You are on page 1of 12

Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105 – 116

www.elsevier.com/locate/schres

Basic neuropsychological dimensions in schizophrenia


Judith Jaeger a,b,*, Pál Czobor c, Stefanie M. Berns a
a
Department of Psychiatric Rehabilitation, Center for Neuropsychiatric Outcome and Rehabilitation Research (CENORR),
Zucker Hillside Hospital, North Shore – Long Island Jewish Health System, Glen Oaks, NY, USA
b
Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA
c
Nathan S. Kline Institute, Orangeburg, NY 10962, USA
Received 20 September 2002; received in revised form 26 December 2002; accepted 16 January 2003

Abstract

Neuropsychological (NP) studies in schizophrenia often require data reduction to avoid statistical type I error from multiple
comparisons. Typically, this involves grouping measures into domains defined by experts a priori based on delineations
validated in brain-injured but not schizophrenia samples (e.g. attention, executive functioning, memory, language visuospatial,
motor). Component measures are arbitrarily selected and validity may not generalize to different samples or within the same
sample over time.
One solution to these problems is illustrated using neurocognitive subdomains based on recent schizophrenia literature, and
validated with data from a longitudinal study (156 subjects) involving repeated NP testing (baseline—within 6 months of
hospital discharge—and 6 and 18 months later). A priori subdomains were grouped and submitted to principal component
analysis (PCA) at each time point. Longitudinal stability of the resulting factors was tested by computing congruency
coefficients. Six stable factors were extracted having good construct, divergent and predictive validity. Five neuropsychological
measures frequently studied in schizophrenia were not correlated with these factors, suggesting that they should be maintained
as independent neurocognitive subdomains. Distinct factors for executive functioning, verbal memory and motor functions
could not be validated; this raises concerns about conclusions of previous studies regarding the pattern, severity and correlates
of specific neurocognitive functions in schizophrenia.
D 2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Neuropsychological domains; Neurocognitive subdomain; Schizophrenia

1. Introduction in schizophrenia has grown steadily in the last three


decades. Flor-Henry and Yeudall were among the
The use by researchers of neuropsychological first in the post-Kraepelinian era to employ NP
(NP) test batteries to profile neurocognitive deficits batteries, previously designed to examine localization
of function in focal brain-damaged populations, in an
* Corresponding author. Department of Psychiatric Rehabilita- effort to reveal pathophysiologic substrates of the
tion, Center for Neuropsychiatric Outcome and Rehabilitation endogenous psychoses (Flor-Henry and Yeudall,
Research (CENORR), Zucker Hillside Hospital, North Shore—
Long Island Jewish Health System, 75 – 59 263rd Street, Glen Oaks,
1979; Flor-Henry, 1969, 1976). Heaton et al. (1978)
NY 11004, USA. Tel.: +1-718-470-8342; fax: +1-718-962-2742. further emphasized the importance of NP tests for
E-mail address: jaeger@lij.edu (J. Jaeger). revealing neurocognitive deficits in the endogenous

0920-9964/$ - see front matter D 2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00052-5
106 J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116

psychoses, concluding that schizophrenia patients had tention, memory, executive functioning, language,
deficits comparable to those seen in brain-injured visuospatial, motor) and averaging the individual
patients. These two influential bodies of work, to- standardized measures within each domain. Howev-
gether with early brain imaging studies reporting er, this approach is problematic: The domains tradi-
abnormal brain structure and function, led a number tionally used derive from delineations valid in focal
of neuropsychologists to employ NP test batteries in brain-injured patients, but not necessarily patients
studies of schizophrenia. Early findings validated this with schizophrenia. For example, schizophrenia
effort, demonstrating marked NP test deficits that studies have revealed that memory, executive func-
discriminated more severe state hospital patients from tioning and attention are overlapping and not neces-
ambulatory samples (Perlick et al., 1992). Imaging sarily valid independent constructs (Gold et al.,
studies subsequently demonstrated region-specific 1997). In practice, the tests within each domain
functional activation with one widely used NP test, are often arbitrarily selected and open to debate.
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Weinberger et al., Indeed our systematic analysis of key studies
1986). Studies suggesting that NP measures are employing this technique revealed surprising differ-
associated with functional disability (reviewed by ences in both the domain grouping of NP measures
Green, 1996) recently fueled a trend in the pharma- and the designation of domains themselves. For
ceutical industry to target NP test performance as a example, Trails B, one of the most commonly used
proxy for a drug’s potential to improve independent NP measures, has been variously grouped under
functioning. These forces, combined with a marked ‘‘Visuomotor’’ (Cuesta and Peralta, 1995), ‘‘Atten-
increase in the percentage of psychologists specializ- tion’’ (Censits et al., 1997), ‘‘Executive’’ (Bilder et
ing in neuropsychology, have led to the current al., 2000), ‘‘Abstraction and Cognitive Flexibility’’
almost ubiquitous use of NP tests in clinical studies (Heaton et al., 2001), and ‘‘Concentration/Speed’’
of schizophrenia. (Hoff et al., 1999). These differences cannot be
The research strategy most widely used for discern- accounted for by differences in the domains desig-
ing patterns of neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia nated by each investigator: The domain of ‘‘Abstrac-
has been to administer a comprehensive NP battery tion/Cognitive Flexibility’’ was distinguished in
consisting of several measures tapping each of several reports by Saykin et al. (1991, 1994), Censits et
putative neurocognitive domains (e.g. Saykin et al., al. (1997), and Heaton et al. (2001), all of which
1991; Bilder et al., 2000; White et al., 1997; Hoff et al., included Trails B in their battery, but only one of
2001; Heaton et al., 2001; Blanchard and Neale, 1994; which grouped this measure within this domain
Censits et al., 1997). Such batteries typically yield an (Heaton et al., 2001) while others instead grouped
impractically large number of variables. Arguments it with a combined domain called ‘‘Visual Motor
have been made for reducing the length of the batteries Processing/Attention’’ (Saykin et al., 1991, 1994) or
used; however, these generally center on the cost and ‘‘Attention’’, as cited above.
subject burden (Gold et al., 1999) rather than more Another concern is that even if distinguishable
basic theoretical grounds. Redundancy in NP testing is domains are validated, this does not guarantee the
valued as an important method for assuring that a few validity of the derived domain measures in a sample
spurious test scores do not yield false conclusions selected differently (e.g. inpatients/outpatients, first-
regarding the functioning of a particular cognitive episode/chronic) or the same sample at different time
domain (Lezak, 1995). However, comprehensive bat- points. Covariance matrices may differ between dif-
teries yield many variables, raising the risk of statistical ferent samples or within the same sample over time.
type I error, and the need for data reduction. Ideally, test For example, if the range on a particular measure is
scores would be aggregated into reliable indices each truncated in one group, but is widely distributed in a
reflecting the integrity of a single neurocognitive second group, the relationship between that variable
domain. and other measures in the test battery may differ
The most frequently used method for NP data between the two groups.
reduction in the literature involves grouping test This report seeks to establish a valid data reduction
measures into conventional domains (e.g. viz., at- method for neuropsychological research in schizo-
J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116 107

phrenia. NP variable groupings, formed based on the With respect to treatment course, at the time of the
recent schizophrenia literature, are studied for their baseline assessment, an average of 81.5 days
validity and replicability. (S.D. = 56) had passed since discharge from the index
hospitalization. Two subjects were not taking psycho-
active medications and six were on psychoactive
2. Method medications other than an antipsychotic. Of the 148
subjects taking antipsychotics, 59 were exclusively on
2.1. Subjects a new generation compound, 44 were on both old and
new generation compounds, and 45 were only on a
Subjects are consenting patients in an ongoing 3- conventional drug. As a whole, psychopathology
year study of schizophrenia and schizoaffective dis- ratings at baseline were in the mild range. Average
order (diagnosed using SCID for DSM-IV) involving scores for each factor using the five-factor solution for
repeated NP testing. Subjects are enrolled within 6 the PANSS of Bell et al. (1994) were 2.6 (S.D. = 8.2)
months of symptom exacerbation requiring hospitali- for positive symptoms, 2.4 (0.82) for negative symp-
zation, and received a comprehensive NP test battery toms, 2.8 for the cognitive factor, 2.9 (0.99) for
and Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS) emotional discomfort and 2.1 (0.71) for hostility.
(Kay et al., 1987) ratings at baseline and again after 6, Ranges for these factors were from 1 to 5 and medians
18 and 36 months. Staff administering NP tests were ranged from 2.0 for hostility to 2.8 for emotional
previously trained and observed in test battery admin- discomfort. (PANSS items are rated on a seven-point
istration to assure uniformity. PANSS raters had Likert scale with a 7 indicating ‘extreme’, 5 ‘moderate
demonstrated interrater reliability compared to an to severe’, 3 ‘mild’ and 1 ‘absent’.)
expert (ICCs z 0.80). The Multidimensional Scale
for Independent Functioning (MSIF) developed and 2.2. Procedures
validated in our laboratory (Jaeger et al., in press) was
administered monthly to capture recovery of life The first step was to a priori identify neuro-
functions. The MSIF distinguishes role responsibili- cognitive subdomains based on recent schizophrenia
ties from support received, and quality of perfor- literature, refining the conventional NP domains
mance in work, school and residential roles, and (see Table 1). A series of iterative procedures
provides global ratings. ensued to identify the constituent NP measures,
For the present analyses, the dataset from this
ongoing study (N = 250) was frozen. Subjects were
included if at that time, they had completed the Table 1
baseline NP assessment and at least one follow-up Conventional neuropsychological domains and neurocognitive
subdomains often distinguished in schizophrenia
NP assessment at 6 (N = 146) or 18 months (N = 124).
A total of 156 subjects was studied. Mean age at Conventional Neurocognitive subdomains
neuropsychological for schizophrenia
baseline was 36.3 years (S.D. = 9.2), 41% were fe-
domains
male, 65 (41.7%) were white, 62 (39.7%) African
. Attention . Sustained Vigilance
American, 22 (14.1%) Hispanic, 2 (1.3%) Asian and 5 . Short-Term Memory
were ethnically classified as ‘‘other’’ (3.2%). Sixty- Capacity/Span
four cases (41%) of the sample had schizoaffective . Executive Fx . Working Memory
disorder. Age at first symptom presentation averaged . Set Shifting/

18.7 years (S.D. = 7.0) and 16.2 years (S.D. = 9.0) had Cognitive Flexibility
. Ideational Fluency
elapsed since first psychiatric treatment. Education . Memory . Verbal Learning
averaged 12 years (S.D. = 2.4) [49 did not finish high . Non-Verbal Learning
school (HS), 72 had a HS diploma, 17 completed a . Language . Verbal Knowledge
GED, 2 had an Associate’s degree, 15 completed a . Visuospatial . Non-Verbal Reasoning/
bachelor’s degree, and one had completed graduate Problem Solving
. Motor . Motor
school. Education was missing for one case].
108 J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116

and determine the validity and stability of the Table 2


resultant factors. All analytic procedures were con- Neuropsychological tests used in the present study
ducted first in the baseline dataset. The replicability Neuropsychological tests
of valid subdomains was then evaluated using Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R)
follow-up datasets. (Wechsler, 1981)
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) (Wechsler, 1987)
As mentioned above, neuropsychological test bat-
Letter Number Span (Gold et al., 1995)
teries typically yield datasets with a large number of Complex Ideational Material (Goodglass and Kaplan, 1983)a
variables, obtained on a relatively small number of Concentration Endurance Test (D2) (Brickenkamp, 1981)
subjects. Due to the high number of variables, and to Stroop Test (Uttl and Graf, 1997)
the unfavorable case per variable ratio, such datasets Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (128 card manual version)
(Grant and Berg, 1995)
are not tractable by traditional multivariate statistical
Trail Making Test (A and B) (Lezak, 1995)
methods, including factor and principal component Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT)
analyses (PCAs). In particular, in a single principal (Benton and Hamsher, 1978)
component analysis, the number of cases should be at Animal Naming Test (Goodglass and Kaplan, 1983)
least 5 to 10 times more than the number of variables Ruff Figural Fluency Test (Ruff et al., 1987)
Grooved Pegboard Test (Matthews and Love, 1964)
to derive a robust, generalizable solution. To deal with
Finger Tapping Test (Reitan and Davidson, 1974)
this problem, in the present study, a two-stage proce- Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971)b
dure was designed to implement the principal com- a
This measure was not used in subsequent analyses because it
ponent analysis in a stratified way. revealed almost no variance among sample subjects.
In stage 1, the variables were divided into blocks b
This was used for determining preferred hand for motor tests.
based on a priori knowledge about their observed
associations, derived from recent schizophrenia liter-
ature. To establish a clear delineation among adjacent which is comparable to batteries typically used in NP
neuropsychological domains, these preselected blocks studies of schizophrenia.
of variables were intentionally chosen to be broad, Table 3 identifies the NP measures from each of these
permitting the investigation of potential overlap instruments used in the analyses, grouped according to
among hypothesized domains. In stage 2, the varia- the hypothesized subdomains for schizophrenia. Most
bles in each block were subjected to principal com- NP tests generate many variables of potential interest.
ponent analysis. The outcome of this analysis Those measures most widely studied in the schizophre-
(variance explained, component loadings) helped us nia literature were selected for these analyses.
to investigate the factors that explained most of the To validate the subdomain distinctions delineated
variation within such a block of variables and to in Table 3, conceptually related (‘‘adjacent’’) subdo-
identify the underlying neuropsychological domains. mains were combined and submitted to principal
A technique called ‘block principal component component analysis with Promax rotation. By group-
analysis’ (BPCA) has been described recently in the ing potentially related subdomains, this procedure
literature (Liu et al., 2002), which analogous to the offers a conservative method for determining the
two-stage procedure employed in the current study, validity of a priori subdomain delineations. In the
relies on variable stratification. These authors pro- case of ‘‘Motor’’, the concern centered not on poten-
posed the procedure ‘‘for extracting information from tial overlap of adjacent subdomains, but rather on the
a database with a large number of variables and validity of combining measures of motor dexterity and
relatively small number of subjects’’. Using multivar- fine motor speed into a single ‘‘Motor’’ domain, as
iate statistical theory, they demonstrated that BPCA is has been done in several studies (e.g. Bilder et al.,
as efficient as ordinary principal component analysis 2000; Heaton et al., 2001). Four PCAs were thus
for dimensionality reduction. (For additional technical computed consisting of the component variables that
details and numerical examples, please refer to Liu et comprised one or more of the a priori subdomains
al. (2002).) listed in Table 3.
Table 2 provides details about the neuropsycholog- For Attention – Working Memory (ATTNWM),
ical test battery used (administration time: 4 –7 h), variables making up the a priori subdomains of (1)
J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116 109

Table 3 Set Shifting/Cognitive Flexibility and (5) Ideational


NP test measures comprising each hypothesized subdomain for Fluency were combined and submitted to a second
schizophrenia at the first stage of analysis (all scores are raw scores)
PCA for Executive Functions (EXEC). Similarly for
(1) Sustained Vigilance (6) Verbal Learning
General Knowledge and Memory (KNOWMEM),
D2—Fluctuation WMS-R Logical
(worst – best row) Memory I variables making up the subdomains of (8) Verbal
D2—Total letters WMS-R Logical Knowledge, (9) Non-Verbal Reasoning, (6) Verbal
minus errors Memory II and (7) Non-Verbal Learning were combined, and
Stroop—words only WMS-R Verbal measures of motor speed and dexterity (MOTSPDX)
Stroop—color only Paired Associates I
were subjected to a fourth PCA.
Trails A WMS-R Verbal
(2) Short-Term Memory Paired Associates II Subdomains were retained if their internal consis-
Capacity/Span (7) Non-Verbal Learning tency (Cronbach’s alpha) was acceptable and all
WAIS-R Digit Span WMS-R Visual item – total correlations exceeded 0.50. If not, alterna-
Forward raw Reproduction I tive groupings of measures were tested until either it
WMS-R Visual Memory WMS-R Visual
was concluded that a subdomain was not valid or a
Span Forward Reproduction II
WMS-R Figural Memory WMS-R Visual measure could not be reliably included in any of the
WMS-R Logical Paired Associates I factors. The stability of each subdomain (replicability
Memory Immediate WMS-R Visual of the factor structure) was then examined by repeat-
(3) Working Memory Paired Associates II ing each PCA at the 6- and 18-month time point and
Letter – Number Span test (8) Verbal Knowledge
calculating the congruency coefficient (CC; range 0 –
(LNS), # correct WAIS-R Vocabulary
Letter – Number Span test, WAIS-R Information 1) between each time point.
longest span WAIS-R Comprehension The final factors/subdomains were further examined
WAIS-R Arithmetic WAIS-R Similarities for construct and divergent validity by studying the
WAIS-R Digit (9) Non-Verbal Reasoning/ relationship of each with clinical symptoms using the
Span Backward Problem Solving
five-factor solution of the PANSS of Bell et al. (1994).
WAIS-R Digit symbol WAIS-R Block Design
(4) Set Shifting/ WAIS-R Object Assembly
Cognitive Flexibility WAIS-R Pict. Completion
WCST # Perseverative WAIS-R Pict. Arrangement 3. Results
Errors (10) Motor
WCST # Categories Grooved Pegboard
3.1. Attention –Working Memory (ATTNWM)
WCST Failure to
Maintain Set Grooved Pegboard
Stroop—color/word Measures selected a priori for Sustained Vigilance,
interference score Finger Tapping—NH Short-Term Memory Capacity/Span, and Working
(Trails A Trails B)/Trails A Memory subdomains yielded two valid factors: Atten-
Trails B number errors Finger Tapping—PH
tion and Working Memory (Table 4A –4D). WMS-R
Ruff Figural Fluency—Errors
(5) Ideational Fluency Figural Memory did not load on either factor. A
Ruff Figural Fluency— ‘‘Short-Term Memory’’ factor distinct from either
# Unique Designs Attention or Working Memory could not be discerned.
COWAT Phonemic Fluency Standardized alpha coefficients (a) for the constit-
Animal Naming—
uent measures in the Attention and Working Memory
Semantic Fluency
factors were 0.60 and 0.74, respectively, confirming
D2 = Concentration Endurance Test, LNS = Letter Number Span
Test, WAIS-R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, WMS-
adequate construct validity. As shown in Table 4A,
R = Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, WCST = Wisconsin Card the same two factors were revealed with very similar
Sorting Test, COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test. constituent variables at each of three time points.
PH = Preferred hand, NH = Nonpreferred hand. Congruency coefficients (CCs) ranged from 0.86 to
0.96 over the three time points, indicating that the two-
Sustained Vigilance, (2) Short-Term Memory Capac- factor solution is highly replicable and stable over an
ity/Span, and (3) Working Memory were combined 18-month period (Table 5 lists individual CCs for all
and subjected to PCA. Variables included within (4) analyses).
110 J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116

3.2. Executive Functioning (EXEC) Ideational Fluency items and WCST Perseverative
Errors (see Table 4B).
A PCA was computed combining measures grouped Remaining variables loaded in different combina-
a priori into Set Shifting, Cognitive Flexibility and tions of Factors 2 and 3 at each time point (and a
Ideational Fluency subdomains. Factor 1 comprised all fourth factor at the third time point). In contrast,
Table 4
Factor groupings for each of the a priori subdomains at each time point
(A) Attention/Working Memory
A priori subdomains Individual variables Time point each variable loaded on resulting factorsa
entered into PCA
Factor 1 Factor 2
T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3
ATTNWM PCA generated two factors Attention Working Memory
(1) Sustained D2—fluctuation – * * –
Vigilance D2—letters minus errors * * *
Stroop—words only * * *
Stroop—color only * * *
Trails A * * *
(2) Short-Term WAIS-R Digit Span Forward * * *
Memory WMS-R Visual Memory * * *
Capacity/Span Span Forward
[WMS-R—Figural Memory]b – – – – – –
(3) Working LNS, # correct * * *
Memory LNS, longest * * *
WAIS-R Arithmetic * * *
WAIS-R Digit Span Backward * * *
WAIS-R Digit symbol * * *
WMS-R Logical – * – *
Memory I

(B) Executive
A priori subdomains Individual variables Time point each variable loaded on resulting factorsa
entered into PCA
Factor 1
T1 T2 T3
EXEC PCA generated one factor Ideational Fluency + WCST Perseverative Errors
(4) Set Shifting/ WCST # Perseverative Errors * * *
Cognitive [WCST # Categories] – – –
Flexibility [WCST Failure to – – –
Maintain Set]
[Stroop—color/word – – –
interference]
[(Trails A Trail B)/Trail A] – – –
[Trails B # errors] – – –
[Ruff Figural Fluency— – – –
Errors]
(5) Ideational Ruff Figural Fluency— * * *
Fluency Unique Designs
COWAT * * *
Animal Naming * * *
J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116 111

Table 4 (continued)
(C) Knowledge/Memory
A priori subdomains Individual variables Time point each variable loaded on resulting factorsa
entered into PCA
Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3
T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3
KNOWMEM PCA generated three factors Learning Verbal Non-verbal
knowledge functions
(6) Verbal Learning [WMS-R Logical – – * – – – –
Memory I]
[WMS-R Logical – – * – – – –
Memory II]
WMS-R Verbal Paired I * – * – –
WMS-R Verbal Paired II * – * – –
(7) Non-Verbal WMS-R Visual Reproduction I * * *
Learning WMS-R Visual Reproduction II * * *
WMS-R Visual Paired I * – * – –
WMS-R Visual Paired II * – * – –
(8) Verbal WAIS-R Vocabulary * * *
Knowledge WAIS-R Information * * *
WAIS-R Comprehension * * *
WAIS-R Similarities * * *
(9) Non-Verbal WAIS-R Block Design * * *
Reasoning/ WAIS-R Object Assembly * * *
Problem WAIS-R Pict. Completion * * *
Solving WAIS-R Pict. Arrangement * * *

(D) Motor
A priori subdomain Individual variables Time point each variable loaded on resulting factorsa
entered into PCA
Factor 1 Factor 2
T1 T2 T3 T1 T2 T3
MOTSPDX PCA generated two factors Motor dexterity Motor speed
(10) Motor Grooved Pegboard Pref * * *
Grooved Pegboard NonP * * *
Finger Tapping Pref * * *
Finger Tapping NonP * * *
‘‘*’’ Indicates a variable loaded on the factor at that time point, ‘‘ – ’’ indicates that variable did not load on any factor at that time point.
D2 = Concentration Endurance Test, LNS = Letter Number Span Test, WAIS-R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, WMS-
R = Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; for Motor
tests, Pref and NonP = Preferred and Nonpreferred hand, respectively.
a
T1 = baseline; T2 = 6-month follow-up; T3 = 18-month follow-up.
b
Variables in italics failed to load on any factor.

Factor 1 was highly replicable, both in terms of the 1, indicating inadequate construct validity. Item –total
constituent variables at each time point and as mea- correlations (correlation of each variable to total factor
sured by the CCs (0.95, 0.96 and 0.96 for time point score computed with that item dropped) revealed a
pairs 1 – 2, 1 – 3 and 2 – 3, respectively) (Table 5). value of 0.06 for WCST Perseverative Errors, while
However, standardized a equaled only 0.16 for Factor ranging from 0.34 to 0.42 for the three ideational
112 J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116

Table 5 3.4. Motor speed and dexterity (MOTSPDX)


Coefficient of congruence for each factor at each pair of time points
Neurocognitive Standardized Congruency coefficients at Motor speed and dexterity variables comprised the
factor alpha each pair of time points studied
final set. Two independent factors emerged from four
1–2 1–3 2–3 variables, and a virtually identical solution was ob-
Attention 0.60 0.93 0.96 0.96 served at each time point (Table 4D). CCs equaled
Working 0.74 0.90 0.92 0.86 0.99 for each pair of time points for each factor.
Memory
Standardized alphas could not be meaningfully
Learning 0.80 0.69 0.89 0.66
Verbal 0.88 0.95 0.95 0.96 computed because each factor contained only two
Knowledge variables. However, alpha was only 0.30 for the
Non-Verbal 0.82 0.93 0.96 0.88 original ‘‘Motor’’ factor, further supporting the con-
Functions clusion that a single ‘‘motor’’ factor could not be
Ideational 0.16a 0.95 0.96 0.96
reliably derived, and that motor dexterity and speed
Fluency +
WCST should be distinguished.
Perseverative
Errorsa
a
Standardized alpha was 0.69 after WCST Perseverative errors 4. Summary of new variable set
variable was removed. Thus, the retained factor excludes this
variable. Table 5 summarizes the six retained NP factors and
for each, the CC for each pair of time points and the
fluency variables. On this basis, WCST was eliminat- corresponding alpha. Note that the last factor listed
ed, resulting in a = 0.69 for the remaining variables. required removal of one variable to be retained as a
Thus, the subdomain Ideational Fluency was adopted reliable factor. Several NP measures would not be
with WCST Perseverative Errors excluded. realiaby combined with any of these factors or with
one another. Five of these have been widely studied in
3.3. General Knowledge and Memory (KNOWMEM) schizophrenia and should, in our view, be studied
separately. These include: WCST Perseverative
Verbal Knowledge, Non-Verbal Reasoning, Verbal Errors, Stroop Interference, Trails B Trails A/Trails
and Non-Verbal Learning variables were next submit- A, Grooved Pegboard Preferred + NonPreferred
ted to PCA, yielding three factors: Learning (a = 0.80), Hands, Finger Tapping Preferred + NonPreferred
Verbal Knowledge (a = 0.88) and Non-Verbal Reason- Hands.
ing/Problem Solving (a = 0.82). The latter two consist
of WAIS-R subtests and correspond closely with 4.1. Validation of NP factor set: relationship with
WAIS-R Verbal and Performance IQ, respectively. As PANSS ratings and functional disability
shown in Table 4C, the same three factors were
revealed with largely overlapping constituent variables The ongoing study from which the present dataset
at each time point. The exceptions were Visual Repro- is derived investigates relationships between NP def-
duction I and II which loaded on the Learning factor at icits, psychopathology and functional outcome for 3
time point 3, and Logical Memory I and II which only years following an acute exacerbation of illness.
loaded on one of the three retained factors at the third Preliminary analyses are reported to further examine
time point. Thus, the convention of distinguishing the construct, divergent and predictive validity of the
between ‘‘Verbal’’ and ‘‘Non-Verbal’’ secondary and NP factors.
immediate memory was not supported in this analysis. Consistent with the literature (e.g. (Addington et
All but two CCs exceeded 0.88 (Table 5). CCs for al., 1991; Berman et al., 1997; Harvey et al., 1996;
the Learning factor were somewhat lower for time O’Leary et al., 2000; Voruganti et al., 1997), relating
points 1– 2 (0.69) and for time points 2– 3 (0.66). negative symptoms to NP test performance, three of
However, overall, results indicate that these two the six NP factors were significantly correlated with
factors are replicable and stable. negative symptoms while none was correlated signif-
J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116 113

icantly with positive symptoms [employing the five- factors are distinguishable from one another and can
factor PANSS solution of Bell et al. (1994) and a be fruitfully employed to study relationships between
significance threshold of P < 0.001 to correct for individual cognitive subdomains and a variety of
multiplicity (30 tests)] (Table 6). All six NP factors outcome variables (e.g. disability, medication treat-
were correlated with the ‘‘cognitive’’ factor of the ment response, etc.).
PANSS, which is made up of items sensitive to many
of the deficits assessed by NP tests (e.g. goal-directed
sequencing of thought, alertness to external stimuli, 5. Discussion
interpretation of proverbs and analogies, disorders of
thought involving fluidity, spontaneity, flexibility and An obvious alternative data reduction method
planning and motor abnormalities including unnatural would be to conduct a single PCA on the entire
or awkward movements as well as tremors). dataset. The problem with this approach (and the
The predictive validity of the final NP variable set reason it has not been widely adopted) is because
was examined using multiple regression analyses with the first factor, corresponding roughly to ‘‘general
baseline NP measures predicting the last available cognitive functioning’’ or ‘‘g’’, typically explains the
MSIF global rating (the principle measure of func- great majority of the variance. Such an approach does
tional disability) covarying for negative and positive not therefore permit the examination of group differ-
symptoms. While positive symptoms were not related ences or treatment response with respect to distinct
to functional outcome, negative symptoms displayed a cognitive operations. The approach of grouping var-
significant association with the MSIF in all analyses iables into indices corresponding to putatively dis-
( P < 0.001) (i.e. higher negative symptom severity cernable cognitive subdomains seems intuitively
was associated with poorer functional outcome). After optimal. The challenge to researchers is to discern
negative symptoms were accounted for, significant the most valid and replicable subdomains.
associations remained for Attention ( F = 5.73, df = Several important findings emerged from the anal-
1,142, P < 0.01), Working Memory ( F = 4.18, df = yses reported here. Our analysis of EXEC variables
1,142, P < 0.04), and Ideational Fluency ( F = 3.92, did not reveal any reliable factor containing either
df = 1,142, P < 0.05) factors. These analyses revealed Trailmaking or Stroop, both widely studied in schizo-
a pattern of relationships that is generally consistent phrenia. One possible explanation could be that these
with the literature (e.g. Voruganti et al., 1997). The measures were grouped with the wrong a priori
fact that only some NP factors were predictive and subdomain. While these measures seem to share the
that the pattern of significant findings is consistent requirement of response inhibition and set switching
with the literature thus far, suggests that the derived (as does the WCST), they may require intact Working

Table 6
Correlations (Pearson r) between derived NP factor set and factor score ratings from the PANSSa
Neurocognitive factor N Positive Negative ‘‘Cognitive’’ Emotional Hostility
symptoms symptoms discomfort
(1) Attention 142 0.14 0.14 0.30*** 0.07 0.20*
(2) Working Memory 143 0.18 0.23** 0.38**** 0.12 0.22**
(3) Ideational Fluency 142 0.10 0.23* 0.39**** 0.02 0.21*
(4) Learning 141 0.08 0.21** 0.33**** 0.12 0.05
(5) Verbal Knowledge 143 0.11 0.26** 0.34**** 0.13 0.07
(6) Non-Verbal Functions 142 0.05 0.15 0.30**** 0.03 0.14
Two-tailed P values, indicating nominal level of significance.
a
Factors scores from five-factor solution of Bell et al. (1994).
* P < 0.05.
** P < 0.01.
*** P < 0.001.
**** P < 0.0001.
114 J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116

Memory. However, an additional PCA conducted at Our investigation has several important limitations.
each time point, including the original EXEC and new Principal component analysis unavoidably involves a
Working Memory factor variables, failed to yield any series of judgments on which experts will differ.
new replicable factors. In fact, each time point yielded While our technique was designed to help us ‘follow
an entirely different factor solution. For none of the the data’, choices were made with respect to selection
three time points did Trails or Stroop Interference load of NP tests or individual measures, as well as with
on the same factor. We therefore conclude that, as respect to initial groupings of measures. While the
with WCST Perseverative Errors, these measures availability in our dataset of three time points offers
must be treated as independent subdomains. some further validation of judgments that were made,
The widely held view that ‘executive deficits’ different choices might nevertheless have yielded
reflect impairment in a unitary cognitive operation different outcomes. Further, even employing the same
warrants critical review. Defined as impairment in decisions, our findings may not generalize to a sample
planning, organizing, sequencing and modulating studied either earlier or later in the course of illness, or
behaviors, and incorporating such conceptually dis- to a different diagnostic mix. The solutions offered
sociable phenomena such as behavioral inertia, idea- here should therefore be examined in other datasets.
tional fluency, and working memory, it is surprising Finally, in spite of the relatively large sample size,
that the concept of a unitary operation subsuming all given the number of potential NP variables, we were
these functions is still favored in the absence of still constrained by the case per variable ratio. Our
supporting empirical data. motivation in constructing the a priori factors was to
With respect to our findings regarding verbal create a representative set of items within the limits
versus visuospatial memory, most reports in the liter- permitted by our case per variable ratio.
ature that averaged standard scores within a priori In conclusion, these findings may offer useful
domains discriminated separate subdomains to reflect guidance for future investigations of NP performance
this putative ‘modality specificity’ of memory perfor- in schizophrenia. Further, they raise a number of
mance. (Bilder et al., 2000; Saykin et al., 1991, 1994; important questions about findings to date. A sub-
Blanchard and Neale, 1994; Cuesta and Peralta, 1995; stantial proportion of the large-scale NP studies in
Hoff et al., 1999; Censits et al., 1997). Our findings schizophrenia employ the method of grouping varia-
failed to validate this distinction in schizophrenia, and bles into domains which, once constructed, are trea-
suggested instead that for this population, variation in ted as valid and reliable measures of discrete
performance on visuospatial memory tasks is more cognitive operations. Our findings did not validate
closely associated with other visuospatial problem- discrete ‘‘executive functioning’’ or ‘‘motor’’
solving tasks than other memory tasks. Another dis- domains. Also, several subdomain distinctions often
tinction applied in the NP literature (Heaton et al., used (e.g. immediate memory/learning versus delayed
2001; Bilder et al., 2000) not shown to be valid in our recall, visuospatial versus verbal memory) were not
analyses is that between immediate and delayed recall. validated.
Our findings are consistent with Bilder et al. (2002) Harvey and Keefe (2001) offer guidelines for
who employed PCAs for NP data reduction and found studying the cognitive enhancement properties of
that WMS-R Visual Reproduction Immediate and new medications, and recommend selecting cognitive
Delayed grouped together and in the same factor as domains with demonstrated correlations to functional
WAIS-R Block Design rather than into the memory outcome as potential treatment targets. However,
factor, which contained only Logical Memory and validation of such target domains is essential—for
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Immediate and - example, conclusions from the Green et al. (2000)
Delayed. meta-analysis assume the existence of valid and
Finally, our findings do not support the validity of distinct subdomains for ‘‘Secondary Verbal Memory’’
combining motor speed and dexterity measures into a and ‘‘Immediate Verbal Memory’’, not distinguished
single motor factor (Bilder et al., 2000; Saykin et al., in the present analysis. The guidelines also address
1991,1994; Blanchard and Neale, 1994; Censits et al., prior exposure effects resulting from repeated NP
1997; Heaton et al., 2001). testing. We would add to this caution the need to
J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116 115

validate the statistical stability of domain groupings if ture of schizophrenia: generalized or differential deficit? Amer-
calculated at more than one time point. ican Journal of Psychiatry 151, 40 – 48.
Brickenkamp, R., 1981. Concentration – Endurance Test Manual.
Future studies examining NP performance in Verlag fur Psychologie, Gottingen.
schizophrenia will need to consider the validity of Censits, D.M., Ragland, J.D., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., 1997. Neuro-
their data reduction methods in light of the findings psychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model
reported here. of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Research
24, 289 – 298.
Cuesta, M.J., Peralta, V., 1995. Cognitive disorders in the positive,
negative, and disorganization syndromes of schizophrenia. Psy-
Acknowledgements chiatry Research 58, 227 – 235.
Flor-Henry, P., 1969. Psychosis and temporal lobe epilepsy. A con-
The authors thank the patients who volunteered trolled investigation. Epilepsia 10, 363 – 395.
their time for this study, as well as Cristina Gomes, Flor-Henry, P., 1976. Lateralized temporal – limbic dysfunction and
psychopathology. Annals of the New York Academy of Scien-
Anne-Marie Donovan-Lepore, Sherif Abdelmessih, ces 280, 777 – 797.
Stephen Panopolous, Susan Farella, and Drs. Rose- Flor-Henry, P., Yeudall, L.T., 1979. Neuropsychological investiga-
marie Basile-Szulc and Rashmi Rastogi for their tion of schizophrenia and manic-depressive psychoses. In: Gru-
contributions in assessing the patients. We are also zelier, J., Flor-Henry, P. (Eds.), Hemisphere Asymmetries of
grateful to two anonymous reviewers for very helpful Function in Psychopathology. Elsevier/North-Holland Biomed-
ical Press, Amsterdam, pp. 341 – 362.
comments on an earlier draft of this paper. The project Gold, J., Carpenter, C., Randolph, C., Goldberg, T., Weinberger, D.,
was supported by an NIMH Grant (R01 MH 55585) 1995. Auditory Working Memory and the Wisconsin Card Sort-
entitled ‘‘Neuropsychology of Psychiatric Disability ing Test. Schizophrenia Research 15, 117 – 118.
and Service Needs’’ (J. Jaeger, Principal Investigator). Gold, J.M., Carpenter, C., Randolph, C., Goldberg, T.E., Weinberg-
er, D.R., 1997. Auditory Working Memory and Wisconsin Card
Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Archives of General
Psychiatry 54, 159 – 165.
References Gold, J.M., Queern, C., Iannone, V.N., Buchanan, R.W., 1999.
Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological
Addington, J., Addington, D., Maticka-Tyndale, E., 1991. Cogni- status as a screening test in schizophrenia: I. Sensitivity, reli-
tive functioning and positive and negative symptoms in schizo- ability, and validity. American Journal of Psychiatry 156,
phrenia. Schizophrenia Research 5, 123 – 134. 1944 – 1950.
Bell, M.D., Lysaker, P.H., Beam-Goulet, J.L., Milstein, R.M., Lin- Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., 1983. The Assessment of Aphasia and
denmayer, J.P., 1994. Five-component model of schizophrenia: Related Disorders. Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia.
assessing the factorial invariance of the positive and negative Grant, D., Berg, E., 1995. A behavioral analysis of degree of re-
syndrome scale. Psychiatry Research 52, 295 – 303. inforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-
Benton, A., Hamsher, K., 1978. Multiphasic Aphasia Examination type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology
Manual. University Of Iowa, Iowa City. 38, 404 – 411.
Berman, I., Viegner, B., Merson, A., Allan, E., Pappas, D., Green, Green, M.F., 1996. What are the functional consequences of neuro-
A.I., 1997. Differential relationships between positive and neg- cognitive deficits in schizophrenia? American Journal of Psy-
ative symptoms and neuropsychological deficits in schizophre- chiatry 153, 321 – 330.
nia. Schizophrenia Research 25, 1 – 10. Green, M.F., Kern, R.S., Braff, D.L., Mintz, J., 2000. Neurocognitive
Bilder, R.M., Goldman, R.S., Robinson, D., Reiter, G., Bell, L., deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measur-
Bates, J.A., Pappadopulos, E., Willson, D.F., Alvir, J.M., ing the ‘‘right stuff’’? Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 119 – 136.
Woerner, M.G., Geisler, S., Kane, J.M., Lieberman, J.A., Harvey, P.D., Keefe, R.S., 2001. Studies of cognitive change in
2000. Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial patients with schizophrenia following novel antipsychotic treat-
characterization and clinical correlates. American Journal of ment. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 176 – 184.
Psychiatry 157, 549 – 559. Harvey, P.D., Lombardi, J., Leibman, M., White, L., Parrella, M.,
Bilder, R.M., Goldman, R.S., Volavka, J., Czobor, P., Hoptman, M., Powchik, P., Davidson, M., 1996. Cognitive impairment and
Sheitman, B., Lindenmayer, J.P., Citrome, L., McEvoy, J., negative symptoms in geriatric chronic schizophrenic patients:
Kunz, M., Chakos, M., Cooper, T.B., Horowitz, T.L., Lieber- a follow-up study. Schizophrenia Research 22, 223 – 231.
man, J.A., 2002. Neurocognitive effects of clozapine, olanza- Heaton, R.K., Baade, L.E., Johnson, K.L., 1978. Neuropsycholog-
pine, risperidone, and haloperidol in patients with chronic ical test results associated with psychiatric disorders in adults.
schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. American Journal of Psychological Bulletin 85 (1), 141 – 162.
Psychiatry 159, 1018 – 1028. Heaton, R.K., Gladsjo, J.A., Palmer, B.W., Kuck, J., Marcotte, T.D.,
Blanchard, J.J., Neale, J.M., 1994. The neuropsychological signa- Jeste, D.V., 2001. Stability and course of neuropsychological
116 J. Jaeger et al. / Schizophrenia Research 65 (2003) 105–116

deficits in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, in chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clin-
24 – 32. ical Neuroscience 4, 428 – 434.
Hoff, A.L., Sakuma, M., Wieneke, M., Horon, R., Kushner, M., Reitan, R., Davidson, L., 1974. Clinical Neuropsychology: Current
DeLisi, L.E., 1999. Longitudinal neuropsychological follow-up Status and Applications. Hemisphere, New York.
study of patients with first-episode schizophrenia. American Ruff, R.M., Light, R.H., Evans, R.W., 1987. The Ruff Figural
Journal of Psychiatry 156, 1336 – 1341. Fluency Test: a normative study with adults. Developmental
Hoff, A.L., Kremen, W.S., Wieneke, M.H., Lauriello, J., Blankfeld, Neuropsychology 3, 37 – 51.
H.M., Faustman, W.O., Csernansky, J.G., Nordahl, T.E., 2001. Saykin, A.J., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., Mozley, D., Mozley, L.,
Association of estrogen levels with neuropsychological per- Resnick, S.M., Kester, B., Stafiniak, P., 1991. Neuropsycho-
formance in women with schizophrenia. American Journal of logical function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in
Psychiatry 158, 1134 – 1139. memory and learning. Archives of General Psychiatry 48,
Jaeger, J., Berns, S.M., Czobor, P., 2003. The multidimensional 618 – 624.
scale for independent functioning: a new instrument for measur- Saykin, A.J., Shtasel, D.L., Gur, R.E., Kester, D.B., Mozley, L.H.,
ing functional disability in psychiatric populations. Schizophre- Stafiniak, P., Gur, R.C., 1994. Neuropsychological deficits in
nia Bulletin (in press). neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Ar-
Kay, S.R., Fiszbein, S., Opler, L.A., 1987. The positive and neg- chives of General Psychiatry 51, 124 – 131.
ative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Uttl, B., Graf, P., 1997. Color – Word Stroop Test performance
Bulletin 13, 261 – 276. across the adult life span. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Lezak, M., 1995. Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford Univ. Neuropsychology 19 (3), 405 – 420.
Press, New York. Voruganti, L.N., Heslegrave, R.J., Awad, A.G., 1997. Neurocogni-
Liu, A., Zhang, Y., Gehan, E., Clarke, R., 2002. Block principal tive correlates of positive and negative syndromes in schizo-
component analysis with application to gene microarray data phrenia. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 42, 1066 – 1071.
classification. Statistics in Medicine 21, 3465 – 3474. Wechsler, D., 1981. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised
Matthews, C.G., Love, H., 1964. Instructions Manual for the Adult Manual. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio.
Neuropsychology Test Battery. University of Wisconsin Medi- Wechsler, D., 1987. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psycholog-
cal School, Madison, WI. ical Corporation, New York.
Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: Weinberger, D.R., Berman, K.F., Zec, R.F., 1986. Physiologic dys-
the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97 – 113. function of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: I.
O’Leary, D.S., Flaum, M., Kesler, M.L., Flashman, L.A., Arndt, S., Regional cerebral blood flow evidence [see comments]. Ar-
Andreasen, N.C., 2000. Cognitive correlates of the negative, chives of General Psychiatry 43, 114 – 124.
disorganized, and psychotic symptom dimensions of schizo- White, L., Harvey, P.D., Opler, L., Lindenmayer, J.P., 1997. Empiri-
phrenia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences cal assessment of the factorial structure of clinical symptoms in
12, 4 – 15. schizophrenia. A multisite, multimodel evaluation of the factorial
Perlick, D., Mattis, S., Stastny, P., Teresi, J., 1992. Neuropsycho- structure of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. The
logical discriminators of long-term inpatient or outpatient status PANSS Study Group. Psychopathology 30, 263 – 274.

You might also like