasized spanking as a back-up for a
study. Mothers were asked, ‘‘When you had to spank milder tactic found beneficial child outcomes. First,
or hit [your child], how often did you spank because the series of clinical studies by Roberts and col-
you were so angry that you ‘lost it’?’’ (p. 357). Moth- leagues specified only one use for spanking: a back-
ers who reported ‘‘losing it’’ half of the time or more up for noncompliance with the time-out procedure.
showed a much stronger association between fre- The beneficial outcomes from the Larzelere series of
quency of spanking and the child’s antisocial and studies occurred primarily when spanking was used
impulsive behavior than those never losing it. For in combination with reasoning, usually with the
mothers who never lost control due to anger, their spanking coming after the reasoning. Both sets of
frequency of spanking was correlated about zero with studies found that the milder disciplinary tactic be-
the child’s antisocial and impulsive behavior. came more effective by itself after being backed up
Other studies have investigated the effects of by spanking. A conditional spanking back-up was
mild or moderate physical punishment and the effects also used by Bernal et al. (1968) that led to improved
of severe physical punishment in the same study. The compliance and parental affection and reduced
types of severe physical punishment with detrimental fighting.
outcomes included (i) whipping, punching, kicking, The conditional use of spanking after other tac-
or beating up (Bryan & Freed, 1982); (ii) beatings tics have failed is also consistent with how mothers
(Holmes & Robins, 1988); (iii) yelling, throwing change their tactics during extended disciplinary inci-
things, or attempting to injure someone when frus- dents with 3-year olds (Ritchie, 1999). Mothers were
trated or annoyed (McCord, 1988); and (iv) hitting more likely to use verbal commands and reasoning
or beating up a child (Strassberg, Dodge, Pettit, & and offer alternatives early in an extended disciplin-
210 Larzelere

ary incident. The longer an extended disciplinary inci- controlled longitudinal studies investigated the ef-
dent lasted, however, the more likely mothers were fects of physical punishment with ethnic minorities,
to impose negative consequences such as physical usually African Americans. All three studies found
power assertion, time out, a spank, or privilege re- significantly differential effects of spanking by eth-
moval. Other tactics, such as threatening, consenting, nicity. Physical punishment never predicted predomi-
and ignoring, became more frequent during the mid- nantly detrimental outcomes in ethnic minorities, ex-
dle of an extended incident, but then decreased sub- cept for ethnically diverse 6- to 9-year olds spanked
sequently. Putting those patterns together, mothers three times a week or more (Straus et al., 1997). In
tend to use mild verbal tactics at first. Then they contrast, lower spanking frequency predicted signifi-
decide whether to consent, ignore, or continue to cantly lower rates of fighting 5 years later in African-
insist on child cooperation. Finally, they implement American children in one study (Gunnoe & Mariner,
negative consequences or other power assertion as 1997). The other study found neutral outcomes of
a last resort in an extended incident. Resorting to spanking frequency in African-American children
spanking too quickly might lead to its overly frequent but detrimental outcomes in European-American
use and detrimental outcomes. In contrast, skilled children (McLeod et al., 1994).
use of spanking as an occasional back-up for milder Two uncontrolled longitudinal studies also
disciplinary tactics with 2- to 6-year olds is more ef- found significantly distinct outcomes of spanking for
fective. European Americans and for African Americans
Finally, flexible use of nonabusive spanking and (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997; Deater-Deckard,
alternatives is associated with better child outcomes Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1996). The outcomes were
than primary or exclusive use of physical punishment. predominantly detrimental for European Americans
Flexible use is best illustrated in the study of clinically but neutral for African Americans. The strongest
noncompliant 2- to 6-year olds by Roberts and Pow- evidence of ethnic differences seems to occur in pre-
ers (1990). They investigated four alternative ap- dicting school aggression (see also Polaha, 1998).
proaches to enforcing compliance with time-out: a 2- One study found differences in outcomes of
swat spank, a 1-minute room isolation, a physical spanking by religious groups (Ellison et al., 1998).
hold, and a child-determined release from time out. The results varied by whether spanking persisted
The first two back-ups for time out were equally from about age 3 to about age 10 and by whether
effective overall, and more effective than the last the family was conservative Protestant. Beneficial
two procedures. Some clinically defiant preschoolers outcomes occurred for early, discontinued spanking
would persist in their noncompliance to time-out relative to those not spanked at either age, but only
even after repeated uses of the assigned back-up pro- for conservative Protestants. Detrimental outcomes
cedure. Roberts and Powers (1990) dealt with this occurred for persistent spankers, but only for the part
by switching to an alternative back-up procedure if of the sample that was not conservative Protestants.
the initial back-up was repeated 6 times for the same These ethnic and religious subcultural differ-
time-out. The spank back-up was changed to a brief ences in the outcomes of spanking probably depend
room isolation, whereas the brief room isolation and on how spanking is used and its normative acceptance
the physical hold were changed to the spank back- in those subcultures. For example, spanking is more
up. Switching to an alternative back-up tactic was likely to be perceived as evidence of parental concern
sufficient to gain compliance with time-out in all in African-American families, whereas it is more
cases. This suggests that parents should switch disci- likely to be seen as an indication of parental rejection
plinary tactics when the first one is not working, in European-American families (Deater-Deckard et
rather than increasing the intensity of the first tactic. al., 1996). The tradition within the African-American
It also implies that parents need more disciplinary subculture views spanking as a means of establishing
options, not fewer ones, to maximize their flexible appropriate disciplinary control to prevent parents
use of nonabusive alternatives. from the need to yell at their children (Mosby, Rawls,
Meehan, Mays, & Pettinari, 1999). Understanding
these ethnic differences in the outcomes of spanking
Cultural Context has been the subject of numerous recent articles
(Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997, and 11 commentar-
Finally, the child outcomes of physical punish- ies; Baumrind, 1996; Mosby et al., 1999; Whaley,
ment differed by the cultural context. Three of the 2000).
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 211

Confounding Factors findings, 1 detrimental finding in Straus et al., 1997).


The outcomes in this review did not appear to be
To this point, this review examines the results affected by positive parenting characteristics, al-
of the 17 causally relevant studies of physical punish- though this warrants further investigation.
ment one dimension at a time. However, those di- Detrimental outcomes were more likely when
mensions were often confounded with each other. In outcomes occurred 6 months or more after the physi-
the previous review (Larzelere, 1996), beneficial child cal punishment (8 detrimental vs. 3 beneficial find-
outcomes were found consistently in the causally rele- ings). Beneficial outcomes occurred more often when
vant studies (i.e., four randomized clinical studies), outcomes occurred less than 6 months later (e.g.,
whereas detrimental child outcomes were more likely during the next discipline incident or the next day;
in causally ambiguous studies, such as uncontrolled all three findings were beneficial). Thus detrimental
longitudinal and retrospective studies. However, sev- outcomes were more likely for long-term outcomes
eral dimensions were confounded with causal conclu- than for short-term outcomes, but this time dimen-
siveness as potential explanations for discriminating sion was often confounded with the other dimensions
beneficial from detrimental child outcomes of physi- in Table II.
cal punishment. Studies showing beneficial child out- The outcomes of physical punishment varied lit-
comes of physical punishment were also more likely tle by type of outcome in Table II. Beneficial and
to (i) focus on short-term outcomes (e.g., during the detrimental outcomes were equally likely for the cat-
next discipline incident or the next week), (ii) empha- egory of subsequent behavior problems. There was
size compliance as the child outcome, (iii) study chil- only one more detrimental outcome than beneficial
dren from the ages of 2 to 6, (iv) investigate the use outcomes for mental health or competence outcomes.
of spanking to back up milder disciplinary tactics,
and (v) make some attempt to rule out abusive use
of physical punishment. In contrast, detrimental child Comparisons with Alternative Disciplinary Tactics
outcomes were most likely in studies that were oppo-
site on each of those dimensions as well as being A comparison of the outcomes of physical pun-
causally ambiguous. The controlled longitudinal ishment with outcomes of alternative disciplinary tac-
studies are directly relevant to untangling some of tics is important for sorting out the causality issues.
these confounds. Table II lists them and three other If the apparently detrimental outcomes of nonsevere
causally relevant studies that varied the characteris- physical punishment are artifacts actually caused by
tics that have been confounded with the causal con- the initial levels of child misbehavior, then the appar-
clusiveness of the studies. ent outcomes of alternative disciplinary tactics should
Predominantly detrimental long-term outcomes be similar to those of spanking when analyzed in the
never occurred in the causally relevant studies in same way. However, if the detrimental outcomes are
Table II under the following conditions: if overly unique to physical punishment, this would strengthen
severe users of physical punishment were removed the case for uniquely detrimental effects of physical
from the analyses, if spanking was measured as a punishment. Twenty (20) studies in Table I investi-
conditional back up rather than as a frequency mea- gated alternative disciplinary responses as well as
sure, or if the sample of children was initially high physical punishment. They included all 6 clinical stud-
on externalizing problems. Only one study found det- ies, all 3 sequential-analysis studies, 3 of the 8 con-
rimental outcomes for children under the mean age trolled longitudinal studies, 6 of the 15 uncontrolled
of 6 (Baumrind & Owens, 2000), and it no longer longitudinal studies, and 2 of the 6 retrospective
had predominantly detrimental outcomes after those studies.
parents who overused physical punishment were re- Only three alternative disciplinary tactics ever
moved from the analyses. In contrast, 6 of 9 findings had more beneficial outcomes than did physical pun-
for children over the mean age of 6 indicated predom- ishment, and each of those tactics compared unfavor-
inantly detrimental outcomes, and only the single- ably with physical punishment elsewhere. When used
case study found beneficial outcomes for children in as a back-up with 2- and 3-year olds, nonphysical
this age range (Bernal et al., 1968). punishment enhanced the subsequent effectiveness
Almost all the outcomes in Table II were neutral of disciplinary reasoning by itself in 9 of 10 analyses,
or beneficial for subcultural groups that are more whereas physical punishment enhanced reasoning’s
likely to endorse the use of spanking (3 beneficial effectiveness in only 4 of 10 analyses (Larzelere et
Table II. Causally Relevant Studies of Physical Punishment Pertinent to Major Confounding Variables
Severe Back-up
Association by outcome type
use of use vs. Clinically Child’s
Behavior Mental punishment frequency Positive disruptive age
Authors problemsa healtha Competencea Long-term? removed of spanking parenting sample (years)
Controlled longitudinal studies
Adams (1995) 0 — 0 Yes No Frequency Controlled No 6–12
Baumrind & 0 0 — Yes Then 0 Frequency 0 effect when No 4–5
Owens effectsf controlled
(2000)
Ellison et al. ⫹ (Cons Prosb) ⫹ (Cons Prosb) Yes No Frequency Not controlled No 2–4 and
(1998) ⫺ (Othersb) ⫺ (Othersb) 9–11
Gunnoe & ⫹ (Younger, Yes No Frequency Controlled for No 4–7 and
Mariner Blacks, girls) praise 8–11
(1997) ⫺ (Older, Whites)
Larzelere et al. 0 Yes Yes Back-up Controlled No 2–3
(1998)
Larzelere & ⫺d Yes No Frequency Controlled No 6–9
Smith (2000)
McLeod et al. ⫺ (Whites) Yes No Frequency Not controlled No 6 or
(1994) 0 (Blacks) older
Straus et al. ⫺ Yes No Frequency Controlled Noc 6–9
(1997)
Other causally relevant studies
Bernal et al. ⫹ No Yes Back-up Enhanced by Yes 8
(1968) spank
Larzelere & ⫹ No Yes Back-up Partially No 2–3
Merenda controllede
(1994)
Larzelere et al. ⫹ No Yes Back-up Partially No 2–3
(1996) controllede
Note: The most causally conclusive studies (Roberts’ four randomized clinical trials) are not included because the purpose of this table is to evaluate confounds that differentiate those
studies (with uniformly beneficial outcomes) from uncontrolled longitudinal and retrospective studies (with predominantly detrimental outcomes).
a
⫹ ⫽ predominantly beneficial effect of physical punishment; ⫺ ⫽ predominantly detrimental effect of physical punishment; 0 ⫽ neutral effect of physical punishment.
b
Cons Pros ⫽ Conservative Protestants. Beneficial effects only for Conservative Protestants spanked only at ages 2–4. Detrimental effects only for others spanked both at ages 2–4 and
at 9–11.
c
Initial antisocial behavior interacted with spanking in three of five cohorts such that spanking decreased subsequent antisocial in the most antisocial children, but increased subsequent
antisocial in the least antisocial children (Straus, personal communication, 1996).
d
The apparent effect of spanking disappeared with an improved statistical control for initial externalizing problem behavior.
e
Partly within-subject analyses.
f
Only 1 of 21 associations were significantly detrimental.
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 213

al., 1998b). However, a retrospective study found that 1997) and an evidence-based perspective that at-
spanking predicted a lower likelihood of using three tempts to differentiate between effective vs. counter-
illegal drugs, whereas noncontact punishment pre- productive spanking (Larzelere, Baumrind, & Polite,
dicted a greater likelihood of using one illegal drug 1998a). The unconditional antispanking viewpoint
(Tennant, Detels, & Clark, 1975). Ignoring and physi- holds that spanking invariably has detrimental child
cal power assertion both elicited a significantly in- effects regardless of how it is used, the age of the
creased probability of immediate compliance in Rit- child, the disciplinary situation, the parent–child con-
chie (1999) that was not achieved by spanking or text, or the cultural context. The evidence-based per-
any other disciplinary tactic in that study. However, spective holds that there may be some parental
ignoring was ineffective before spanking was tried spanking that would enhance child outcomes, or at
with an 8-year old’s clinically defiant referral (Bernal least not detract from them. The evidence-based per-
et al., 1968), and physical restraint was a significantly spective also questions whether the current scientific
less effect back-up for time-out than was a spank evidence is adequate for imposing an antispanking
back-up (Roberts & Powers, 1990). In addition to value on all parents. What are the implications of
these three tactics, grounding had more beneficial this review for these contrasting perspectives? The
outcomes with teenagers than did physical punish- next section first considers evidence for the uncondi-
ment (Caesar, 1988; Joubert, 1992; Larzelere, 1996). tional antispanking perspective.
Six additional disciplinary responses were found
to have less beneficial outcomes than did physical
punishment in at least one study: a child-determined Is Spanking Invariably Detrimental?
release from time-out with 2- to 6-year olds (Bean &
Roberts, 1981; Roberts & Powers, 1990); reasoning The view that detrimental child outcomes invari-
without punishment, punishment without reasoning, ably follow from nonabusive spanking is contradicted
and disciplinary responses other than punishment or by several patterns in these results. First, the stronger
reasoning with 2- and 3-year olds (Larzelere et al., the causal conclusiveness of the studies, the more
1996, 1998b); love withdrawal with 5-year olds likely they are to detect beneficial outcomes of spank-
(Crowne, Conn, Marlowe, & Edwards, 1969); and ing, not detrimental outcomes. Second, causally rele-
verbal punishment with 3- to 5-year olds (Baum- vant studies (e.g., controlled longitudinal studies or
rind & Owens, 2000). better) have never found detrimental child outcomes
Focusing only on the 8 controlled longitudinal under any one of the following conditions: if abusive
studies, detrimental outcomes of spanking were parents were removed from the spanking group, if
found as strongly for both undesirable alternatives spanking was measured as a back-up for milder disci-
(e.g., verbal punishment: Baumrind & Owens, 2000) plinary tactics, or if the sample were clinically defiant
and recommended alternatives (e.g., privilege re- children. Most causally relevant studies of children
moval, grounding, allowance removal, sending to averaging less than 6 years old have found beneficial
room: Larzelere & Smith, 2000; reasoning: Larzelere child outcomes of spanking. The only exception
et al., 1998b). changed to neutral outcomes after removing the 5%
In sum, the relative child outcomes in direct com- to 7% most frequent/severe spankers and controlling
parisons of spanking and alternatives varied by age. for positive parenting (Baumrind & Owens, 2000).
Nonabusive spanking compared favorably with six The strongest evidence of invariably detrimental
alternatives in 2- to 6-year olds. Four recommended child outcomes came from controlled longitudinal
alternatives show outcomes equivalent to spanking studies of the effect of spanking 6- to 9-year olds on
during the ages 6 to 9. Grounding has been replicated their antisocial behavior 2 to 5 years later (Gunnoe &
as a more effective disciplinary alternative than Mariner, 1997; Larzelere & Smith, 2000; McLeod et
spanking with teenagers (Larzelere, 1996). al., 1994; Straus et al., 1997). Together, these studies
found that the detrimental effect did not generalize
consistently to African-American families or to child-
DISCUSSION reported fighting at school. Only Straus et al. (1997)
found consistently detrimental outcomes of spanking
There are two major current perspectives on pa- frequency at ages 6 to 9, predicting increased subse-
rental use of nonabusive physical punishment: an un- quent antisocial behavior according to maternal re-
conditional antispanking perspective (Straus et al., ports. The significance of their effect seems to be
214 Larzelere

solely due to those spanked at the rate of three times unavoidable negative side effects is difficult to find.
or more a week. Overall, the unique effect of spank- Reviews of side effects of punishment in general have
ing accounted for only 1.3% of the variance in subse- found such effects to be limited and readily avoided
quent antisocial behavior in their study (Larzelere et (Newsom, Favell, & Rincover, 1983; Walters & Gru-
al., 1998a). sec, 1977). For example, Newsom et al. (1983) con-
Larzelere and Smith (2000) replicated and ex- cluded
tended the Straus et al. (1997) study, using the same
Punishment procedures are avoided and underuti-
dataset. They showed that four recommended alter- lized more often from uninformed fears of hypotheti-
native disciplinary tactics predicted the same small cal, all-powerful negative side effects than from
increase in antisocial behavior 2 years later, when knowledgeable appraisals of their generally limited
analyzed in the same way as spanking. Such effects and manageable negative side effects. The result is
for spanking and all four alternatives became nonsig- often the continuation of serious behavior problems
for months and years when they might be eliminated,
nificant when initial child misbehavior was controlled to the client’s immense long-term benefit, in a matter
for more adequately, with a 16-item measure of exter- of days or weeks. (pp. 285–286)
nalizing problems rather than a trichotomous mea-
sure of antisocial behavior. Thus, the most consistent Walters and Grusec (1977) came to a similar
evidence of detrimental outcomes of spanking on overall conclusion, but noted that increased aggres-
subsequent antisocial behavior (Straus et al., 1997) sion was a likely side effect of physical punishment.
seems to be due to residual confounding, which oc- This review found mixed evidence on that point.
curs when an inadequate measure of the confounding Three causally relevant studies (Bernal et al., 1968;
variable is used as a covariate (Rothman, 1986). Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997; Larzelere et al., 1996)
The equivalence of child outcomes for spanking found that physical punishment reduced subsequent
and alternatives is sometimes used as an argument fighting in some circumstances, whereas one of those
that spanking can be banned because equally effec- studies (Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997) found that it also
tive alternatives are available (Graziano, Hamblen, & increased subsequent fighting in other circumstances.
Plante, 1996). By itself, this type of argument would Walters and Grusec (1977) did not have access to
be considered insufficient for banning nonparental causally relevant studies on spanking and aggression.
interventions. For example, the equivalent effective- The strongest evidence for a negative side effect
ness of a new drug (e.g., Tylenol) would not be suffi- of physical punishment occurred for the broader out-
cient grounds for banning a traditional drug (aspirin). come of antisocial behavior, which was considered
Rather, two drugs whose effectiveness is equivalent previously (Ellison et al., 1998; Gunnoe & Mariner,
on the average would each be preferable in particular 1997; McLeod et al., 1994; Straus et al., 1997). The
cases. Second, each would provide an alternative detrimental effect on antisocial behavior tended to
when the other proved ineffective in a particular case. be small, was contradicted in two subcultural groups,
Third, some applications (e.g., treating high fevers) and was replicated by every alternative disciplinary
would involve the combined use of both drugs. The tactic investigated to date (Larzelere & Smith, 2000).
application of all three principles to spanking and This particular side effect (increased antisocial be-
alternatives are illustrated in Roberts and Powers havior) seems to be contradicted by the replicated
(1990). As a backup for time-out, spanking and a effectiveness of behavioral parent training for treat-
brief room isolation proved equally effective on the ing conduct disorder (Brestan & Eyberg, 1998; Kaz-
average. Each one worked with some children better din, 1995). Its effectiveness has been documented
than the alternative. Each alternative (spanking or a with either the spank back-up or an alternative back-
brief room isolation) was effective when the other up for time-out. So the empirical evidence for nega-
alternative was slow in accomplishing the goal of tive side effects of nonabusive spanking is sparse
compliance with the time-out procedure. This sug- and inconsistent.
gests that parents can be more effective with multiple Of course, the unconditional antispanking per-
options in disciplinary tactics, just as they can be spective sometimes considers empirical evidence ir-
more effective with multiple drugs for treating fevers. relevant due to the ethical value of protecting chil-
Another concern is that negative side effects are dren from all hitting. This is certainly commendable,
more likely for spanking than for alternative disci- but it is not applied absolutely in other areas. There
plinary tactics. Unintended side effects are not a spe- is no widespread advocacy against piercing a child’s
cial focus of this review, but conclusive evidence of skin for medical shots or surgery. So the value of
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 215

minimizing bodily pain to children is a relative value olds by loving parents. This is consistent with the
that gets balanced with the presumed value of such conditions under which mothers are most likely to
practices. Thus evidence for the effectiveness of use spanking; that is, after milder disciplinary tactics
spanking is relevant to an appropriate balance of have failed (Goodenough, 1931; Mosby et al., 1999;
antipain values with competing values. Ritchie, 1999). The series of studies by Roberts and
Moreover, some ethnic and religious groups colleagues show that the spank back-up is one of the
have explicit values supporting spanking. There has two most effective tactics for backing up time-out in
been little careful scholarship on the justification for clinically defiant children in this age range. The series
imposing one set of values on groups with differing of studies by Larzelere and colleagues (1998b)
values, given various levels of empirical scientific jus- showed a similar effect in enforcing disciplinary rea-
tification. Instead, the discussions about spanking are soning among nonclinical 2- and 3-year olds. A bene-
often based on a superficial understanding of the fit of using spanking as a back-up is that the milder
empirical data or on a simplistic absolute stance disciplinary tactic becomes more effective by itself,
against any pain, which is applied inconsistently to thus rendering spanking less necessary subsequently.
other issues. At the other extreme, most detrimental out-
When 94% of parents use physical punishment comes in causally relevant studies are due to overly
at least occasionally with 3- and 4-year olds (Straus & frequent use of physical punishment. This suggests
Stewart, 1999), social scientists must clarify these that overly frequent spanking or its correlates are
kinds of issues carefully. The inadequacy of the social indicative of counterproductive ways of using spank-
scientific evidence suggests the possibility that uncon- ing. Because the same apparently detrimental effects
ditional antispanking advocates are inadvertently im- occur for overly frequent use of four recommended
posing one set of values on a very complex issue. alternative discipline tactics, the dysfunctionality as-
Reasoning and nonphysical disciplinary tactics may sociated with overly frequent punishments general-
work better for highly verbal parents in the wealthy izes across disciplinary responses rather than being
suburbs than for many less-advantaged parents. We unique to spanking. Further research is needed to
must be sensitive to cultural, religious, and socioeco- determine the nature of this dysfunctionality. An ini-
nomic distinctions before imposing our values on tial study suggested that these apparently detrimental
other parents on this important issue. effects are methodologic artifacts (e.g., residual con-
In sum, Diana Baumrind’s (1996) assessment founding when the initial level of child misbehavior is
still seems applicable: ‘‘A blanket injunction against imperfectly controlled for: Larzelere & Smith, 2000).
disciplinary spanking by parents is not scientifically Between these two extremes, the research evi-
supportable’’ (p. 828). That is not equivalent, of dence leaves a gray area with a few empirical clues.
course, to endorsing or even tolerating all forms of The clues suggest that the following guidelines are
customary physical punishment (Bauman & Fried- more characteristic of effective spanking than of
man, 1998). counterproductive physical punishment:
1. Not overly severe.
2. Under control, not in danger of ‘‘losing it’’
Effective vs. Counterproductive Physical from anger (Straus & Mouradian, 1998).
Punishment 3. During ages 2 to 6, not during the teenage
years (Gunnoe & Mariner, 1997; Simons et
This review clarifies one form of effective spank- al., 1998). Although conclusive evidence is
ing and one indication of generally counterproductive scarce, spanking should be phased out as soon
use of physical punishment. Additional research is as possible between the ages of 7 and 12.
needed to clarify the appropriate boundaries be- 4. Used with reasoning (Larzelere et al., 1996),
tween these two extremes of customary physical pun- preferably eliciting an intermediate rather
ishment. A few empirically based hints about this than a high level of child distress (Larzelere &
middle gray area are summarized from this review Merenda, 1994).
and related studies. 5. Used privately (Holmes & Robins, 1988).
First, spanking has consistently beneficial out- 6. Motivated by concern for the child, not by
comes when it is nonabusive (e.g., two swats to the parent-oriented concerns (e.g., from frustra-
buttocks with an open hand) and used primarily to tion, to show who is boss: Larzelere, Klein,
back up milder disciplinary tactics with 2- to 6-year Schumm, & Alibrando, 1989).
216 Larzelere

7. Used after a single warning, generalizing from Prohibition Amendment seemed a plausible way to
Roberts (1982). Roberts showed that a single reduce alcohol abuse (see also Baumrind, 1983).
warning before time-out reduced the neces- However, the Roberts series of studies and the Lar-
sary time outs by 74% without sacrificing any zelere series of studies have shown that firmer tactics
effectiveness of the behavioral parent are sometimes necessary to enhance the subsequent
training. effectiveness of milder disciplinary tactics. The in-
8. Used flexibly. If spanking does not work, par- creased effectiveness of milder disciplinary tactics
ents should try other approaches and other should then reduce the risk of escalation during sub-
tactics rather than increasing the intensity of sequent discipline incidents. This suggests the possi-
the spanking (Roberts & Powers, 1990). bility that how spanking is used is the critical factor
in whether it increases the risk of escalation toward
The first four points are based on multiple stud- abuse. Of course, how other tactics are used within
ies, whereas the last four points are based primarily disciplinary incidents would also affect changes in
on one study each. Thus more and better research is the risk of escalation (see Snyder, Edwards, McGraw,
needed, which is the topic of the next section. Kilgore, & Holton, 1994). Baumrind (1973) noted
that it was permissive parents who admitted more
often to ‘‘explosive attacks of rage in which they
Research on Physical Punishment inflicted more pain or injury upon the child than they
had intended. . . . Permissive parents apparently be-
A major implication of this review is that better came violent because they felt that they could neither
research is needed on physical punishment and on control the child’s behavior nor tolerate its effect
parental discipline in general. Others have noted upon themselves’’ (p. 35).
major gaps in the empirical evidence for currently The second pressing issue is the need for a rigor-
accepted views of optimal parental discipline (e.g., ous evaluation of legislation banning parental spank-
Grusec & Goodnow, 1994; Yarrow, Campbell, & ing. To my knowledge, there has been very little
Burton, 1968). empirical evaluation of the first spanking ban in Swe-
The following problems seem pervasive in re- den (Larzelere & Johnson, 1999) and no evaluation
search on physical punishment: (i) failure to take a of such bans in other countries. One of the stated
development perspective; (ii) failure to discriminate purposes of a major survey on corporal punishment
between nonabusive vs. overly severe physical pun- in Sweden was to see whether the spanking ban had
ishment; (iii) failure to control adequately for the even decreased the prevalence of corporal punish-
initial level of child misbehavior; (iv) failure to com- ment 15 years after the ban (Statistics Sweden, 1996).
pare spanking directly with the alternatives that par- That survey found that more than 30% of children
ents could use for similar incidents; (v) failure to raised after the spanking ban had experienced corpo-
distinguish between disciplinary tactics at the initial, ral punishment from their parents. Furthermore, this
middle, and later stages of extended disciplinary inci- percentage had dropped very little compared to the
dents; (vi) failure to consider whether the effects of generation before the spanking ban (e.g., 32% by
spanking depend on a positive parenting context, fathers, compared to 34% in the next oldest genera-
such as reasoning or nurturance; and (vii) failure to tion). Most of the decrease in physical punishment
appreciate subcultural differences in the outcomes in Sweden occurred before the spanking ban, not
and risks of physical punishment. after it. The biggest changes after the spanking ban
Along with correcting those problems, two other was a small decrease in support for mild spanking,
research issues seem particularly pressing. The first whereas the most problematic types of spanking (e.g.,
is to understand escalation processes within disciplin- spanking of teenagers) had not decreased at all.
ary incidents and the role of physical punishment in It is possible that the prohibition of all spanking
them, when used in different ways. Most cases of eliminates a type of mild spanking that prevents fur-
physical abuse occur during discipline incidents (Ka- ther escalation within discipline incidents. Milder dis-
dushin & Martin, 1981), but there is little solid evi- ciplinary tactics do not get backed up as effectively
dence about what increases or decreases the risk of and thus are less likely to be used. Palmerus and Scarr
escalation toward abuse. (1995) compared American and Swedish parents on
Banning all physical punishment seems to be a the disciplinary tactics they used. As expected, Swed-
plausible means to reduce child abuse, just as the ish parents used less corporal punishment, but they
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 217

also used less reasoning and fewer behavior modifi- (1994) conclusions about reasoning, it finds the em-
cation tactics, more yelling, and more restraining.5 pirical support for commonly held conclusions about
These kinds of changes in the entire disciplinary sys- nonabusive spanking to be surprisingly weak. An im-
tem might partially account for the 489% increase plication shared with Grusec and Goodnow is that
from 1981 to 1994 in Swedish criminal rates of physi- we must move beyond inconsistently supported over-
cal child abuse of children under 7 (Wittrock, 1992, generalizations to conceptualize and investigate disci-
1995). Many other factors could also have accounted plinary responses more innovatively.
for those increases. The main point is that we need One possibility is a conditional sequence model
rigorous, unbiased evaluations of such spanking bans, of optimal disciplinary responses (Larzelere, in
especially given the lack of scientific support for the press). This model says that parents should begin
unconditional antispanking perspective. with the mildest disciplinary tactic that they think
will be effective in producing appropriate coopera-
tion from a child (e.g., reasoning). If the disciplinary
Research on Parental Discipline in General issue is important and neither an appropriate cooper-
ation or negotiation occurs, then the parent should
In addition to better research on spanking, we back up the gentle disciplinary tactic (e.g., reasoning)
need better research on parental discipline in general. with a nonphysical punishment (e.g., time-out). Only
To understand the role of physical punishment or if the child fails to comply with the nonphysical pun-
any disciplinary tactic, we must understand it in the ishment should the parent resort to nonabusive
context of parental discipline as a whole. spanking (e.g., two open-handed swats to the but-
Much research on physical punishment as well tocks). Each back-up step should be preceded by a
as other disciplinary tactics views a given tactic as single warning (Roberts, 1982). If something like this
invariably effective or invariably counterproductive. conditional sequence is used at ages 2 to 6, then the
There may be more potential in identifying more vs. parent should gradually phase out the ultimate back-
less effective ways to use each disciplinary tactic. up tactic (spanking) and then the intermediate back-
The effect of any tactic may depend on the overall up tactic (nonphysical punishment). In this way, gen-
disciplinary style and the parent–child, family, and tle disciplinary tactics such as reasoning will be used
cultural contexts. for most disciplinary incidents and they will be effec-
A major article by Grusec and Goodnow (1994) tive in keeping the child within acceptable behav-
had implications that overlap with this review, even ioral limits.
though it emphasized the opposite end of the contin- This conditional sequence model is consistent
uum of traditional disciplinary tactics. Grusec and with several developmental perspectives whose im-
Goodnow concluded that the greater effectiveness of plications for integrating gentle and firmer disciplin-
disciplinary reasoning than various forms of power ary tactics have not been fully exploited (Baumrind,
assertion was supported inconsistently in the empiri- 1973; Bell & Harper, 1977; Hoffman, 1977; Patterson,
cal literature. Empirical support for the relative effec- 1982; Valsiner, 1987). In general, these develop-
tiveness of reasoning came primarily from samples mental mini-theories recognize that disciplinary re-
with low rates of disruptive behavior problems (e.g., sponses act as part of a control system process, some-
girls in middle-class families). In contrast, reasoning what similar to a thermostat-controlled heating
often did not look relatively more effective than system. Traditional methods of statistical analyses
power-assertive methods in samples with high rates are ill suited for investigating such control system
of disruptive behavior problems (e.g., families with processes, whether cross-sectionally or longitudi-
lower socioeconomic status; children with difficult nally. To illustrate, the average morning temperature
temperaments). in my home during the past year was 61⬚F when
This review focuses on disciplinary spanking, at the furnace was running and 67⬚F when it was not
the opposite end of the continuum of traditional disci- running. This cross-sectional analysis suggests super-
plinary tactics. Similar to Grusec and Goodnow’s ficially that running the furnace causes my house to
be colder. I could design a longitudinal version of
5
Palmerus (personal communication, April 2000) thinks that the this study to prove that running the furnace is either
yelling and restraining done by Swedish parents was less aversive
than that done by American parents. However, she has not divided
positively or negatively correlated with subsequent
her variables into milder and more aversive categories to test house temperatures, depending on the time between
this impression. data collection points (6 or 12 months). This illus-
218 Larzelere

trates that our traditional analytic methods can easily more forceful tactics should not be overused as initial
come to incorrect conclusions about control system or primary disciplinary responses. This is consistent
processes, depending on how we incorporate the ini- with the fact that overly frequent physical punish-
tial presenting problem into the analyses and whether ment has the most consistent evidence of detrimental
our analyses fit the actual time lag of the causal mech- causal effects on children’s behavior.
anisms. An optimal disciplinary control system fits an
Parents use disciplinary tactics as part of a con- authoritative parenting style, which combines paren-
trol system process. Their disciplinary responses vary tal nurturance and give-and-take communication
depending on their assessment of such things as the with firm control when necessary. There are a range
importance of the disciplinary issue, whether the of appropriate ways to combine parental nurturance,
child has been cooperating recently, the appropriate- communication, and control with approximately sim-
ness of the child’s negotiation attempts, and the ilar outcomes for the child (Baumrind, 1991). It is
child’s defiance. Accordingly, Ritchie (1999) showed the extreme parenting styles that yield detrimental
that maternal tactics differ in the beginning, middle, child outcomes, whether the extreme is permissive
and end of an extended disciplinary incident. or authoritarian. Similarly, there are many ways for
The control system perspective has many re- parents to implement a reasonably effective disciplin-
search implications. More information is needed ary control system within a loving parent–child rela-
about optimal ways for parents to maintain children’s tionship. The extremes are most likely to be counter-
behavior in the appropriate range, ways to prevent productive. One extreme would be never or rarely
children from testing the limits, ways to make gentler using firm tactics to set effective limits. The other
disciplinary tactics effective when the limits are extreme would be rarely using milder disciplinary
tested, and so on. Especially for children with more tactics as a preferred disciplinary response before
difficult temperaments or disruptive behavior prob- resorting to harsher disciplinary tactics.
lems, parents must back up those gentle disciplinary A control system process illustrates only one
tactics with power-assertive tactics occasionally, es- possible innovative way to investigate optimal disci-
pecially during the ages from 2 to 6. plinary responses. It seems to be counterproductive
The most consistent finding of beneficial child and simplistic to continue viewing some disciplinary
outcomes of nonabusive spanking is consistent with tactics as invariably good and others as invariably
such a control system process. Spanking was effective bad, as long as those tactics are nonabusive. Another
in reducing subsequent noncompliance and fighting innovative approach would consider how disciplinary
in 2- to 6-year olds when it was used primarily to response tactics influence the parent–child relation-
back up milder disciplinary tactics such as reasoning ship quality and how that quality in turn influences
or time-out. Roberts and Powers (1990) showed that, the effects of disciplinary tactics (Kuczynski & Lollis,
on the average, a brief room isolation was equally in press). The main point is that parental discipline
effective as spanking as a back-up of last resort. research needs to break out of some boxes, both
The control system perspective also has impor- methodologically and conceptually.
tant implications for last-resort disciplinary tactics. In conclusion, this review indicates that we have
At some point, eliminating all forceful backup tactics a lot to learn about the complexities of appropriate
renders the entire disciplinary control system ineffec- parental discipline. Research on spanking does not
tive. This seems to be illustrated by mothers who support an unconditional antispanking position at
used reasoning frequently with 2- and 3-year olds this time. It is more difficult to establish conclusive
without ever backing the reasoning up with negative boundaries between effective and counterproductive
consequences (Larzelere et al., 1998b). Their chil- physical punishment. Spanking as a back-up for
dren’s disruptive behavior increased the most during milder disciplinary tactics in 2- to 6-year olds seems
the next 20 months. Such a failure of last-resort back- to produce beneficial child outcomes, at least in re-
ups could easily lead to the development of ‘‘natter- ducing noncompliance and fighting. Overly frequent
ing,’’ which Patterson (1982) identified as a typical spanking predicts a wider range of detrimental out-
parenting pattern in families of antisocial boys. Nat- comes, but to a degree matched by overly frequent
tering is defined as nagging or scolding irritably with use of every alternative disciplinary tactic investi-
no intention of following through (Patterson, 1982, gated to date. Future research must distinguish be-
p. 112 ; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992, p. 66). tween effective vs. counterproductive physical pun-
A control system perspective also implies that ishment. It must also distinguish between effective
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 219

vs. counterproductive alternatives and how those al- Caesar, P. (1988). Exposure to violence in the families-of-origin
among wife-abusers and maritally nonviolent men. Violence
ternatives compare to nonabusive spanking in similar and Victims, 3, 49–63.
disciplinary situations. Research must also explore Crowne, D. P., Conn, L. K., Marlowe, D., & Edwards, C. N. (1969).
how proactive discipline, disciplinary reasoning, Some developmental antecedents of level of aspiration. Jour-
nal of Personality, 37, 73–92.
firmer disciplinary tactics, and parental nurturance Day, D. E., & Roberts, M. W. (1983). An analysis of the physical
mutually influence each other. Several theoretic per- punishment component of a parent training program. Journal
spectives in developmental psychology suggest ways of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 141–152.
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behav-
to conceptualize such an integration, but those leads ior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and
must be more fully exploited. variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological In-
quiry, 8, 161–175.
Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S.
(1996). Physical discipline among African American and Eu-
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ropean American mothers: Links to children’s externalizing
behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1065–1072.
I thank Sheila Eyberg and two anonymous re- Ellison, C., Musick, M., & Holden, G. (1998). A longitudinal study
of the effects of corporal punishment: The moderating effect
viewers for very helpful critiques of a previous draft of conservative Protestantism. Unpublished manuscript.
of this article. Gershoff, E. T. (1999). The effects of parental corporal punishment
on children: A process model and meta-analytic review. Un-
published manuscript.
Goodenough, F. L. (1931). Anger in young children. Minneapolis:
REFERENCES University of Minnesota Press.
Graziano, A. M., Hamblen, L., & Plante, W. A. (1996). Subabusive
Adams, M. J. (1995). Youth in crisis: An examination of adverse risk violence in child rearing in middle-class American families.
factors affecting children’s cognitive and behavioral/emotional Pediatrics, 98(4), 845–848.
development, children ages 10–16. Unpublished doctoral dis- Grinder, R. E. (1962). Parental childrearing practices, conscience,
sertation, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas. and resistance to temptation of sixth grade children. Child
Alibrando, S. A., Jr. (1987). The effects of corporal punishment Development, 33, 803–820.
and contextual parental characteristics on rebelliousness, neu- Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental disci-
roticism and introversion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, pline methods on the child’s internalization of values: A recon-
Biola University, LaMirada, CA. ceptualization of current points of view. Developmental Psy-
Bauman, L. J., & Friedman, S. B. (1998). Corporal punishment. chology, 30, 4–19.
Pediatric Clinics of North America, 45, 403–414. Gunnoe, M. L., & Mariner, C. L. (1997). Toward a developmental–
Baumrind, D. (1973). The development of instrumental compe- contextual model of the effects of parental spanking on chil-
tence through socialization. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota dren’s aggression. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medi-
Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 3–46). Minneapo- cine, 151, 768–775.
lis: University of Minnesota Press. Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Moral internalization: Current theory and
Baumrind, D. (1983). Specious causal attributions in the social research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
sciences: The reformulated stepping-stone theory of heroin social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 85–133). New York: Aca-
use as exemplar. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
demic Press.
45, 1289–1298.
Holmes, S. J., & Robins, L. N. (1988). The role of parental disciplin-
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adoles-
ary practices in the development of depression and alcoholism.
cent competence and substance use. Journal of Early Adoles-
Psychiatry, 51, 24–36.
cence, 11, 56–95.
Hyman, I. A. (1997). The case against spanking. San Francisco:
Baumrind, D. (1996). A blanket injunction against disciplinary use
of spanking is not warranted by the data. Pediatrics, 98, Jossey-Bass.
828–831. Johannesson, I. (1974). Aggressive behavior among school chil-
Baumrind, D., & Owens, E. B. (2000). Does normative physical dren related to maternal practices in early childhood. In J.
punishment by parents cause detrimental child outcomes: A deWit & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Determinants and origins of
prospective longitudinal study. Manuscript in preparation. aggressive behavior (pp. 413–426). The Hague: Mouton.
Bean, A. W., & Roberts, M. W. (1981). The effect of time-out Joubert, C. E. (1992). Antecedents of narcissism and psychological
release contingencies on changes in child noncompliance. reactance as indicated by college students’ retrospective re-
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 9, 95–105. ports of their parents’ behaviors. Psychological Reports, 70,
Bell, R. Q., & Harper, L. V. (1977). Child effects on adults. Hills- 1111–1115.
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. A. (1981). Child abuse: An interactional
Bernal, M. E., Duryee, J. S., Pruett, H. L., & Burns, B. J. (1968). event. New York: Columbia University Press.
Behavior modification and the brat syndrome. Journal of Con- Kazdin, A. E. (1995). Conduct disorders in childhood and adoles-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, 447–455. cence (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Brestan, E. V., & Eyberg, S. M. (1998). Effective psychosocial Kuczynski, L., & Lollis, S. (in press). Four foundations for a dy-
treatments of conduct-disordered children and adolescents: namic model of parenting. In J. R. M. Gerris (Ed.), Dynamics
29 years, 82 studies, and 5,272 kids. Journal of Clinical Child of parenting. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology, 27(2), 180–189. Larzelere, R. E. (1996). A review of the outcomes of parental use
Bryan, J. W., & Freed, F. W. (1982). Corporal punishment: Norma- of nonabusive or customary physical punishment. Pediatrics,
tive data and sociological and psychological correlates in a 98, 824–828.
community college population. Journal of Youth & Adoles- Larzelere, R. E. (in press). Combining love and limits in authorita-
cence, 11, 77–87. tive parenting: A conditional sequence model of disciplinary
220 Larzelere

responses. In J. C. Westman (Ed.), Parenthood in America. per presented at the conference of the Society for Research
Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press. on Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.
Larzelere, R. E., Baumrind, D., & Polite, K. (1998a). Two emerging Paschall, M. J., & Straus, M. A. (1998). Corporal punishment by
perspectives of parental spanking from two 1996 conferences. mothers and child’s cognitive development: A second longitudi-
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 152, 303–305. nal study. Unpublished abstract.
Larzelere, R. E., & Johnson, B. (1999). Evaluation of the effects Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR:
of Sweden’s spanking ban on physical child abuse rates: A Castalia Press.
literature review. Psychological Reports, 85, 381–392. Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial
Larzelere, R. E., Klein, M., Schumm, W. R., & Alibrando, S. boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia.
A., Jr. (1989). Relations of spanking and other parenting Polaha, J. A. (1998). The relationship between physical discipline
characteristics to self-esteem and perceived fairness of paren- and child behavior problems: A study of group differences.
tal discipline. Psychological Reports, 64, 1140–1142. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Auburn University, Au-
Larzelere, R. E., & Merenda, J. A. (1994). The effectiveness of burn, AL.
parental discipline for toddler misbehavior at different levels Rhue, J. W., & Lynn, S. J. (1987). Fantasy proneness: Develop-
of child distress. Family Relations, 43, 480–488. mental antecedents. Journal of Personality, 55, 121–137.
Larzelere, R. E., Sather, P. R., Schneider, W. N., Larson, D. B., & Ritchie, K. L. (1999). Maternal behaviors and cognitions during
Pike, P. L. (1998b). Punishment enhances reasoning’s effec- discipline episodes: A comparison of power bouts and single
tiveness as a disciplinary response to toddlers. Journal of acts of noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 35,
Marriage and the Family, 60, 388–403. 580–589.
Larzelere, R. E., Schneider, W. N., Larson, D. B., & Pike, P. L. Roberts, M. W. (1982). The effects of warned versus unwarned
(1996). The effects of discipline responses in delaying toddler time-out procedures on child noncompliance. Child & Family
misbehavior recurrences. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, Behavior Therapy, 4, 37–53.
18, 35–57. Roberts, M. W. (1988). Enforcing chair timeouts with room ti-
Larzelere, R. E., & Smith, G. L. (2000, August). Controlled longitu- meouts. Behavioral Modification, 12, 353–370.
dinal effects of five disciplinary tactics on antisocial behavior. Roberts, M. W., & Powers, S. W. (1990). Adjusting chair timeout
Paper presented at the annual convention of the American enforcement procedures for oppositional children. Behavior
Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Therapy, 21, 257–271.
Lonigan, C. J., Elbert, J. C., & Johnson, S. B. (1998). Empirically Rothman, K. S. (1986). Modern epidemiology. Boston, MA: Little,
supported psychosocial interventions for children: An over- Brown and Co.
view. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 138–145. Schenck, E. R., Lyman, R. D., & Bodin, S. D. (2000). Ethical
Lyons, J. S., Anderson, R. L., & Larson, D. B. (1993). The use beliefs, attitudes, and professional practices of psychologists
and effects of physical punishment in the home: A systematic regarding parental use of corporal punishment: A survey.
review. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Acad- Children’s Services: Social Policy, Research, and Practice,
emy of Pediatrics, Washington, DC. 3(1), 23–38.
MacIntyre, D. I., & Cantrell, P. J. (1995). Punishment history and Sears, R. R. (1961). Relation of early socialization experiences to
adult attitudes towards violence and aggression in men and aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal and
women. Social Behaviour and Personality, 23, 23–28. Social Psychology, 63, 466–492.
McClelland, D. C., & Pilon, D. A. (1983). Sources of adult motives Sears, R. R. (1970). Relation of early socialization experiences
in patterns of parent behavior in early childhood. Journal of to self-concepts and gender role in middle childhood. Child
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564–574. Development, 41, 267–289.
McCord, J. (1988). Parental aggressiveness and physical punish- Simons, R. L., Lin, K.-H., & Gordon, L. C. (1998). Socialization
ment in long term perspective. In G. Hotaling, D. Finkelhor, in the family of origin and male dating violence: A prospective
J. Kilpatrick, & M. Straus (Eds.), Family abuse and its conse- study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 467–478.
quences. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Smith, J. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1997). Correlates and conse-
McCord, J., & Ensminger, M. E. (1997). Multiple risks and comor- quences of harsh discipline for young children. Archives of
bidity in an African-American population. Criminal Behav- Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 151, 777–786.
iour and Mental Health, 7, 339–352. Snyder, J., Edwards, P., McGraw, K., Kilgore, K., & Holton, A.
McLeod, J. D., Kruttschnitt, C., & Dornfeld, M. (1994). Does (1994). Escalation and reinforcement in mother–child conflict:
parenting explain the effects of structural conditions on chil- Social processes associated with the development of physical
dren’s antisocial behavior? A comparison of blacks and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 305–321.
whites. Social Forces, 73, 575–604. Statistics Sweden. (1996). Spanking and other forms of physical
Michels, S., Pianta, R., & Reeve, R. (1993). Parent self-reports of punishment (Demography, the Family, and Children
discipline practices and child acting-out behaviors in kinder- 1996:1.2). Stockholm: Statistics Sweden.
garten. Early Education and Development, 4, 139–144. Stattin, H., Janson, H., Klackenberg-Larsson, I., & Magnusson,
Miller, N., & Pollock, V. E. (1994). Meta-analytic synthesis for D. (1995). Corporal punishment in everyday life: An intergen-
theory development. In H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), erational perspective. In J. McCord (Ed.), Coercion and pun-
The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 457–484). New York: ishment in long-term perspectives (pp. 315–347). Cambridge,
Russell Sage Foundation. England: Cambridge University Press.
Mosby, L., Rawls, A. W., Meehan, A. J., Mays, E., & Pettinari, Strassberg, Z., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. W., & Bates, J. E. (1994).
C. J. (1999). Troubles in interracial talk about discipline: An Spanking in the home and children’s subsequent aggression
examination of African American child rearing narratives. toward kindergarten peers. Development and Psychopathol-
Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 30, 489–521. ogy, 6, 445–461.
Newsom, C., Favell, J. E., & Rincover, A. (1983). Side effects of Straus, M. A., & Mouradian, V. E. (1998). Impulsive corporal
punishment. In S. Axelrod & J. Apsche (Eds.), The effects of punishment by mothers and antisocial behavior and impul-
punishment on human behavior (pp. 285–316). New York: siveness of children. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 16,
Academic Press. 353–374.
Palmerus, K., & Scarr, S. (1995). How parents discipline young Straus, M. A., & Paschall, M. J. (1998, August 1). Corporal punish-
children. Cultural comparisons and individual differences. Pa- ment by mothers and child’s cognitive development: A longitu-
Outcomes of Physical Punishment 221

dinal study. Paper presented at the 14th World Congress of Walters, G. C., & Grusec, J. E. (1977). Punishment. San Francisco:
Sociology, Montreal, Canada. W. H. Freeman.
Straus, M. A., & Stewart, J. H. (1999). Corporal punishment by Weiss, B., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (1992). Some consequences
American parents: National data on prevalence, chronicity, of early harsh discipline: Child aggression and a maladaptive
severity, and duration, in relation to child and family charac- social information processing style. Child Development, 63,
teristics. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1321–1335.
2(2), 55–70. Whaley, A. L. (2000). Sociocultural differences in the develop-
Straus, M. A., Sugarman, D. B., & Giles-Sims, J. (1997). Spanking mental consequences of the use of physical discipline during
by parents and subsequent antisocial behavior of children. childhood for African Americans. Cultural Diversity and Eth-
nic Minority Psychology, 6(1), 5–12.
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 151, 761–
Wittrock, U. (1992). Barnmisshandel i kriminalstatistiken, 1981–
767. 1991 [Violent crimes against children in criminal statistics,
Tennant, F. S., Jr., Detels, R., & Clark, V. (1975). Some childhood 1981–1991]. KR Info, 1992: 7.
antecedents of drug and alcohol abuse. American Journal of Wittrock, U. (1995). Barnmisshandel, 1984–1994 [Violent crimes
Epidemiology, 102, 377–385. against children, 1984–1994]. KR Info, 1995: 5.
Valsiner, J. (1987). Culture and the development of children’s ac- Yarrow, M. R., Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R. V. (1968). Child
tions: A cultural–historical theory of developmental psychol- rearing: An inquiry into research and methods. San Francisco:
ogy. New York: Wiley. Jossey-Bass.

You might also like