You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/38039053

Doing Gender in Sex and Sex Research

Article  in  Archives of Sexual Behavior · October 2009


DOI: 10.1007/s10508-009-9565-8 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

34 676

1 author:

Ine Vanwesenbeeck
Rutgers, Expert Centre for Sexual and Reproductive Health and Rights
104 PUBLICATIONS   2,844 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

PhD Gendered Sexuality View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ine Vanwesenbeeck on 08 January 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898
DOI 10.1007/s10508-009-9565-8

INVITED ESSAY

Doing Gender in Sex and Sex Research


Ine Vanwesenbeeck

Published online: 27 October 2009


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Abstract Gender is central to sexuality, and vice versa, but Keywords Gender  Heterosexuality  Sex differences 
there are a number of difficulties with the treatment of gender Social interaction
in sex research. Apparently, it is hard to find a balance be-
tween two conflicting needs. First, obviously, it is necessary
to make distinctions between women and men, for political as Introduction
well as research-technical and theoretical reasons. A second
requirement, at odds with the first one, is the necessity to Gender is central to sexuality and vice versa. As a consequence,
understand gender and its relation to sexuality and the body as gender is a much debated and an extensively studied subject in
much more complex than simplistically referring to two sets sexology, most notably in relation to a number of specific life-
of individuals. This is all the more necessary when one real- size fields of research. Important is the work on gender in
izes the possible drawbacks of exaggerating the differences relation to male and female homosexuality. Here, it is, for in-
between the sexes (in particular when they are biologically stance, shown that masculinity is linked to lesbianism and femi-
explained), because of stereotyping, stigmatizing, and expec- ninity to male homosexuality in some contexts but not in others
tancy confirmatory processes. This essay identifies and dis- (Peplau & Garnetts, 2000; Sandfort, 2005). Overall, it may be
cusses 10 difficulties in the treatment of gender in sex research, concluded that the relationship between gender and homosex-
reflects on their origins, and reviews theory and evidence with uality is strong but complex, and historically and culturally var-
the aim to (1) consider the relative strength of gender/sex as an iable. Another huge area of sexological research, to which gen-
explanatory variable compared to other factors and processes der is pivotal, is the study of intersex, gender dysphoria, and
explaining differences between men and women on a number transgenderism, an area which, without doubt, shows as much
of sexual aspects, (2) inform an understanding of gender and its complexity surrounding gender as that of homosexuality does.
relation to sexuality as an ongoing, open-ended, multi-deter- Most often, however, gender is studied in relation to men
mined, situated, interactional process, with the body as a third and women in heterosexuality. Gender and heterosexuality are
player, and (3) argue in favor of a nuanced, well-balanced treat- strongly and mutually dependent and reinforcing of each other.
ment of gender in sex research. The relationship between gender and heterosexuality is so basic
and comprehensive, that it has been proposed to use the phrase
‘‘heterogender’’ as referring to ‘‘the asymmetrical stratification
of the sexes in relation to the historically varying institutions of
patriarchal heterosexuality’’ (Ingraham, 1996, p. 169).
Since there is no such thing as a free lunch, it is in particular
This essay is based on the Presidential Address, International Academy in the study of men and women in heterosexuality that the
of Sex Research, Leuven, Belgium, July 9–12, 2008. treatment of gender in sex research shows a number of diffi-
culties. Heterosexuality is broadly defined here, referring to all
I. Vanwesenbeeck (&)
sexually inspired social interaction between women and men
Rutgers Nisso Groep, P.O. Box 9022, 1071 GD Utrecht,
The Netherlands (alone or in groups), and the (institutionalized) normativity
e-mail: i.vanwesenbeeck@rng.nl surrounding it. In this essay, I will identify and discuss 10

123
884 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

difficulties in the treatment of gender in (hetero)sex research evolution of the meaning of ‘‘gender’’ continues to be the subject
and review theory and evidence illustrating them. I do not of feminist debate (e.g., Crawford & Fox, 2007; Zurbriggen &
pretend to be exhaustive in this review; I will only pull some Sherman, 2007).
tracks through the available literature. References will be Generally, a shift from modern to postmodern approaches of
mostly to articles published after 2000, except for some earlier the concept can be seen, with emphasis shifting from sociali-
key publications. In addition, I will present and illustrate an zation to performativity, from difference to diversity, etcetera
interactional, contextual, process-oriented perspective on gen- (cf. Cameron, 2005). The phrase ‘‘doing gender’’ is one illus-
dered sexual behavior. The aim of this exercise is threefold: (1) tration of this, although my point here will be that these changes
to discuss the explaining strength of gender (or sex) in sexu- have met with too little response in academia in its entirety,
ality, relative to other explaining variables, (2) to consider specifically in sex research. In any case, the meaning of the con-
gender and sexuality as ongoing social processes, and (3) to cept of gender seems to have evolved into something more gen-
argue for a nuanced, well-balanced treatment of gender in sex eral than originally meant, referring to (and this is in the words of
research. Jackson, 2006): ‘‘All aspects of the distinction and division
between male and female’’ (p. 106). In this essay, I will use the
words sex and gender interchangeably where they basically
‘‘Gender’’ versus ‘‘Sex’’ refer to the same global division, but either one or the other
where specifically applicable.
The first difficulty pertains to the fact that in sex research (as well
as in other disciplines), ‘‘gender’’ is often used when actually bio-
logical ‘‘sex’’ is referred to, i.e., concrete, embodied men and Gender and Sexuality
women. Looking at the evolution of gender as a concept (see
Moi, 2005), the following development can be seen. The dis- A second difficulty relates to the fact that gender is mostly seen
tinction between sex and gender in the English language was first as preceding and determining of sexuality, despite at least some
developed in the 1950s and 1960s by psychiatrists working with claims of gender and sexuality being mutually related in a two-
intersexed and transsexual patients. Then, in the 1970s, the con- way process. Throughout history, there have been different epis-
cept was adapted by feminist scholars as an antidote against temological arguments in relation to gender and sexuality. Rich-
biological determinism, or as Moi (2005) formulated it, ‘‘as a ardson (2007), an English sociologist, recently described the
barricade thrown up against the insidious pervasiveness of sex’’ evolution of the understanding of the relationship between gen-
(p. 15). In particular, Gayle Rubin introduced her ‘‘sex/gender der and sexuality. She illustrates that the prioritization of gender
system’’ in 1975, with gender being ‘‘the oppressive result of a over sexuality has been dominant throughout, although she also
social production process,’’ referring to cultural femininity and points to Freud and MacKinnon (remarkably combined) as two
masculinity as opposed to concrete female and male bodies, to examples of the exceptions who saw it the other way around, i.e.,
the social level, to social norms, as well as to personal identity on sexuality as the basic driving force and thereby constitutive of
the level of the individual. gender. Be this as it is, it’s Richardson’s crucial assertion that
Gradually, however, ‘‘gender’’ lost its original meaning. The now has come the time to acknowledge that gender and sexu-
word is now used more and more to simply refer to concrete men ality are inherently co-dependent. Others before her (e.g., Bur-
and women. All sorts of forms and questionnaires routinely man, 2005; Connell, 1985; Jackson, 2006) also argued that gen-
require us to tick the box for female or male gender. And in der and sexuality are fundamentally conflated categories, funda-
academia many of us, myself included, have used ‘‘gender’’ mentally intersected, inextricably interwoven, ‘‘mutually inform-
when actually ‘‘sex’’ is meant in our studies and publications. ing.’’ In particular, the relationship between gender and hetero-
Moi (2005), in discussing this subject matter, explains it, at least sexuality is tight and reciprocal. Heterosexuality is a key-site for
for the U.S., with the observation that the good thing about the the intersection between gender and sexuality.
word gender is that it provides for avoiding the word ‘‘sex,’’ Moreover, Richardson stresses that the relationship between
something very much welcomed in what she calls ‘‘the latest gender and sexuality is dynamic, fluid, and unstable, although
puritanical backlash’’ (p. 87). Specifically in sex research, dom- certainly patterned as well. She characterizes the relationship
inated as many other disciplines by the English language, the between the two as ‘‘patterned fluidity.’’ And she introduces a
possible confusion between sex as referring to males and fe- new metaphor to describe the intersection between gender and
males and sex as referring to sexuality may also have motivated sexuality: the shoreline, in which she proposes that the ‘‘land =
the popularity of the word gender. Particularly bitter in this gender’’ because of its greater fixity, and the ‘‘sea = sexuality’’
development is, however, that gender is now often used in ways because of its greater fluidity. I am sympathetic towards this
it was meant to fight in the first place, i.e., with a biological metaphor. When encountering shorelines in all their variety and
determinist perspective. To Gayle Rubin and others, this is difference, one can well relate to the idea of gender and sexuality
probably a thorn in the flesh; to me, it sometimes is. The related as those boundaries in eternal motion, in some places

123
Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898 885

more defined, or ‘‘thickly coded,’’ than in others, always af- subjects may be characterized as Tiefer (1995) did for Masters
fected by local conditions, capable of rapid or major transfor- and Johnson’s weeding out of subjects who were ‘‘unable to
mation (think of tsunamis or earthquakes), but always subtly respond sexually,’’ i.e., as research on human singing behavior
changing with the tides or through construction or erosion. And by using the Metropolitan Opera Singers as a sample.
one can see how various levels of influence are at work, bio- Next, and not in the least, of course, there are political reasons
logical in the physical conditions of sea and land, institutional in to be utterly sensitive to sex differences. Gender sensitivity is
the form of laws and regulations, and ideological in the form of prerequisite in gender emancipatory processes, the emancipation
discourses of tourism, farming, and coastal management. How- of women to begin with. Historically speaking, the acknowl-
ever, I don’t succeed adequately symbolizing the individual as edgement of, sensitivity for, and the very study of sex/gender
an active agent in this metaphor, interacting with other indi- differences cannot be seen other than as a huge scientific (and
viduals and with his or her social surroundings, doing gender societal) benefit and advance after decennia of male-dominated
and doing sexuality on a day-to-day basis. Considering the point and male-centered research. This is also why calls to ‘‘go beyond
I am about to make in this essay, I feel this detracts from the difference’’ have also met with criticism from feminist scholars.
adequacy of the metaphor. In symbolizing the ever-changing Some have stated that it is (and these are the words of Bordo,
interconnectedness between gender and sexuality, however, it is cited in McPhail, 2004, p. 14) ‘‘too early to leave patriarchy off
fully satisfactory. the hook via postmodern heterogeneity.’’ Others have argued
that ‘‘although gender may be a social construct, sexism is real’’
(McPhail, 2004, p. 15) or ‘‘the postmodern approach is theoret-
Preoccupation with Difference
ically strong but politically weak.’’ Moreover, and obviously, the
scientific evidence is one more good reason to address gender
A third difficulty is a persistent, even renewed and sometimes
differences and this essay cannot do without a quick review of it.
polarizing preoccupation with gender/sex differences in sex
research, despite repeated calls to ‘‘move beyond the question
of difference’’ (Tolman, Striepe, & Harmon, 2003, p. 5). The
persistent use of gender (and sexual orientation) as dichotomous The Evidence for Difference
and the consequent creation of the category of ‘‘other’’ has also
been criticized in the clinical sphere (e.g., Knudson-Martin & In addressing the evidence for difference, I will limit myself to a
Laughlin, 2007) and a nuanced approach to gender differences short characterization of the most often discussed sex differ-
in sex therapy has been called for (e.g., McCarthy & Bodhar, ences (leaving biological differences aside for reasons of social
2005). It seems that it has been persistent through history that scientific focus and space) and say something roughly about the
‘‘the pull of categorical thinking has been very strong’’ (Connell, strength or size of these differences.
1985, p. 265). Recently, however, dichotomous, binary thinking
has intensified and renewed with the popularity of sociobiology
(e.g., in evolutionary psychology), in which it is claimed that sex Gender Differences in the Cognitive Realm
differences are hard-wired. Not in the least within this disci-
pline, it is that a related and crucial fourth difficulty shows: There is consistent evidence for sex differences on a number of
Gender/sex differences are often exaggerated, even if evidence sex related attitudes, among which the notably large effects
shows that they are small. And in discussing these difficulties (d [ .65) are found on attitudes towards casual sex (Oliver &
below, I will relate them to a fifth one: Certain aspects of sex Hyde, 1993). In addition, women are quite consistently found to
research methodology (sometimes inescapably) inflate sex dif- be more relation oriented and partner-focused, men more ori-
ferences and reinforce the double standard. ented towards self-, body-, or recreation (e.g., Impett & Peplau,
Clearly, there are many arguments in favor of ‘‘categoricali- 2003; Patrick, Maggs, & Abar, 2007). In their recent YSEX
sation’’ (Connell, 1985). First and beyond discussion, there are study, Meston and Buss (2007) found that men endorsed all
conceptual reasons for thinking difference in sexology, first and reasons for sex more frequently than women did, except for
foremost between women and men. In addition, there is a prin- emotional reasons such as love and commitment. In terms of
cipal necessity that we categorize, make distinctions. There may sexual self-schemas, men tend to see themselves as slightly less
also be research-technical reasons for a blow-up of differences. loving/warm and reserved/conservative (e.g., Hill, 2007), and as
In animal research, for instance, it is common practice to ‘‘opti- more sexually responsive, experienced, and deviant than wo-
mize conditions to maximize sex differences’’ (cf. McCarthy, men, whereas women see themselves as more romantic and
2007). Whether we like it or not, it may be necessary, to a certain sexually attractive compared to men (Garcia & Carrigan, 1998).
extent, to go over sex differences with a fine-tooth comb. We However, these sex differences in sexual motivations and self-
must realize, however, that this may result in a blow-up of real schemas, attitudes to casual sex excepted, are often notably
differences in much the same way as any conscious selection of small (e.g., Hill, 2007).

123
886 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

Subsequently, there is increasing evidence for sex difference ‘‘initiation assertiveness’’ (for an overview, see Impett & Pe-
in the cognitive processing of sexual information. The larger sex plau, 2003). On the other hand, sex differences in sexual auton-
differences appear to exist in the more or less deliberate mental omy (the feeling that one’s sexual behavior is self-determined)
processes that translate automatic responses into actual urges are no more than a statistical trend in some studies (e.g., San-
and arousal (Gillath, Mikulincer, Birnbaum, & Shaver, 2007; chez, Crocker, & Boike, 2005). Nevertheless, connected to the
Janssen, Everaerd, Spiering, & Janssen, 2000; Laan, van Ste- previous, large differences are found in sexual aggression,
geren, Scholte, & van Lunsen, 2007). There is evidence for coercion, and harassment (e.g., Peplau, 2003; Vanwesenbeeck,
conscious, controlled self-regulation in particular by women, 2008). This is possibly the most robust sex difference in the area
a systematic inhibition or ‘‘downsizing’’ of uncontrolled res- of sexuality and, by contrast, remarkably absent in much of the
ponses. A different research tradition shows that, among men, overview literature on sex differences in sexuality.
on the other hand, a failing inhibition may be an important
factor in sexual aggression by (adolescent) perpetrators (e.g., Gender Differences in Sexual Experience
van Outsem, 2007). In line with this, women scored significantly
lower on sexual excitation and higher on sexual inhibition (Car- Generally speaking, women report somewhat more sexual prob-
penter, Janssen, Graham, Vorst, & Wicherts, 2008). Related is lems and dysfunctions than men do (e.g., Simons & Carey,
the still growing body of evidence on sex differences in memory 2001), although the differences are often not very large, de-
for sexually relevant information (Geer & Manguno, 1997; pending on what is measured. In the Netherlands, for in-
McCall, Rellini, Seal, & Meston, 2007). Men are more likely to stance, 20% of women vs. 17% of men reported at least one
remember erotic or explicit information whereas women are dysfunction in the past year (Bakker & Vanwesenbeeck,
more likely to recall romantic details. Here as well, gender dif- 2006). Meta-analyzing the evidence on self-reported sexual
ferences appear to emerge particularly when explicit or con- arousal, Murnen and Stockton (1997) found only small to
scious memory is involved. However, the within-gender var- moderate effects for gender difference, and they stressed the
iability on measures such as all three excitation/inhibition large heterogeneity in research findings. It’s probably more in
scales as developed by the Kinsey researchers, is much great- the nature of sexual problems or dysfunctions that men and
er than the average difference between women and men (Car- women differ (see, for instance, Kashak & Tiefer, 2001) than
penter et al., 2008). in their prevalence numbers. The sex differences in the area
of sexual victimization are very robust and very consistently
Gender Differences in the Behavioral Realm found, however. Women’s relative frequent experience there-
of is the flip coin of men’s relative prevalent sexual aggres-
When asked about frequencies of sexual thoughts or acts, or sion. In addition, women harbor more negative feelings to-
numbers of partners, men consistently report higher numbers ward sex than do men (Geer & Robertson, 2005). They con-
(e.g., Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, 2001; Peplau, 2003). Men sistently score higher on sexual aversion, shame and guilt, and
show relatively strong sociosexual behavior, but effects are overall score lower on sexual satisfaction, which appears to be
particularly large (d [ .65) when solo acts, such as masturba- particularly true as women grow older (Carpenter et al., 2008).
tion (Hyde, 2005; Oliver & Hyde, 1993) or the use of porn and
other forms of seeking sexual stimuli are concerned. In the Plasticity versus Rigidity
Netherlands, twice as many men (81%) as women (40%) re-
ported some form of porn use in the past six months (Bakker & Finally, there is evidence for sex difference in sexual plasticity, a
Vanwesenbeeck, 2006). Some, for instance Baumeister et al. concept introduced by Baumeister (2000), who reviewed evi-
(2001), conclude that men’s intrinsic sexual motivation is dence that women show more within-person variance over time
stronger than that of women. Others question the factual basis of on many sexual variables, show more response to various socio–
the stereotype, bring forward that the evidence is mixed or that cultural variables, and show less attitude-behavior consistency.
operationalizations are biased towards frequency instead of Baumeister concluded that, in the area of sexuality, men are
intensity (see Impett & Peplau, 2003). Yet others (also) stress more ‘‘natural’’ and women are more ‘‘cultural.’’ As a conse-
that, in intimate relationships, women and men are more alike quence, the intra-group variability may be greater among wo-
than they are different in terms of sexual attitudes and behavior men than among men, in particular when sex differences are
(e.g., Tiegs, Perrin, Kaly, & Heesacker, 2007). relatively strong. For instance, masturbation scores by women
Next, there is a variety of research showing more sexual show a normal distribution, while men show an ‘‘elevated flat.’’
assertiveness and autonomy in men and more compliance and Baumeister’s conclusions regarding women’s greater sexual
inauthenticity in relationships in women (Impett, Schooler, & plasticity (or, as Hyde and Durik [2000] propose, men’s greater
Tolman, 2006). Related concepts on which men often score sexual rigidity) have not met with much criticism, although
relatively high are sexual self-efficacy (the conviction that one his genetically based explanations for the sex difference have
can communicate and act upon one’s own sexual needs) or (e.g., Andersen, Cyranowski, & Aarestad, 2000; Hyde & Durik,

123
Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898 887

2000). Nevertheless, here as well, the question is how large these stigmatizing is crossed sooner when differences are genetically
sex differences really are. explained, and sex differences in sexuality are, more so than
other sex differences, susceptible for genetic explanations (cf.
Cole, Epstein Jayaratne, Cecchi, Feldbaum, & Petty, 2007).
Difference, What Difference? Genetic explanations result in increased efforts to uncover
similarities among group members and hence increase stereo-
Clearly, gender differences are there, and they’re sometimes typing. This is exactly what is happening now within evolu-
substantial. This is particularly true for sociosexuality and re- tionary psychology: there is an endless search for within-group
lated attitudes, frequency of masturbation, seeking of sexual similarities and between group differences, all with the purpose
stimuli, sexual aggression and victimization, and the nature of of theory confirmation. Without doubt, the popularity of socio-
sexual problems and dysfunctions. But it will also have to be biological perspectives has strongly contributed to the polari-
acknowledged that gender differences are often small, certainly zation of male/female sexuality and psychology. However, it is
much smaller than popular opinion wills it. Even when statis- evolutionary psychology in particular that does so whereas
tically significant, they may still be a matter of some percentage evolutionary biology is much more tuned to diversity as an
points only. Hyde (2005) has shown that with a small effect size essential precondition for sexual selection (cf. Bussey & Ban-
of d = 0.2 (a correlation coefficient of .10), 85% of the area’s of dura, 1999). Nevertheless, evolutionary perspectives largely re-
the distributions overlap between groups. Why should all the inforce the status quo of asymmetrical gender relations and are
attention go to the diverging 15% instead of to the within-group subject to many a feminist’ critique. Or, as Tolman et al. (2003)
differences in the remaining 85%? stated it: ‘‘the meanings and realities associated with male and
Moreover, we have to acknowledge that differences found female sexuality are socially constructed to serve political sys-
must, in part, be attributed to methodological bias, one important tems that generate and sustain white, heterosexual, middle- and
bias being that much research is being done among young, col- upper class male privilege’’ (p. 5). The idea is that ‘‘the very fact
lege student samples, among whom gender differences in the of continuing to label qualities and behaviors as masculine or
area of sexuality have been shown to often be substantially larger feminine will foster sex-based stereotypes’’ (Moi, 2005, p. 107).
than among older or more heterogeneous samples. In addition, Specifically, it has been argued that the construction of gender
sex differences in relation to sexuality are relatively easily attrib- difference is bound up with the assumption of gender comple-
uted to genetic, biological processes, while this is, in fact, unfoun- mentarity: the idea that men and women ‘‘are made for each
ded. other’’ (Jackson, 2006), thus fostering heteronormativity. More-
In criticism of the ‘‘overinflated claims’’ or inadequacy of over, the continuous creation of sex/gender difference does not
the notion of binary gender difference, authors have provided only potentially support a stigma on women, but also supports
alternative proposals. Fausto-Sterling (2000) has suggested we discomfort with intersex and transsex phenomena and thus the
have five genders, adding ‘‘herms’’ (‘‘true’’ hermaphrodites), stigmatization of people associated with them.
‘‘ferms’’ (female ‘‘pseudo-hermaphrodites’’), and ‘‘merms’’ In addition, stereotyping can have expectancy confirmatory
(male ‘‘pseudo-hermaphrodites’’) to the usual two. Hyde (2005) effects. Barnett and Rivers (2004), for instance, have argued that
contends, on the basis of 46 meta-analyses, that males and fe- when men and women in couples strongly believe what they are
males are more alike than different on most, though not all, remonstrated with, i.e., that the differences between them are
psychological and some sexual variables and argues for the enormous, they may give up on all attempts to solve their dif-
‘‘gender similarity hypothesis.’’ I tend to concur with Bancroft’s ferences or to deviate from scripted paths. Inflated claims of
(2003) account who (himself citing Frank Lillie) maintains that sexual dysfunction in women may make women feel that they
‘‘there is a dimorphism which stares us in the face,’’ for the are to blame for sexual problems with their partners, while alter-
deconstruction whereof Bancroft finds purpose or justification native factors may be their partners’ inadequate behavior, bad
for, because ‘‘it’s there, like the sun in the morning and the moon communication, or the inadequacy of sexual stimuli. Chances to
at night’’ (p. 290). solve problems may be missed that way. One aspect of the costs
of stereotyping is that the non-conforming behavior gets all the
more considered deviant and thus negatively evaluated. This
Drawbacks of Thinking Difference may also work in the relationship between parents and children.
There is sufficient evidence that there are expectancy confir-
Differences, however, should not be exaggerated, for one be- matory processes going on when parents expect their children to
cause there are clear drawbacks to thinking difference. Many behave according to stereotypes. There is a world of theory
have warned against the exaggeration of gender differences, in informing us on these processes of expectancy conformation.
sexuality as well as more generally, because of the risk of Stereotypes have a general tendency to confirm themselves,
polarization, stereotyping, and stigmatizing. The boundary be- because behavior that confirms expectations is selectively at-
tween typing and stereotyping or between stereotyping and tended to, is more easily stored in memory, is more cognitively

123
888 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

available for recall, and is seen as more relevant and informa- that men are urged to find a balance just as women are, and that
tive, taken more seriously. Thus, stereotyping can be utterly the sexual message for men to initiate and direct but not to domi-
supportive of the status quo and forego social change. We have nate and coerce shows ambiguity as well (Kiefer & Sanchez,
to be careful what expectations to put out there. Besides, any- 2007b), an observation that may have become more applicable
body who is preoccupied with gender differences basically re- through recent years. In addition, the warning for men to take
duces women to their femininity and men to their masculinity care to appropriately balance between mandated sexual preoc-
and this is as oppressive as ignoring difference. Generalizations cupation on the one and sexual impulsivity/compulsivity on the
about gender may be as oppressive as generalizations about sex. other hand may get out there in the nearby future as well.
And last but not least, one of the most sorry drawbacks of the These developments notwithstanding, I am convinced that
preoccupation with sex differences is the fact that various forms the ambiguity in the evaluation of women’s sexuality draws
of diversity get obscured and alternative explanations are neg- heavily on the sexual experience of women and girls particu-
lected. larly, and may, as a matter of fact, cause ambiguous, confused
sexual behavior and experience. Detrimental effects of the sex-
ual double standard for women’s as well as men’s sexual experi-
Confounding Factors ence have been described and empirically confirmed by many.
A relatively recent review is by Crawford and Popp (2003),
Clearly, and pointed out by many, the preoccupation with gen- illustrating, among others, that the double standard ‘‘may cause
der/sex differences obscures the understanding of within-group women to develop a negative sexual identity,’’ that ‘‘the role of
diversity and intra-individual variability (Difficulty 6) and the gatekeeper can have many negative implications for women’s
persistent casualness in presenting sex as explanatory for vari- sexuality,’’ that ‘‘it may lead women to sacrifice their sexual
ability in the sexual realm obscures the multidetermination of autonomy in exchange for social desirability,’’ and that, most
gendered behavior and this casualness persists despite multiple importantly, ‘‘token resistance can cause women to become
claims and evidence of the importance of many other generating disconnected from their own desires.’’ Generally speaking, the
and mediating factors (Difficulty 7). costs of gender role adherence frequently outweigh the benefits.
Pressure to meet gender norms limits social possibilities and
Societal Factors produces negative emotional and behavioral outcomes. In par-
ticular, however, restrictive gender norms, which undermine
Most notably, it is often maintained that the explaining factor in women’s power, competence, and agency, help account for
differences between the sexes is not as much (biological) sex but women’s higher rates of depression, poorer standardized scores
that various societal factors should be turned to as explanatory. on a variety of psychological outcomes, and higher discontent
The double standard is first to come to mind in this context. with sex (see also Sanchez et al., 2005). Moreover, the ambi-
Presently, the double standard is both challenged and alive. There guity of the sexual standards for women might also be an impor-
is a growing body of literature showing local or subcultural tant explaining factor in women’s so-called ‘‘plasticity.’’ One
deviations from it (e.g., Dworkin & O’Sullivan, 2005; O’Sullivan conclusion stands out: to the extent that women’s sexual behav-
& Byers, 1993), but there is much evidence for its persistent ior is more variable and ‘‘plastic’’ than men’s, it is in particular
topicality as well. Jackson and Cram (2003), for instance, who detrimental to the understanding of the sexuality of women as a
studied deviations from the norm in adolescent girls’ accounts group, when preoccupation with sex differences obscures the
about heterosexuality, do find evidence for disruption but at the understanding of within-group diversity.
same time declare that their findings also suggest that voices of Clearly, there is more to societal factors than the double
resistance to the sexual double standard may be muted and indi- standard. From a social structuralist perspective, and I quote
vidual rather than collective. Eagly and Wood (1999) here, ‘‘a society’s division of labor
An important aspect in the double standard is that the norm between the sexes is the engine of sex-differentiated behavior
for women and girls is notably ambiguous. It says be sexy but because it summarizes the social constraints under which men
sexually modest, look sexy but say no, attract desire but mind and women carry out their lives’’ (p. 409). Sex differences, or
your own. An additional way in which the double standard is gender roles, are viewed as accommodations to the differing
utterly double for women is that it positions feminine sexuality restrictions and opportunities that a society maintains for its men
dichotomously as morally good or bad, which has come to be and women. From this perspective, there is a wealth of gender
known as the whore-madonna distinction. Men are more, as division, institutionalized or not, linked to women’s and men’s
Hyde and Durik (2000) formulated it ‘‘pulled in one pro-sex biological potential. However, it’s not the biological potential,
direction.’’ The double standard urges men to be utterly sexual. or sex, per se that causes gender (role) differences to emerge,
It presents men with a sexual mandate it seems. ‘‘Sexual com- but the way society differentially treats these potentials. It does
petence’’ is part—some would say the central part—of con- so with the support of many different political and economi-
temporary masculinity’’ (Frank, 2003, p. 72). Some have noted cal structures and processes, informing and exploiting human

123
Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898 889

intimate relations. There is no room to expand on these here. as did gender, race, age, and socioeconomic status combined
Rather, I’ll move on to the evidence for the relevance of per- (Cooper, Shapiro, & Powers, 1998). However, this conclusion
sonality factors in the enactment of gender. applies more strongly to ‘‘risky’’ sexual behaviors, such as high
frequency of intercourse and high number of partners, than to
Personality Factors prevention behavior, such as birth control and condom use. In
this and related studies, it is often concluded that risk preventive
Within a diversity framework (cf. Cameron, 2005), the ques- behavior needs to be considered from a dynamic, relational per-
tion how are women different from men will immediately be spective. It takes two to tango, a conclusion that is grist to my
met with the question: Which women and which men do you mill when arguing for an interactional perspective, as I will do
mean? There are, to begin with, at least three demographic further on. Other authors have, in addition, stressed that HIV risk
aspects for which substantial confounding has been shown in is related to endorsement of traditional (and rape-supportive)
the literature: educational level, religious affiliation or com- attitudes in (South African) men, but not in women, since wo-
mitment, and, in particular, age. Hyde and Durik (2000), for men’s risks are the product of partner characteristics and male-
instance, illustrated how a number of gender differences in dominated sexual relations (Kalichman et al., 2005).
sexuality almost disappeared when controlling for educatio- Further, some research relates to Big Five personality char-
nal level. A study by Cubbins and Tanfer (2000) provides an acteristics and attachment styles as factors in sexual diversity,
example of how gender differences in high risk sexual behav- although mostly in same-gender study populations. Anderson
iors could, in part, be explained by religiosity as measured by and Cyranowski’s (1994, 1995) work on female sexual self-
the frequency of church visits. The crucial role of age, finally, schemas shows that extraversion is an important factor in the
has been shown in many studies, among which some important likelihood of engaging in sex by young women, while among
meta-analyses by Hyde (2005) for a number of sexual variables older women, whose patterns of sexual behavior and responding
and by Murnen and Stockton (1997) for sexual arousal, but also may be more established, the dimension of neuroticism appears
for sex differences in sexual compliance by Impett and Peplau to be a more important personality variable. Attachment style
(2003) and for sexual excitation versus inhibition by Carpenter has been brought forward in the context of sexual aggression.
et al. (2008). A qualitative study by Johnston and Morrison Mahalik, Aldarondo, Gilbert-Gokhale, and Shore (2005), for
(2007) also provides a nice example in showing how Irish male instance, found anxious attachment (as well as gender-role
subjects’ need to adhere to gender stereotypes lessened as they stress) to be related to controlling behaviors by men towards
matured out of adolescence and how their goals changed from women. However, we don’t know the extent to which these
‘‘being masculine enough’’ to ‘‘being your own man.’’ Basi- personality factors confound with sex/gender.
cally, we cannot say anything about sex differences without Another relevant individual characteristic is cognitive pro-
asking which age category we’re talking about. On the other cessing. As said, there are gender differences in cognitive pro-
hand, however, Fischtein, Herold, and Desmarais (2007) ana- cessing, but cognitive processing may be a factor in sexual
lyzed Canadian data and found that demographic predictor behavior independent of gender as well (as is true for several
variables did significantly improve the explanatory power of attitudes). A recent study by McCall et al. (2007) investigated
multivariate models, but that the significance of sex (called recall of love/emotional bonding versus erotic/explicit stimuli
gender by them as well) as predictor of sexual attitudes and and found that levels of recall of love stimuli were negatively
behaviors was hardly diminished with the addition of these associated with sexual desire and positively with sexual satis-
variables. faction, independent of one’s sex. There is an increasing interest
Some emerging evidence argues that it’s not as much about in the role of cognitive processing, and this research shows that,
differences between men and women but at least as much about besides looking at sex differences in sexuality related cognitive
differences according to scores on masculinity or femininity processing, the role of cognitive processing independent of
measures, i.e., one’s gender role orientation or gender identity. one’s sex needs further study as well. Finally, the mediating, or
This evidence introduces gender as confounding for sex, and even generating role, of earlier experiences in sex differences in
has, for instance, been shown for relational maintenance strat- sexuality needs acknowledgement and attention. In this context,
egies (Ayler & Dainton, 2004), sexual self-perceptions (Garcia the large differential in (childhood) victimizing experiences and
& Carrigan, 1998), and assertiveness, initiation, and satisfaction its relation to both sexual aggression (e.g., Malamuth, 2003) as
(Greene & Faulkner, 2005). well as sexual victimization (e.g., Hines, 2007) and sexual sat-
Subsequently, sex related motives, attitudes, and beliefs have isfaction in general is probably the most telling.
been shown to be confounding for sex, for instance in relation to So what I’ve tried to do here is mention some factors that may
sexual risk behavior. The risk behavior of women as well as men be as important for sex differences found in relation to sexuality
is found to vary with their sexual motivations (e.g., Gebhardt, than ‘‘sheer’’ being male or female is, and may account for large
Kuyper, & Dusseldorp, 2006). One study even found motives to but often unattended within-gender differences. One might ask:
account for more than twice as much variance in risky practices Where is the body in all of this?

123
890 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

Perspectives on the Body Young chose her title right, because from the meta-meta-anal-
ysis by Hyde (2005) on gender differences it turned out that, by
There is a wealth of knowledge from various disciplines to be far, the largest sex differences have been found with regard to
presented about the body in relation to gender and sexuality. throwing velocity and speed. However, some sex differences in
I’ll modestly stick to highlighting a couple of perspectives. the sexual realm were next in line in terms of effect sizes (be it at
a large distance), and this suggests that the handicaps women
Genes, Hormones, and the Brain experience because of their objectification may extent into
sexuality in a substantial way.
First, one obvious perspective is, of course, the biological one, This has been a point indeed in ‘‘objectification theory,’’ first
looking at physiological, hormonal, and brain characteristics formulated by Fredrickson and Roberts (1997). Objectification
and processes. As a social scientist, I will not get into these, but, theory posits that girls’ and women’s internalized observer’s per-
even as a social scientist, it’s fit to say that, despite considerable spective leads to self-objectification and habitual body monitor-
progress in this area, many uncertainties remain. There are con- ing, which, in turn, can increase women’s opportunities for shame
flicting results concerning the role of hormones and their actual and anxiety. And because women are vigilantly aware of their
working in relation to the organization of the brain, or to sexual outer body appearance, they may be left with fewer perceptual
behavior, is still much of a riddle. Likewise, a lot of questions resources available for attending to inner body experience. This is
concerning the role of genes remain to be answered. There are a the so-called ‘‘limited resources perspective’’: minding the outer
number of studies on twins raised apart that show a relatively body distracts from awareness of the internal bodily states. This
small role for genes but a substantial contribution of non-shared phenomena was described as ‘‘spectatoring’’ by Masters and
environmental influences (Bussey & Bandura, 1999). Never- Johnson as early as 1970 and they argued that this division of
theless, it is beyond dispute that biology is an important com- attention greatly hinders women’s sexual satisfaction. The liter-
ponent in gendered sex, but it is also widely accepted now that ature presenting evidence that indeed self-objectification and
biology is not all determining. Moreover, the foundational status sexual dysfunction are related (e.g., Steer & Tiggemann, 2008) is
of the body is now disputed, at least among some social scien- now growing, as well as is the literature on the omnipresent media
tists. Fausto-Sterling (2000) has argued this, and it has also been as an important contributor to objectification and sexualization of
illustrated in the growing literature on neurobiological conse- women and girls (see Ward & Harrison, 2005), and historical
quences of trauma, not in the least child sexual abuse (e.g., shifts towards an increasing sexualization of the male body in
Bremner, 1999; Noll, 2008). When personal experience as well media and fashion has been noted as well (e.g., Rohlinger, 2002).
as culture may influence neuronal development and brain struc- An American Psychological Association Task Force on the
ture, experience and culture are ingrained into the body instead Sexualization of Girls (2007) has recently reviewed the evidence
of (or in addition to) the other way around. Obviously, we still concerning the sexualization of women and girls and suggested
have a long way to go in understanding how culture is written that, because of the limited resources perspective, women may
into the biological body. not only be hampered in their sexual lives by self-objectification
and hypervigilance of their bodies, but also in scholarly and sports
Objectification Theory accomplishments as well as in professional ambitions.
Remarkably absent in this otherwise important and excel-
Another perspective on the body is looking not as much at the lent body of literature on the negative consequences of the
body as a biological entity, but more at the way the body is sexualization of women and girls are considerations on (the
treated in society. The cultural objectification of the female conditions under which) sexualization may be capitalized
body is the most important angle here. One landmark relatively upon and have positive, empowering consequences (see also
early work in this context is an essay by Young (1990), entitled Vanwesenbeeck, 2009). The discussion on female sexual
‘‘Throwing like a girl.’’ Heavily drawing on Simone de Beau- power limits itself mostly to assertions that women should not
voir, Young argued that the objectification of women’s bodies only be looked at as victims of objectification, because they
make her experience her body as a thing that exists primarily as also resist this oppression (Young has, for instance, criticized
looked at and acted upon. This has consequences for women’s her own work in this line), or that female sexual power is no
body comportment and conduct, which is often ambiguous, hes- real power because it’s not under women’s control (just get old
itant, and contradictory. Motion that is primarily experienced as and it’s gone), but I have not come across many lines of rea-
looked-at is felt to be not entirely under one’s control. Moreover, soning on if and when objectification might have other than
‘‘bold outward-directedness’’ is at odds with traditional femi- oppressive effects in the first place. As one consequence,
ninity. All this makes women take distance from their bodies, among others, of this omission, we still do not know to what
doubt their bodies’ capacities, and results in a lack of experience extent, when, or how the experience of objectification may
in using the body effectively. Young (1990) concludes: ‘‘Wo- relate to arousal and lust in positive ways, as was also noted by
men in sexist society are physically handicapped’’ (p. 153). Segal (1994).

123
Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898 891

Poststructuralist Views Zimmerman, 1987, p. 126). The phrase ‘‘doing gender’’ spoke to
the imagination of many and came to refer to the day-to-day
One final perspective on the body I will spend a couple of words enactment of prescribed social roles and the double standard.
on is the poststructuralist perspective. It has been maintained by Recently, Jackson (2006) emphasized that ‘‘doing gender’’ re-
Butler (1990, 1993) and others that the sexed body, just like fers both to everyday interpretive interaction through which a
gender, is variable and historical, a cultural construct. From this socially intelligible reality is produced, as well as actual prac-
point of view, the body is constructed by gender, is the perfor- tical activities—having sex, getting dressed for work, organiz-
mative effect of gender. One aspect here is that our knowledge ing a night out—constructing a gendered, (hetero)sexual perfor-
of ‘‘biological facts’’ is always filtered through ideas about gen- mance. In this vein, doing gender is both seeing and doing,
der. Regardless of one’s appreciation of a perspective on the interpreting and acting, all in daily interaction with others within
body as a construct, it is undoubtedly important to acknowledge a specific context.
that our ways of knowing, also of knowing the material body, are In the same year as West and Zimmerman introduced ‘‘doing
heavily informed by cultural norms and visions. Surely, we have gender,’’ Deaux and Major (1987) presented their landmark
still a long way to come to bridge the conceptual gap between the Interactive model of gender-related behavior. They developed
biological and the cultural or symbolic, between the body in the model as an answer to the evidence that gender typed behav-
terms of chromosomes and hormones, and gender in terms of the ior (e.g., nurturance by women, or men dashing to the help of
social and the psychological. Clearly, the body and gender are in women) was typically a ‘‘now you see it now you don’t’’ phe-
many ways related, not least in the area of sexuality, but nomena. They made their model to capture both stability and
the complexity of these relationships we are only beginning to flexibility, to stress display (now often called enactment) rather
grasp. than acquisition of gendered behavior, and to build on proximal
So I would like to conclude these reflections with Difficulty rather than distal factors. According to the model, the display of
8: There are difficulties understanding the relationship between gender typed behavior depends on characteristics of the behav-
gender and the body (or between biology and sexuality for that ior, the situation, the interacting partner, and the subject him/
matter). Although one conclusion may be rightly drawn: when it herself, and on social processes, such as selective attention, inter-
comes to sex differences in sexuality, it may not as much be pretation and recall, regulation and self-regulation, and expec-
about sex in terms of the male or female body, but about the way tancy confirmation. And there is no going beyond mentioning
female and male bodies are treated and looked upon, and in the Butler (1993), working within a philosophical instead of social
rebound, experienced by the subject, or in the words of Moi psychological framework, who introduced the related concept of
(2005): ‘‘We are continuously making something of what the ‘‘performative gender’’ and argued that gender is not an identity,
world continuously makes of us, our existence is always a be- but an activity that is performed, enacted, and reconstituted in
coming’’ (p. 117). Which brings me to an interactional and everyday life, based upon reactions and feedback from others.
process-oriented perspective on gender and sexuality. Gender is a phenomenon brought into being when it’s performed,
an ongoing accomplishment produced by people’s repeated ac-
tions, not something that is acquired once and for all at an early
The Interactional, Process-Oriented Perspective stage of life.
So there is plenty of interaction-oriented theory from various
Through social scientific history, the interactional perspective disciplines, stressing that gender, and sexuality, are put into
has been taken by many illustrious philosophers, such as Sartre effect in an ongoing day-to-day process of situated interactive
and de Beauvoir (stressing the generative nature of practice as interpretation and performance. At the same time, there is only
foundational in human identity) and by Goffman (1967), for a modest interaction-oriented sex research tradition on how
instance, in Interaction Ritual. Moreover, it has culminated in gender is ‘‘negotiated’’ or enacted in day-to-day (hetero)sex-
important bodies of theory such as social learning theory, social ual(ly tainted social) interaction. Thus, the following difficulties
role theory, and symbolic interactionism. More specifically in are certainly applicable to sexology. Gender is mostly referred
the area of sexuality, Simon and Gagnon’s script theory and to as (either a cultural or) an individual phenomena, despite a
Risman and Schwartz’ microstructural approach are of note. many assertions of the relevance of the interpersonal, interac-
More specifically in the area of gender, some additional ap- tional level (Difficulty 9). In addition, gender is often presented
proaches are noteworthy. First, the introduction of the axiom as a static, coherent individual attribute, despite assertions that
‘‘doing gender’’ (West & Zimmerman, 1987) was groundbreak- gender should be looked at as a plural, fluid, and foremost sit-
ing. West and Zimmerman innovatively proposed an under- uated process (Difficulty 10).
standing of gender as a routine, methodical, and recurring Nevertheless, some applicable evidence for an interactional,
accomplishment. ‘‘Doing gender’’ is an individual act, but it’s a process-oriented perspective on gendered sexuality is available.
situated doing, carried out in the virtual or real presence of others In considering the format in which this evidence could best be
who are presumed to be oriented to its production’’ (West & presented, taking the full benefit of available theoretical insight

123
892 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

at the same time, I decided to lean on Deaux and Major’s model. evaluated as a reputational attribute among low income Amer-
Without pretending to reach the level of sophistication that char- ican men than among high incomers (Aronson, Whitehead, &
acterizes their work, I took the liberty to apply their model to the Baber, 2003). Evidence for women, on the other hand, shows
applicable sexological evidence, an exercise that resulted in an that being ‘‘too obviously sexual’’ is evaluated more negatively
interactional, contextual perspective on gendered sexuality. and provokes more public distaste when being seen in working-
class women as opposed to in ‘‘women with independent life-
styles’’ (Skeggs, 2004). Thus, evaluations vary with the wider
‘‘Doing Gendered Sexuality’’ context, among others with the norms within specific social
groups. Typical gendered behavior is more likely to be positively
Gender typed sexual behavior (for a characterization based on evaluated and thus to be enacted in lower-income, working-class
what is previously presented in this essay, see Fig. 1) is seldom social strata (and this is most probably related to the larger gender
enacted in its pure form, and sometimes strongly contradicted. differences among the lower educational groups, as referred to
The question I will treat here is: what are the conditions that earlier).
make it relatively likely that sexuality is enacted in these ste-
reotypical gendered ways? Following Deaux and Major, the Situation-Related Aspects
enactment of stereotypical gendered sexual behavior is depen-
dent upon characteristics of the behavior, the context or situa- To a certain extent, people have some freedom to ‘‘choose’’ their
tion, the interacting partner(s), and the subject or actor. situations. The sexual arena is not the same ground for everyone
and this is partly due to active processes on the part of the sub-
Behavior-Related Aspects ject. On the other hand, situations are differentially available or
accessible to people, not in the least people of different gender.
First, the enactment of gender typed behavior is more likely the Risman and Schwartz (1989) called this ‘‘the social organization
more the behavior is central to the standard heterosexual script. of opportunity’’ in their microstructural approach. A telling ex-
For example, women’s sexual restraint is more socially scripted ample is the extensive commercial sex circuit that is available
than their dysfunction, and the first is much more prevalent than for both heterosexual and homosexual men but much less so for
the latter (although one might suggest that with the continuous women. Frank (2003) stresses that commercial sex provides a
aggressive marketing of female sexual dysfunction, it may be- context, an institution supportive of the articulation of norma-
come more and more scripted as we go along). Related to this, tive gender typed masculinity (and sociosexuality) and that is
the enactment of gendered sexual behavior is more likely the indeed the point here, although I (Vanwesenbeeck, 2005) and
more the behavior is positively evaluated, a notion put forward others, remarkably enough also for the Netherlands (Aalbers,
in role congruity theory (Eagly & Diekman, 2005). Generally 2005) have argued that there is much variety, not in the least
speaking, behavior that meets social norms is more positively much non-gender typed behavior going on in the commercial
evaluated. According to the double standard, sexual initiative, circuit as well. This does not detract from the fact, there are
arousal, and assertiveness, et cetera by women are more nega- numerous diverging opportunities, pressures as well as temp-
tively evaluated than among men and thus less likely. But this is tations for men and women to enter certain situations. The point
not always true to the same extent under all circumstances. to be made here is, that situations with certain characteristics,
Certain behaviors fit certain roles and situations better than make the enactment of gendered sexuality relatively likely,
others, and are more positively evaluated when they do (Diek- characteristics such as the following.
man, 2007). Obviously, there are and have been important his- First, gender roles are not consistently or rigidly in force, they
torical gender shifts, and there is subcultural variability. Evi- are more so when gender is salient (cf. Diekman, 2007) and/or
dence shows, just to mention the example that social class when situations are highly structured and defined towards gen-
matters, that the display of sexual prowess is more positively derstereotypic behavior. For one, sexual interaction in itself is
strongly sex-linked and ‘‘one of the most powerful domains in
FEMININE MASCULINE which men and women feel pressure to enact gender roles’’
(Sanchez et al., 2005, p. 1456), is heterosexual interaction in
partner/love-oriented recreation/body-oriented
reactive (gate-keeper)
y al
proactive (sexual mandate)
particular. Heterosexuality is the pre-eminent context for gen-
dered behavior. The homosexual context, by contrast, is much
cognitive inhibition
x x u
failing inhibition, excitation
less monolithically inviting of typical feminine or masculine
se
ambivalence, hesitance assertiveness, initiative
plasticity
outer-body spectatoring
victimisation
s e
rigidity
awareness inner bodily cues
aggression
sexual behavior. Other situations where gender is notably salient
are, for instance: a commercial sex club, a sorority house, a
singles bar, gender typical work or sports, dance cultures, etc.
aversion, shame, problems pre-occupation, performance
Dance cultures, such as Hip Hop, for instance, have been de-
Fig. 1 Gendered sexuality scribed as an intrinsically gendered experience in which the

123
Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898 893

display of gendered sexual behavior is pre-eminently normative overview, see Hyde, 2005). For instance, in a recent study by
(e.g., Muňoz-Laboy, Weinstein, & Parker, 2007). Same-sex Fisher (2007), men exaggerated their sexual experience and re-
groups are often encouraging of gender typed behavior as well ported more sexual partners with female research assistants (but
(e.g., Athenstaedt, Haas, & Schwab, 2004), as has for instance not with male assistants), in particular when first threatened in
been noted for the display of sexual prowess, experience and their masculine identity by having been told that women are
aggression (or at least the boosting about it) by men in same-sex nowadays more sexually permissive than men. Other studies use
groups. Group pressure by peers is a noted factor in sexual watching porn as a situational trigger. One such study shows that
aggression by adolescent perpetrators (van Outsem, 2007). male subjects, after having watched porn for 15 min, showed
In relation to this, Deaux and Major showed that gendered more sexual interest in the female researcher, came to sit closer to
behavior is more likely to be displayed when the situation is her, and could remember more physical characteristics of hers
public as opposed to private/anonymous. One observation in afterwards (McKenzie-Mohr & Zanna, 1990). However, as sup-
this context related to sexuality is provided by sociological re- ported by many other studies on the effects of watching porn:
search that has shown that it is in certain spaces—namely public, these effects show only, or show stronger, among male subjects
mixed-gender, unstructured ones—that women’s bodies are that are strongly ‘‘gender typed’’ or that score high on measures
most stereotypically subject to evaluative commentary by oth- of hypermasculinity and ambivalent sexism in the first place.
ers (e.g., Gardner, 1980, cited in Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Effects among non-gender-typed men may even show reverse
However, most evidence of the public/private distinction being results.
relevant for sexuality related behavior comes from research on
report bias in sexuality research. This research shows, that re-
search subjects are more likely to report in gender typical direc- Partner(s)-Related Aspects
tions when research settings provide less anonymity. Report
bias is notably low and gender differences relatively small in Men’s greater interpersonal power in heterosexuality has been
anonymous research settings, as is shown in many studies by argued by many. Hyde and Durik (2000), for instance, relate it to
now for a variety of sexual variables (Alexander & Fisher, 2003; men’s greater attitude-behavior consistency in matters of sex-
Durant & Carey, 2000, 2002; Meston, Heiman, Trapnell, & uality, since ‘‘men do what they want to do and what they believe
Paulhus, 1998; Weinhardt, Forsyth, Carey, Jaworski, & Durant, in, whereas women often do not precisely because of men’’s
1998). influence on them’’ (p. 376). Clearly, interpersonal, relational
Connected is the distinction between new versus familiar power and gender typed relational (in)dependency are in many
situations. People behave more gender typical when among ways related to ideological, social and economical status and
strangers than when among friends. This is partly due to the fact resources, not to mention physical strength. Generally speaking,
that new situations are relatively confusing of one’s inner states. the display of gender typed behavior is more likely when the
Scripted, traditional, gendered behavior may be something to partner controls resources or rewards for the subject (Deaux &
hang on to in such unsettling situations, providing a welcome Major, 1987). Subjects have more concern with self-presenta-
guideline for what to do when one doesn’t really know. This tion when they are dependent upon their interacting partners,
may explain why gender differences are stronger among young- and are thus more likely to behave in accordance with social
er research samples, for whom sexuality is relatively new and norms. There is evidence for compliant behavior by women
confusing. Several studies indicate that traditionally gendered who are more economically dependent upon their partners, even
behavior is less likely in longer term relationships (e.g., Ka- in abusive relationships.
lichman et al., 2005), and that, for instance, gender differences in In addition, there are many more concealed and more uncon-
sexual compliance decrease among couples that have been to- sciously exerted powers and influences by which a partner may
gether for a longer time (Impett & Peplau, 2003). The standard elicit or enforce subject’s gender typed behavior. Following the
heterosexual script seems to be adhered to most rigidly by inex- interactive model of gender-related behavior, the display of
perienced, younger, adolescent newcomers in the sexual arena gender typed behavior is more likely when the interacting part-
and the same is true for first dates. On the other hand, cohort ner is more strongly gender typed and finds adherence to gender
effects and recent historical shifts may account for relatively norms important. The more interacting partners do so, the more
strong gender typed ingrained habits in the presently older gen- will they be selectively attentive to, selectively interpretative of,
erations, as is for instance noted for Germany by Matthiesen and and strongly expectant of gender typical behavior on the part of
Hauch (2004). the subject. The same is true when the partners are actually more
Finally, specific situations may provide triggers that prime conforming of gender norms, i.e., being more masculine or
stereotypical behavior, as has been demonstrated in several lab- feminine themselves. I am aware I am using the term gender
oratory studies that found gender differences to be enhanced typed somewhat sloppily here by having it sometimes refer to
or diminished as a function of context effects, such as the struc- valuing gender norm adherence and at other times to con-
ture of the testing situation or the wording of questions (for an forming to gender norm (for which I apologize). Crucial is that,

123
894 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

in both cases, these partners are more likely of eliciting gender assertiveness of their male partners. Moreover, the submissive
typed behavior in the subject. This is what has been called sexual role may further enhance ‘‘typical’’ feminine sexual
‘‘norm-sending’’ in social role theory, as one mechanism by problems, such as arousal difficulties (e.g., Sanchez, Kiefer, &
which normative beliefs about gender maintain sex differences Ybarra, 2006), and thus help accomplish ‘‘typical,’’ hampered
(e.g., Wood, Christensen, Hebl, & Rothgerber, 1997). sexual satisfaction in women.
There is not much evidence collected in the area of sexuality
here yet. A somewhat relevant body of evidence in this context Subject-Related Aspects
may be the one related to the inferral of sexual interest. It has
been contended that men who have relatively salient sexual Finally, there are a number of subject related aspects postulated
schemas have those schemas readily accessible, are inclined to in Deaux and Major’s interactive model of gendered behavior to
‘‘oversexualize’’ and infer more sexual interest in their partners make the enactment of gender typed behavior more likely. For
than is actually felt by them (Abbey, Zawacki, & McAuslan, one, typed behavior is more likely when the subject highly
2001). And several studies have indeed found perceived sexual values meeting gender norms. There is diversity among people
interest in a (potential) partner to be predicted (among others) by in this respect. Some believe it is important to be similar to tra-
hypermasculinity and traditional attitudes towards women in ditional norms and avoid gender deviance, whereas others do
men, and by hyperfemininity in women (e.g., Fisher & Walters, not share that belief (Wood et al., 1997). Sanchez et al. (2005)
2003), although there is some contradictory evidence as well have argued that valuing gender conformity in itself negatively
(e.g., Levesque, Nave, & Lowe, 2006). Generally speaking, affects sexual pleasure for both men and women. This works
highly gender typed partners are likely to address the actor spe- through increased contingency on others’ approval and re-
cifically as a man or a woman, thus, again selectively perceiving, stricted sexual autonomy. Their model fits the data for both men
interpreting, expecting and thus eliciting gender typed behavior and women. They call it ‘‘being externally contingent.’’ Others
on the part of the subject. This is more likely to happen when the have studied it as approval motivation (e.g., McCormick, Bran-
partner has less individuated information about the subject. This nigan, & LaPlante, 1984) or social desirability (e.g., Meston
is one reason why gender typical behavior is less likely when et al., 1998). The YSEX study by Meston and Buss (2007), for
people are familiar to each other. Perceived attractivity may instance, suggests that women report sexual motivation in a less
enhance gender typed expectations as well. Levesque et al. constraint (less gender typed) way when less concerned about
(2006) found men to infer more sexual interest in a female social dictates.
partner they find physically attractive, an observation that fits Connected but not the same is the notion that gender typed
Deaux and Major’s account of laboratory studies on helping behavior is more likely when the subject has a gender script high
behavior that show that men behave more masculine with wo- in his or her belief system. There are a number of studies linking
men they consider attractive and women more feminine with belief in the double standard or their opposites to gender typical
men they find attractive. behaviors or their opposites in both women and men, such as in
Next, according to the interactive model of gendered behav- terms of assertive sexual talk, sex refusal, socio-sexual behav-
ior, the interacting partner may trigger stereotypic behavior by ior, et cetera (for an overview, see Sanchez et al., 2005). The
stereotypic self-presentation, in attire or behavior. Evidence explaining mechanism is that people who see sex typed norms as
shows, for instance, that more help is offered to women in typi- self-relevant standards, experience favorable self-evaluations
cal feminine attire, and women who are offered more help will in and feel good when they conform to these norms (Wood et al.,
turn act more helpless (see Deaux & Major, 1987). There is 1997). For instance, Tolman (1999) found that adolescent girls
relevant evidence related to gendered sexuality in this respect. with relatively strong conventional femininity ideologies are
Several researchers have pointed out (for a discussion, see more likely to, very ‘‘girly,’’ embrace romance conventions
Kiefer & Sanchez, 2007a), that regardless of one’s gender, that disable them in relationships. Kiefer and Sanchez (2007a)
having a sexually assertive partner might lead to the adoption of showed that endorsement of traditional sexual roles enhances
a recipient, passive sexual role. An unpublished study I con- women’s sexual passivity but reduces it in men. Kiefer and San-
ducted with Roger Ingham and others illustrated this mecha- chez (2007b) have also found evidence in a number of priming
nism for adolescent girls: When forced to respond to unremit- studies that ‘‘men who do not invest in masculine gender ideals
ting, constant sexual initiative of boys, chances were that girls are more likely to inhibit dominant thoughts following sex
would be totally preoccupied with whether or not they would primes’’ and sooner refrain from dominant sexual behavior.
give in or comply, only dealing with the question ‘‘yes or no’’ Subsequently, and again connected, is evidence that gender
and hardly at all with ‘‘what or how.’’ As a consequence, once it typed sexual behavior is also more likely to be displayed when it
was decided to indeed comply and have sex in such situations, is more consistent with and central to subject’s self-schema or
girls would often leave the whole subsequent orchestration to when the subject is highly gender typed him/herself. Clearly,
the boy, taking the full-fletched feminine passive role. Thus, people vary to the extent femininity or masculinity and, in
girls’ gender typed passivity may be a reaction to the sexual addition, feminine or masculine sexuality, are central to their

123
Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898 895

self-concepts. Behavior that fits one’s self-image is more likely across sexual and non-sexual contexts, appearance concerns
to be displayed. The same is true when behavior is strongly were positively related to both men and women’s sexual prob-
generalized or acquired. If a behavior has been strongly gen- lems. The relationship between body shame and sexual pleasure
eralized in the subjects repertoire across situations and contexts, and problems was mediated by sexual self-consciousness dur-
and has become a firm part of one’s sexual self-image, it is more ing physical intimacy. This evidence as well as evidence pre-
likely to show. This refers to the confounding role of gender in sented before in the context of objectification theory, illustrates
sex differences and related evidence has been touched upon that concerns with self-presentation may interfere with aware-
above. In addition, Garcia and Carrigan (1998) found sexual ness of inner sexual states, sexual autonomy, and sexual satis-
self-perceptions to be more stereotypically gendered among faction. And the evidence shows that women are more likely to
men who score higher on masculinity or women who score do so than men are. Overall, I would say, conditions are still such
higher on femininity. Ayler and Dainton (2004) found women that women loose out more in sexuality than men do when they
as well as men who score relatively high on femininity, to dis- conform to gender norms.
play more relation orientedness. And several studies have sug- The point here, however, is that appearance and presentational
gested that, in general, adopting masculine traits encourages a concerns make gender typical behavior more likely in men as
masculine pattern of sexual behavior for both men and women. well as in women. So it seems that women are relatively likely to
Hypermasculinity in men has been associated with gender- have self-presentational concerns in sexuality, and when they do
based violence against women (e.g., Mahalik et al., 2005) as to a relatively large extent, they are once more relatively likely to
well as with antigay anger and aggression (Parrott & Zeichner, behave typically feminine, thus in an ambiguous, passive and
2008). Some research has focused on the relation between gen- constraint way, with possibly negative consequences for their
der identity and HIV risk/protective behavior and this rela- sexual satisfaction. Among men who are relatively strongly con-
tion turns out to be complex: masculinity may protect women cerned with self-presentation, the thereby encouraged typical
through the workings of higher self-efficacy but may on the macho behavior may not as much hamper sexual agency or auto-
other hand cause more risk because of masculine behaviors such nomy, but may hamper sexual empathy and communication, and
as a higher number of partners and the use of drugs with sex thus sexual satisfaction, of themselves as well as their partners, in
(Lucke, 1998). different ways. These processes, among women as well as men,
Further, stereotypical self-presentation on the part of the sub- certainly need much more study in the future.
ject (in attire or behavior) may elicit gender typical expectations
with the interacting partner, on the rebound making that behav-
ior more likely in the subject. An example given by Deaux and Conclusion
Major is that more helplessness is expected and more help is
offered to women in typical feminine attire, who consequently From this review, it can be concluded that there is a growing
behave more helpless. These processes of expectancy confir- body of theory and evidence illustrating that there is much more
mation as well as of reciprocity in sexual interaction, certainly to gendered sexuality than sexual difference according to ‘‘sex
need to get further attention in sex research, as far as I can see M/F’’ alone. Rather, it should be acknowledged that gendered
there is not much evidence available yet. It would, for instance, sexuality (broadly defined as all sexually tainted social inter-
be relevant to investigate to what extent adherence to the ambig- action informed by gender norms) is a multifaceted, multide-
uous messages in the standard heterosexual script, makes wo- termined social process, strongly affected by societal and per-
men and girls present themselves in ambiguous ways in sex- sonality factors, showing huge within-sex diversity and intra-
ual interactions. This might cause misunderstandings and wrong individual variability, and, ultimately, put into effect on a day-
expectations among their male partners, which may in turn be to-day basis in various sorts of human social relations and
related to sexual violence. Besides, an ambiguous self-presenta- spheres. Moreover, there is ample reason to suggest that gen-
tion may have detrimental effects on women’s sexual conduct dered sexuality is indeed a situated process, more likely to be
and experience, in much the same way as ‘‘throwing like a girl’’ ‘‘done’’ in certain contexts and situations. The valuation (and
hampers their throwing velocity and speed. thus likelihood) of gender conformative sexual behaviors is
Generally speaking, behavior conforming of social norms is historically and (sub)culturally variant to begin with. In addi-
more likely when subjects are more concerned with self-pre- tion, gender typed sexual behavior may be relatively likely to be
sentation as opposed to self-verification in the first place. Re- enacted in situations where, among others, gender is relatively
lated concepts are being high on self-monitoring or high on salient, or in situations that are public, unfamiliar or confusing,
public (as opposed to a private) self-consciousness. This is re- or when less individuated information about subjects is avail-
lated to importance being placed on meeting gender norms, but able, such as flirting situations, first dates, and new sexual rela-
there is more to it. Sanchez and Kiefer (2007) looked at appear- tions among newcomers in the sexual arena. Furthermore, there
ance concerns and found that, although women were signifi- is evidence that gendered sexuality is also more likely to be put
cantly more likely to report appearance concerns than men into effect when interacting partners positively value gender

123
896 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

norm conformity, are highly gender typed themselves, are Andersen, B. L., Cyranowski, J. M., & Aarestad, S. (2000). Beyond arti-
strongly expectant of matching gender typed behavior in their ficial, sex-linked distinctions to characterize female sexuality: Com-
ment on Baumeister (2000). Psychological Bulletin, 126, 380–384.
partners, and/or trigger gender typed behavior with the other(s) Aronson, R., Whitehead, T., & Baber, W. (2003). Challenges to masculine
by gender typical self-presentation. Apart from obvious, con- transformation among urban, low-income, African-American males.
scious social pressures possibly exerted, there may be many American Journal of Public Health, 93, 732–741.
unconscious forms of social influence at play that are often Athenstaedt, U., Haas, E., & Schwab, S. (2004). Gender role self-
concept and gender-typed communication behavior in mixed-sex
underestimated by those who put it forth (a typical Type II and same-sex dyads. Sex Roles, 50, 37–52.
error). However, through social mechanisms such as selective Ayler, B., & Dainton, M. (2004). Biological sex and psychological
expectation, attention, interpretation and recall, norm sending, gender as predictors of routine and strategic relational mainte-
action–reaction, self-regulation, and expectancy confirmation, nance. Sex Roles, 50, 689–697.
Bakker, F., & Vanwesenbeeck, I. (Eds.). (2006). Seksuele gezondheid in
gendered sexuality is a day-to-day interactional accomplish- Nederland 2006 [Sexual health in the Netherlands 2006]. RNG-
ment that is relatively likely to take place when conditions such studies 9. Delft: Eburon.
as mentioned here are met to a larger extent. Heterosexuality Bancroft, J. (2003). [Review of Sexing the body: Gender politics and the
may be particularly inviting of gendered behavior in the first construction of sexuality]. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 289–291.
Barnett, R., & Rivers, C. (2004). Same difference: How gender myths are
place, and thus be co-constitutive to gender in an important way. hurting our relationships, our children, and our jobs. New York:
It’s still a modest number of studies that explicitly take the Basic Books.
interactional, process-oriented perspective. The preoccupation Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity.
with sex differences in sexology obscures both diversity and Psychological Bulletin, 126, 347–374.
Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Is there a
confounding factors as well as these processes, conditions, and gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views,
triggers that are relevant in putting gendered sexuality into effect. conceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Per-
Surely, they deserve much more attention than they have re- sonality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 242–273.
ceived so far. Clearly, in studying sexuality, we are obliged to be Bremner, J. D. (1999). Does stress damage the brain? Biological Psychi-
atry, 45, 795–805.
utterly sensitive to sex differences and we need to analyze them Burman, E. (2005). Contemporary feminist contributions to debates
on the level of the individual, both body and mind. However, we around gender and sexuality: From identity to performance. Group
also should avoid overinflating difference and be attentive to Analysis, 38, 17–30.
within-sex diversity, among others according to personality as- Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender
development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–
pects (of which personal gender identity is one). But let’s cer- 713.
tainly not limit ourselves to the individual. Let at least some of us Butler, J. (1990). Gender trouble: Feminism and the subversion of identity.
focus on diversity in sexuality as well as gender in relation to the New York: Routledge.
(changing) wider social context (norms, institutions, econom- Butler, J. (1993). Bodies that matter: On the discursive limitations of ‘‘sex’’.
New York: Routledge.
ics), proximal situational triggers, group social mechanisms, and Cameron, D. (2005). Language, gender, and sexuality: Current issues
couple interactional processes. Obviously, this plea is not a new and new directions. Applied Linguistics, 26, 482–502.
one, but apparently it needs to be made from time to time. Carpenter, D., Janssen, E., Graham, C., Vorst, H., & Wicherts, J. (2008).
Women’s scores on the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales
(SIS/SES): Gender similarities and differences. Journal of Sex Res-
earch, 45, 36–48.
Cole, E. R., Epstein Jayaratne, T., Cecchi, L. A., Feldbaum, M., & Petty, E.
References M. (2007). Vive la difference? Genetic explanations for perceived
gender differences in nurturance. Sex Roles, 57, 211–222.
Aalbers, M. B. (2005). Big Sister is watching you!: Gender interaction Connell, R. W. (1985). Theorising gender. Sociology, 19, 260–272.
and the unwritten rules of the Amsterdam red-light district. Journal Cooper, M. L., Shapiro, C. M., & Powers, A. M. (1998). Motivations for sex
of Sex Research, 42, 54–62. and risky sexual behavior among adolescents and young adults: A
Abbey, A., Zawacki, T., & McAuslan, P. (2001). Alcohol’s effects on functional perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
sexual perception. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 61, 688–697. 75, 1528–1558.
Alexander, M. G., & Fisher, T. D. (2003). Truth and consequences: Using Crawford, M., & Fox, A. (2007). From sex to gender and back again:
the bogus pipeline to examine sex differences in self-reported sexu- Co-optation of a feminist language reform. Feminism & Psychol-
ality. Journal of Sex Research, 40, 27–35. ogy, 17, 481–486.
American Psychological Association Task Force on the Sexualization of Crawford, M., & Popp, D. (2003). Sexual double standards: A review
Girls. (2007). Report of the APA Task Force on the Sexualization of and methodological critique of two decades of research. Journal of
Girls. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Sex Research, 40, 13–26.
Retrieved February 22, 2007 from http://www.apa.org/pi/wpo/ Cubbins, L. A., & Tanfer, K. (2000). The influence of gender on sex: A
sexualization.html study of men’s and women’s self-reported high-risk sex behavior.
Andersen, B. L., & Cyranowski, J. M. (1994). Women’s sexual self- Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 229–257.
schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1079– Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interac-
1100. tive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–
Andersen, B. L., & Cyranowski, J. M. (1995). Women’s sexuality: 389.
Behavior, responses, and individual differences. Journal of Consult- Diekman, A. B. (2007). Negotiating the double bind: Interpersonal and
ing and Clinical Psychology, 63, 891–906. instrumental evaluations of dominance. Sex Roles, 22, 551–561.

123
Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898 897

Durant, L. E., & Carey, M. P. (2000). Self-administered questionnaires Impett, E. A., & Peplau, L. A. (2003). Sexual compliance: Gender, moti-
versus face-to-face interviews in assessing sexual behavior in vational, and relationship perspectives. Journal of Sex Research, 40,
young women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 309–322. 87–100.
Durant, L. E., & Carey, M. P. (2002). Reliability of retrospective self- Impett, E. A., Schooler, D., & Tolman, D. L. (2006). To be seen and not
reports of sexual and nonsexual health behaviors among women. heard: Femininity ideology and adolescent girls’ sexual health.
Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 28, 331–338. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 131–144.
Dworkin, S. L., & O’Sullivan, L. (2005). Actual versus desired initiation Ingraham, C. (1996). The heterosexual imaginary. In S. Seidman (Ed.),
patterns among a sample of college men: Tapping disjunctures Queer theory/sociology (pp. 168–193). Oxford: Blackwell.
within traditional male sexual scripts. Journal of Sex Research, 42, Jackson, S. (2006). Gender, sexuality and heterosexuality: The complexity
150–158. (and limits) of heteronormativity. Feminist Theory, 7, 105–121.
Eagly, A. H., & Diekman, A. B. (2005). What is the problem? Prejudice as Jackson, S. M., & Cram, F. (2003). Disrupting the sexual double standard:
an attitude-in- context. In J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman Young women’s talk about heterosexuality. British Journal of Social
(Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport (pp. 19– Psychology, 42, 113–127.
35). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Janssen, E., Everaerd, W., Spiering, M., & Janssen, J. (2000). Automatic
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in processes and the appraisal of sexual stimuli: Towards an information
human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. Amer- processing model of sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 37, 8–
ican Psychologist, 54, 408–423. 23.
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2000). Sexing the body: Gender politics and the Johnston, C. A. B., & Morrison, T. G. (2007). The presentation of
construction of sexuality. New York: Basic Books. masculinity in everyday life: Contextual variations in the Mascu-
Fischtein, D. S., Herold, E. S., & Desmarais, S. (2007). How much does line behavior of young Irish men. Sex Roles, 57, 661–674.
gender explain in sexual attitudes and behaviors? A survey of Can- Kalichman, S., Simbayi, L. C., Kaufman, M., Cain, D., Cherry, C.,
adian adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 451–461. Jooste, S., et al. (2005). Gender attitudes, sexual violence, and HIV/
Fisher, T. D. (2007). Sex of experimenter and social norm effects on AIDS risk among men and women in Cape Town, South Africa.
reports of sexual behavior in young men and women. Archives of Journal of Sex Research, 42, 299–306.
Sexual Behavior, 36, 89–100. Kashak, E., & Tiefer, L. (Eds.). (2001). A new view of women’s sexual
Fisher, T. D., & Walters, A. S. (2003). Variables in addition to gender problems. New York: The Haworth Press.
that help to explain differences in perceived sexual interest. Psy- Kiefer, A. K., & Sanchez, D. T. (2007a). Scripting sexual passivity: A
chology of Men & Masculinity, 4, 154–162. gender role perspective. Personal Relationships, 14, 269–290.
Frank, K. (2003). ‘‘Just trying to relax’’: Masculinity, masculinizing prac- Kiefer, A. K., & Sanchez, D. T. (2007b). Men’s sex-dominance inhibi-
tices, and strip club regulars. Journal of Sex Research, 40, 61–76. tion: Do men automatically refrain from sexually dominant behav-
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectification theory: ior? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1617–1631.
Towards understanding women’s lived experiences and mental Knudson-Martin, C., & Laughlin, M. J. (2007). Gender and sexual
health. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. orientation in family therapy: Toward a postgender approach.
Garcia, L. T., & Carrigan, D. (1998). Individual and gender differences in Family Relations, 54, 101–115.
sexual self-perceptions. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, Laan, E., van Stegeren, A., Scholte, S., & van Lunsen, R. (2007).
10, 59–70. Genderverschillen en- overeenkomsten in de seksuele respons [Gender
Gardner, C. B. (1980). Passing by: Street remarks, address rights, and the differences and similarities in sexual response]. Tijdschrift voor Se-
urban female. Sociological Inquiry, 50, 328–356. ksuologie, 31, 113–118.
Gebhardt, W. A., Kuyper, L., & Dusseldorp, E. (2006). Condom use at first Levesque, M. J., Nave, C. S., & Lowe, C. A. (2006). Toward an
intercourse with a new partner in female adolescents and young adults: understanding of gender differences in inferring sexual interest.
The role of cognitive planning and motives for having sex. Archives of Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 150–158.
Sexual Behavior, 35, 217–225. Lucke, J. (1998). Gender roles and sexual behavior among young
Geer, J. H., & Manguno, G. M. (1997). Gender differences in cog- women. Sex Roles, 39, 273–297.
nitive processes in sexuality. Annual Review of Sex Research, 9, Mahalik, J. R., Aldarondo, E., Gilbert-Gokhale, S., & Shore, E. (2005).
90–124. The role of insecure attachment and gender role stress in predicting
Geer, J. H., & Robertson, G. G. (2005). Implicit attitudes in sexuality: controlling behaviors in men who batter. Journal of Interpersonal
Gender differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 671–677. Violence, 20, 617–631.
Gillath, O., Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Malamuth, N. M. (2003). Criminal and non-criminal sexual aggressors.
Does subliminal exposure to sexual stimuli have the same effect for Integrating psychopathy in a hierarchical mediational confluence
men and women? Journal of Sex Research, 44, 111–121. model. In R. A. Prentky, E. S. Janus, & M. C. Seto (Eds.), Sexually
Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on face-to-face behavior. coercive behavior understanding and management (pp. 33–58).
New York: Pantheon Books. New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
Greene, K., & Faulkner, S. L. (2005). Gender, belief in the sexual double Matthiesen, S., & Hauch, M. (2004). Verschwinden die Geschlechtsunter-
standard, and sexual talk in heterosexual dating relationships. Sex schiede? Auflösung, Umkehr oder Kontinuität traditioneller Gesch-
Roles, 53, 239–251. lechtsunterschiede im sexuellen Verhalten [Will gender differences
Hill, D. R. (2007). Differences and similarities in men’s and women’s disappear? Dissolution, reversal, or continuity of traditional gender
sexual self-schemas. Journal of Sex Research, 44, 135–144. differences in sexual behaviour]. Verhaltenstherapie & Psychosoziale
Hines, D. A. (2007). Predictors of sexual coercion against women and men: Praxis, 36, 491–508.
A multilevel, multinational study of university students. Archives of McCall, K. M., Rellini, A. H., Seal, B. N., & Meston, C. M. (2007). Sex
Sexual Behavior, 36, 403–422. differences in memory for sexually-relevant information. Archives
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychol- of Sexual Behavior, 36, 508–517.
ogist, 60, 581–592. McCarthy, B. W., & Bodhar, L. E. (2005). The equity model of sexuality:
Hyde, J. S., & Durik, A. M. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plastic- Navigating and negotiating the similarities and differences between
ity-evolutionary or sociocultural forces? Comment on Baumeister men and women in sexual behavior, roles and values. Sexual and
(2000). Psychological Bulletin, 126, 375–379. Relationship Therapy, 20, 225–235.

123
898 Arch Sex Behav (2009) 38:883–898

McCarthy, E. (2007, August). Cellular events during development that Sanchez, D. T., Kiefer, A. K., & Ybarra, O. (2006). Sexual submissive-
determine adult sexual behavior in a rodent animal model. Paper ness in women: Costs for sexual autonomy and arousal. Personality
presented at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Res- and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 512–524.
earch, Vancouver, BC, Canada. Sandfort, T. G. M. (2005). Sexual orientation and gender: Stereotypes
McCormick, N. B., Brannigan, G. G., & LaPlante, M. N. (1984). Social and beyond. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 595–611.
desirability in the bedroom: Role of approval motivation in sexual Segal, L. (1994). Straight sex: The politics of pleasure. London: Virago
relationships. Sex Roles, 11, 303–314. Press.
McKenzie-Mohr, D., & Zanna, M. P. (1990). Treating women as sexual Simons, J. S., & Carey, M. P. (2001). Prevalence of sexual dysfunction:
objects: Look to the (gender schematic) male who has viewed pornog- Results from a decade of research. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 30,
raphy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 296–308. 177–219.
McPhail, B. A. (2004). Questioning gender and sexuality binaries: What Skeggs, B. (2004). Class, self, culture. London: Routledge.
queer theorists, transgendered individuals, and sex researchers can Steer, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2008). The role of self-objectification in
teach social work. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services, 17, 3–21. women’s sexual functioning. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol-
Meston, C. M., & Buss, D. M. (2007). Why humans have sex. Archives of ogy, 27, 205–225.
Sexual Behavior, 36, 477–507. Tiefer, L. (1995). Sex is not a natural act, and other essays. Boulder, CO:
Meston, C. M., Heiman, J. R., Trapnell, P. D., & Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Westview Press.
Socially desirable responding and sexuality self-reports. Journal of Tiefer, L. (2001). A new view of women’s sexual problems: Why new?
Sex Research, 35, 148–157. Why now? The Journal of Sex Research, 38, 89–96.
Moi, T. (2005). Sex, gender and the body. Oxford: Oxford University Tiegs, T. J., Perrin, P. B., Kaly, P. W., & Heesacker, M. (2007). My place
Press. or yours?: An inductive approach to sexuality and gender role
Muňoz-Laboy, M., Weinstein, H., & Parker, R. (2007). The Hip-Hop conformity. Sex Roles, 56, 449–456.
club scene: Gender, grinding and sex. Culture, Health & Sexuality, Tolman, D. L. (1999). Femininity as a barrier to positive sexual health
9, 615–628. for adolescent girls. Journal of the American Medical Women’s
Murnen, S. K., & Stockton, M. (1997). Gender and self-reported sexual Association, 54, 133–138.
arousal in response to sexual stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Sex Tolman, D. L., Striepe, M. I., & Harmon, T. (2003). Gender matters:
Roles, 37, 135–153. Constructing a model of adolescent sexual health. Journal of Sex
Noll, J. G. (2008). Sexual abuse of children: Unique in its effects on Research, 40, 4–12.
development? Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 603–605. van Outsem, R. (2007). Sexually abusive behaviour in juveniles: Devi-
O’Sullivan, L. F., & Byers, E. S. (1993). Eroding stereotypes: College ant and non-deviant pathways. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 13,
women’s attempts to influence reluctant male sexual partners. Journal 169–179.
of Sex Research, 30, 270–282. Vanwesenbeeck, I. (2005). Burnout among female indoor sex workers.
Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 627–639.
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51. Vanwesenbeeck, I. (2008). Sexual violence and the MDGs. Interna-
Parrott, D. J., & Zeichner, A. (2008). Determinants of anger and physical tional Journal of Sexual Health, 20, 25–50.
aggression based on sexual orientation: An experimental examination Vanwesenbeeck, I. (2009). The risks and rights of sexualization: An
of hypermasculinity and exposure to male gender role violations. appreciative commentary on Lerum and Dworkin’s ‘‘Bad Girls
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 891–901. Rule’’. Journal of Sex Research, 46, 268–270.
Patrick, M. E., Maggs, J. L., & Abar, C. C. (2007). Reasons to have sex, Ward, L. M., & Harrison, K. (2005). The impact of media use on girls’
personal goals, and sexual behavior during the transition to college. beliefs about gender roles, their bodies, and sexual relationships: A
Journal of Sex Research, 44, 240–249. research synthesis. In E. Cole & D. J. Henderson (Eds.), Featuring
Peplau, L. A. (2003). Human sexuality: How do men and women differ? females: Feminist analyses of media (pp. 3–23). Washington, DC:
Psychological Science, 12, 37–40. American Psychological Association.
Peplau, L. A., & Garnets, L. D. (2000). A new paradigm for under- Weinhardt, L. S., Forsyth, A. D., Carey, M. P., Jaworski, B. C., &
standing women’s sexuality and sexual orientation. Journal of Durant, L. E. (1998). Reliability and validity of self-report
Social Issues, 56, 329–350. measures of HIV-related sexual behavior: Progress since 1990
Richardson, D. (2007). Patterned fluidities: (Re)Imagining the relation- and recommendations for research and practice. Archives of Sexual
ship between gender and sexuality. Sociology, 41, 457–474. Behavior, 27, 155–180.
Risman, B. J., & Schwartz, P. (Eds.). (1989). Gender in intimate rela- West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1987). Doing gender. Gender & Society, 1,
tionships: A microstructural approach. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth 125–151.
Publishing Company. Wood, W., Christensen, P. N., Hebl, M. R., & Rothgerber, H. (1997).
Rohlinger, D. A. (2002). Eroticizing men: Cultural influences on Conformity to sex-typed norms, affect, and the self-concept. Journal
advertising and male objectification. Sex Roles, 46, 61–74. of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 523–535.
Sanchez, D. T., Crocker, J., & Boike, K. R. (2005). Doing gender in the Young, I. M. (1990). Throwing like a girl and other essays in feminist
bedroom: Investing in gender norms and the sexual experience. philosophy and social theory. Bloomington: Indiana University
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1445–1455. Press.
Sanchez, D. T., & Kiefer, A. K. (2007). Body concerns in and out of the Zurbriggen, E. L., & Sherman, A. M. (2007). Reconsidering ‘‘sex’’ and
bedroom: Implications for sexual pleasure and problems. Archives ‘‘gender’’: Two steps forward, one step back. Feminism & Psychol-
of Sexual Behavior, 36, 808–820. ogy, 17, 475–480.

123

View publication stats

You might also like