You are on page 1of 7

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.

ISSN 0077-8923

A N N A L S O F T H E N E W Y O R K A C A D E M Y O F SC I E N C E S
Issue: The Neurosciences and Music V

Efficacy of musical interventions in dementia:


methodological requirements of nonpharmacological trials
Séverine Samson,1,2 Sylvain Clément,1 Pauline Narme,3 Loris Schiaratura,4
and Nathalie Ehrlé1,5
1
Equipe Neuropsychologie: Audition, Cognition et Action (EA 4072), UFR de psychologie, Université Lille-Nord de France,
Villeneuve d’Ascq, France. 2 Unité d’épilepsie, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France. 3 Equipe Neuropsychologie du
Vieillissement (EA 4468), Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France. 4 Laboratoire de Psychologie:
Interactions Temps Emotions Cognition (EA 4072), UFR de psychologie, Université Lille-Nord de France, Villeneuve d’Ascq,
France. 5 Service de Neurologie, CHU de Reims, Hôpital Maison-Blanche, CMRR Champagne-Ardenne, France

Address for correspondence: Séverine Samson, Department of Psychology, University of Lille, BP 60 149, 59653 Villeneuve
d’Ascq Cedex, France. severine.samson@univ-lille3.fr

The management of patients with Alzheimer’s disease is a significant public health problem given the limited effective-
ness of pharmacological therapies combined with iatrogenic effects of drug treatments in dementia. Consequently,
the development of nondrug care, such as musical interventions, has become a necessity. The experimental rigor of
studies in this area, however, is often lacking. It is therefore difficult to determine the impact of musical interventions
on patients with dementia. As part of a series of studies, we carried out randomized controlled trials to compare the
effectiveness of musical activities to other pleasant activities on various functions in patients with severe Alzheimer’s
disease. The data obtained in these trials are discussed in light of the methodological constraints and requirements
specific to these clinical studies. Although the results demonstrate the power of music on the emotional and behav-
ioral status of patients, they also suggest that other pleasant activities (e.g., cooking) are also effective, leaving open
the question about the specific benefits of music in patients with dementia. All these findings highlight the promising
potential for nonpharmacological treatments to improve the well-being of patients living in residential care and to
reduce caregiver burden.

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease; music; nonpharmacological treatment; randomized controlled trial; emotion

Music-based therapeutic strategies in development of nonpharmacological complemen-


persons with dementia tary interventions as first-line treatment.3,4 As a re-
sult, music activities are seeing a growing success
The management of memory and behavioral disor- with PWD as well as with caregivers.
ders in patients with dementia (PWD) is currently PWD are reactive to music until very advanced
unsatisfactory. Behavioral disorders are particularly stages of the disease5–8 and can learn and recognize
difficult to control and represent a main cause of musical tunes even in the severe stage. In contrast
institutionalization1 and caregiver distress.2 In re- with impaired verbal memory,6,9 music memory
cent years, significant advances have been made seems to be partially preserved in PWD.5 In
in terms of understanding the pathophysiologi- addition, music easily elicits movements stimu-
cal mechanisms of neurodegenerative diseases, but lating interactions between perception and action
current pharmacological treatments are essentially systems, as already underlined by Fraisse, Oléron,
symptomatic and do not have a satisfactory im- and Paillard.10 These effects may be underpinned by
pact on symptoms related to dementia progression. diminished control of inhibition in patients with se-
The limited effectiveness of these treatments and se- vere Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Indeed, it is common
vere iatrogenesis associated with neuroleptics have to observe patients who spontaneously synchro-
led several health institutions to recommend the nize their movements with music, and this can
doi: 10.1111/nyas.12621
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (2015) 249–255 
C 2014 New York Academy of Sciences. 249
Musical interventions in dementia Samson et al.

subsequently facilitate social cohesion in a group associated with biased estimates of treatment effects
setting. Finally, by inducing strong emotions,11,12 and with systematic errors. In this paper, we dis-
music can also help patients to relax by modulating cuss this issue by reporting two successive studies
psychological and physiological functions, espe- to illustrate how methodological weaknesses might
cially those related to stress.13 As a consequence, bias results by amplifying the effect of a music inter-
the power of music and its nonverbal nature offer a vention and to warn clinicians and scientists about
privileged communication medium when language the hasty conclusions that can result from a lack of
is diminished or abolished. Although the mech- experimental rigor.
anisms underlying all these effects remain poorly
understood, the intriguing sensitivity of PWD to Insight from RCTs in patients with
music justifies its use for therapeutic purposes. advanced AD
Assessing the efficacy of music-based In two previous studies,17,19 we performed RCTs to
interventions in PWD examine the short- and long-term effects of a mu-
In the literature, numerous studies have claimed sic intervention (group-based musical activities) on
that music-based interventions have positive effects emotional state and behavioral functioning of PWD.
on the well-being of PWD, but there are few The main objective was to assess the efficacy of non-
controlled studies that provide strong evidence pharmacological treatments on the well-being of
supporting this statement. Even if the development PWD. We were also interested in testing the possible
of better-controlled studies in recent years is superiority of music over nonmusic interventions.
incontestable, deficiencies in reports of such clinical As a nonmusical activity, we selected cooking as it
trials are still frequent, as outlined in several provides an ideal control intervention for music.
reviews.4,14–16 Limitations of these studies can be Both activities are easily performed in a group set-
due to various biases (i.e., unspecified selection cri- ting, are multisensory, pleasurable, and can trigger
teria, small sample size, lack of randomization and old memories. The similarities and differences be-
of a blinded assessor, group dissimilarity at baseline, tween these two studies are summarized in Table 1.
no test-retest, no control group). The absence of a
nonmusic or another intervention group to control Study 1
for changes due to patients’ stimulation (and to In the first study,17 14 PWD were recruited within a
the social impact of group activities) also limits single center. All the participants met the diagnostic
the conclusion about the specific impact of a music criteria for dementia of Alzheimer’s type or mixed
intervention. Moreover, there is virtually no ex- dementia according to the DSM-IV.21 Only patients
perimental research about the potential long-term with moderate to severe stages of dementia were
effects of music-based activities in PWD and their included. They were all native French speakers to
caregivers (except for three recent studies17–19 ). ensure familiarity with the songs selected for music
Thus, there is an urgent need to go beyond intuitions sessions. Patients were randomly assigned to each
and investigate the efficiency of music-based inter- intervention group, and a person not directly
vention in the treatment of PWD more rigorously involved in data collection performed the random-
with adequate experimental methodology. ization. Patients engaged in either music or cooking
Well-designed and properly executed random- activities twice a week (2 h each session) for 1
ized controlled trials (RCTs) provide the best month, with the total duration of the intervention
evidence for the efficacy of nonpharmacological being 16 hours. Each intervention group included
treatments. As emphasized by a group of scien- seven patients supervised by a therapist with the
tists and editors who developed the CONSORT help of another person. During each intervention,
(Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) state- receptive (listening to music or tasting recipes) and
ment to improve the quality of RCT reporting,20 productive (clapping hands or singing with music
“trials with inadequate or poor methodological or preparing a recipe) phases were alternated. Two
approaches are associated with exaggerated treat- different therapists were involved in this RCT, each
ment effects” (p. 663). In other words, findings con- supervising the music or cooking intervention.
taminated by numerous uncontrolled factors are The one who supervised music intervention had

250 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (2015) 249–255 


C 2014 New York Academy of Sciences.
Samson et al. Musical interventions in dementia

Table 1. List of items to control in RCTs investigating the efficiency of nonpharmacological treatments on PWD

Study 1: Clément et al. Study 2: Narme et al.


Items to control in RCTs (2012)17 (2014)19

Clinical population PWD PWD No change


Sample size N = 14 N = 48 Different
Attrition bias 21% 23% No change
Single- or multiple-center Single-center Single-center No change
Eligibility criteria (cf. text) Controlled Controlled No change
Study design (parallel or crossover) Parallel group Parallel group No change
Intervention group (music) Yes Yes No change
Control intervention group (cooking) Yes Yes No change
Control group (usual care) No No No change
Random group assignment Yes Yes No change
Test-retest (no differences in baseline Yes Yes No change
data)
Time period of intervention 4 weeks 4 weeks No change
Intensity (number of time per week) 2 2 No change
Dosage (duration of intervention) 16 hours 8 hours Different
Duration of follow-up (from the 8 weeks 8 weeks No change
beginning of intervention)
Number of therapists 2 1 (for both music and Different
cooking)
(1 for music and
1 for cooking)
Therapists’ personal preference Music (for music Cooking (for both Different
intervention) interventions)
No (for cooking
intervention)
Primary outcome measure Emotional state Emotional state No change
Blind assessment (interviewer) No Yes Different
Blind assessment (raters of video Yes Yes No change
recording)
Secondary outcome measures None Cognitive status Different
Functional status
Behavioral status
Caregivers’ distress
Similarities between interventions
(music vs. cooking)
• Active versus passive activities No Yes Different
• Attractiveness No Yes Different
• Novelty between sessions No No No change

a clear preference for music activities but no prior evaluations before the intervention (BL–1 , BL0 ) to
education in music therapy. obtain baseline measures and to control test–retest
Emotional state was determined by assessing effect, two evaluations to test short-term effects of
discourse content, emotional facial expressions, the intervention after the fourth (IMID ) and the
and mood. These three outcome measures were last sessions (IEND ), and two follow-up measures
performed six times with each patient with two to assess long-term effects after the end of the

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (2015) 249–255 


C 2014 New York Academy of Sciences. 251
Musical interventions in dementia Samson et al.

intervention, 2 (POST+2 ) and 4 (POST+4 ) weeks results. The person who interviewed the patients was
postintervention. the therapist in charge of the music intervention.
Discourse content and emotional facial expres- Because she was not blind to the patient’s group,
sions were assessed using short semistructured the change in emotional state observed in the music
filmed interviews. During this interview a psychol- group might have been due to bias, not only by the
ogist questioned each patient individually about his familiarity of the assessors but also by the implicit
or her feelings at the present time. However, this memory of enjoyable social interactions during mu-
person was not blind to the patient’s intervention sic interventions. In addition, the small number of
group. Emotional facial expressions and discourse participants might have compromised the general-
content were analyzed from the first two min of each ization of the data. Therefore, it is difficult to con-
filmed interview. Two independent and blinded ob- clude if the benefit of music in this study is domain
servers rated audio and video recordings to count specific or if it is related to other confounding factors
the number of positive and negative verbal and such as familiarity, arousal, attractiveness, and/or
facial expressions (according to the Facial Action pleasantness. To overcome these pitfalls, we carried
Coding System22 ). Mood was assessed with an out a second study.
adaptation of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory for
Adults (STAI-A).23 Unlike the standard procedure, Study 2
a caregiver completed the questionnaire. Instead of The general design of this second study19 was very
using the global score, we separately summed the similar to that of the previous one, except for a few
ratings of the 10 positive and 10 negative statements, differences that are outlined below and summarized
consistent with how the facial expressions and dis- in Table 1. In this study we increased the sample size
course content were analyzed. From each measure and recruited 48 PWD in a single center (different
(facial expression, discourse, mood), an emotional from study 1), with a similar attrition rate to our
index was derived from the positive and the nega- previous study. We used the same three emotional
tive scores. A positive emotional index meant that indices to examine patients’ emotional state, but we
the patient displayed more positive than negative also measured cognitive, functional, and behavioral
emotions, and a negative emotional index meant functioning as well as caregiver distress. Owing to
the reverse. clinical constraints from the nursing home, we had
The change of emotional state was the primary to reduce the duration of each intervention session
outcome of interest. This change was measured by to 1 h, decreasing the total duration of interven-
the difference in the emotional index for facial ex- tions from 16 to 8 hours. To control the potential
pressions, discourse content, and mood between bias effect of specific therapist, a single person
baseline and posttreatment assessments. As depicted supervised both music and cooking interventions.
in Figure 1 (left panel), the mean emotional index We also improved the attractiveness of cooking
improved for discourse content, facial expressions, sessions by adapting the activities to patients and
and mood after the fourth (IMID ) and eighth (IEND ) balanced the proportion of active and passive ac-
music sessions, and this improvement persisted at 2 tivities in both interventions. Finally, an important
(POST+2 ) and 4 (POST+4 ) weeks after the end of the difference concerned the psychologist in charge of
intervention for discourse content and mood. Based the interview. External to the residential care home,
on this profile of results, we showed that music had this person was completely blind to the patient’s
short-term positive effects on all three emotional intervention group, which had not been the case in
indices and long-term effects on two indices (dis- the previous study. Moreover, the psychologist who
course valence and mood). Conversely, there was assessed cognitive abilities and functional behaviors
no significant benefit of cooking interventions on and the caregivers who completed questionnaires
emotional state, with the exception of a short-term were all blinded as well. Therefore, all the assessors
effect on mood. The findings of this study suggest (interviewer, psychologist, caregivers, and raters)
that music was more effective than cooking in im- were blinded from the patient’s group affiliation.
proving the emotional state of PWD. The results of this “better-controlled” study,
As previously discussed,19,24 several methodolog- displayed in Figure 1 (right panel), showed that
ical weaknesses might have biased the reported both music and cooking interventions resulted in

252 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (2015) 249–255 


C 2014 New York Academy of Sciences.
Samson et al. Musical interventions in dementia

A B

Figure 1. Mean emotional indices (%) of the two intervention groups (music and cooking) for discourse content, facial expressions,
and mood across the six evaluations (bars represent the standard error of the mean, and lines with asterisks correspond to significant
differences (P < 0.05) between a value and the baseline (BL0 )). (A) Left panel, study 1;17 (B) right panel, study 2.19

improved emotional state as indicated by facial importance of using blind assessors (interviewers
expressions and mood indices. It is noteworthy that and raters) in such studies.
the magnitude of the short- and long-term posi- Although we found that the effect of the music
tive effects of music on emotional indices reported intervention on patients’ emotional state was less
in study 1 was reduced. Moreover, the music in- marked after strict control of different factors, there
tervention in study 1 was significantly better than were significant changes in several other measures
cooking in producing positive emotions in PWD, during and after both types of interventions rela-
illustrating the so-called exaggerated treatment ef- tive to baseline. Specifically, we found that music
fect. As depicted in Figure 1, the beneficial effect and cooking interventions also reduced the severity
of music in study 2 was no different from that of of behavioral disorders and consequently dimin-
cooking. Taken together, these findings suggest that ished professional caregiver distress. These addi-
implicit associations between the pleasant activities tional results seem even more reliable because many
and the therapist (without any episodic memory of methodological constraints have been taken into ac-
music sessions) might have influenced the findings count in this second RCT. Overall, the novelty of
of study 1. This effect seems to result, at least in part, study 2 was in its finding that enjoyable or pleasant
from interactions with a therapist rather than from activities other than music can also improve the
the musical intervention per se, emphasizing the well-being of PWD and their caregivers. To further

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (2015) 249–255 


C 2014 New York Academy of Sciences. 253
Musical interventions in dementia Samson et al.

explore the impact of nonpharmacological ap- They will also be useful to clarify the precise mech-
proaches in dementia, it will be necessary to in- anism by which nonpharmacological approaches,
clude another control group of participants who re- and music intervention in particular, induce positive
ceive the usual care without any intervention. This emotional and behavioral changes in patients
additional constraint will help to ensure that posi- with severe dementia and their family or medical
tive changes did not result from familiarity with the caregivers.
assessor (interviewer) and cannot be attributed to In view of the increasing number of PWD and
group activities in general, a placebo effect, or nat- the limited resources of public health care, enjoyable
ural variations of disease. Such a study is presently group activities provide a viable and promising al-
in progress in our laboratory. ternative to pharmacological treatments to improve
the emotional status of PWD and to reduce caregiver
Conclusions and future prospects
distress. Nonpharmacological interventions such as
On the basis of the previously reported data, we can music or cooking involving one or two caregivers
conclude that nonpharmacological interventions, with a group of six to eight patients are feasible
such as music and cooking offered twice weekly activities, especially for patients in residential care,
over 1 month to PWD with severe cognitive decline, with a reasonable cost/benefit ratio.25
can improve emotional and behavioral functioning
Acknowledgments
in patients and reduce caregiver distress. We found
that music activities did not have greater therapeutic The authors are grateful to Amee Baird for her
effects than other pleasant activities. This absence of helpful comments on previous versions of the
difference between the benefits of music and cook- manuscript, to the psychologists (A. Tonini, L.
ing argues against music specificity in improving Sintes, A. Saenz, and S. Desdouits) who carried out
well-being in PWD. the evaluations and the interventions with the as-
As highlighted in this report, trials assessing sistance of L. Foulon and R. Goret. The authors are
behavioral manipulations require additional con- also grateful to the caregivers of the Valenciennes
straints that are not always so crucial or rele- Hospital and Wilson Nursing Home who gave their
vant in pharmacological therapy studies. The major time ensuring the study feasibility. These studies
difficulty in such nonpharmacological trials is to have received funding from the French Ministry
preserve blinding at every stage of behavioral eval- (ANR-09-BLAN-0310-02), the private Fondation
uation. Even if the therapist cannot be blinded, the Plan Alzheimer, and the Institut Universitaire de
healthcare professionals in charge of the participants France to S.S.
and the people assessing the patients (outcome as-
sessors) should be blinded. Contact among care- Conflicts of interest
givers, assessors, and therapists should be avoided, The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
thus limiting the risk for performance bias. In mul-
ticenter studies, other constraints requiring spe-
cific analysis are necessary to control care provider
and center. To help clinicians and scientists to im- References
prove the quality of such studies, we proposed a 1. Luppa, M., T. Luck, E. Brähler, et al. 2008. Prediction of in-
list of items to take into account in designing non- stitutionalisation in dementia. A systematic review. Dement.
phamacological RCTs (see Table 1). Such method- Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 26: 65–78.
ological rigor is in agreement with the CONSORT 2. Black, W. & O.P. Almeida. 2004. A systematic review of the
association between the behavioral and psychological symp-
statement20 and is necessary to document the pos- toms of dementia and burden of care. Int. Psychogeriatr. 16:
sible impact of nonpharmacological therapy on be- 295–315.
havioral responses in RCTs. This will be essential 3. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. 2006.
to propose guidelines for the formulation of pub- NICE clinical guideline 42: dementia, supporting people
lic heath policies. Well-designed prospective trials with dementia and their carers in health and social care.
Available at guidance.nice.org.uk/cg42.
are therefore needed to establish the efficacy of 4. Vink, A.C., M.S. Bruinsma & R.J.P.M. Scolten. 2011. Music
music interventions under clinical circumstances therapy for people with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst.
and to generalize the recently published results.18,19 Rev. 3: CD003477.

254 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (2015) 249–255 


C 2014 New York Academy of Sciences.
Samson et al. Musical interventions in dementia

5. Baird, A. & S. Samson. 2009. Memory for music in dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res.
Alzheimer’s disease: unforgettable? Neuropsychol. Rev. 19: Rev. 12: 628–641.
85–101. 16. McDermott, O., N. Crellin, H.M. Ridder, et al. 2012. Music
6. Samson, S., D. Dellacherie & H. Platel. 2009. Emotional therapy in dementia: a narrative synthesis systematic review.
power of music in patients with memory disorders: clinical Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28: 781–794.
implications of cognitive neuroscience. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 17. Clément, S., A. Tonini, F. Khatir, et al. 2012. Short and longer
1169: 245–255. term effects of musical intervention in severe Alzheimer dis-
7. Takahashi, T. & H. Matsushita. 2006. Long-term effects of ease. Music Percept. 29: 533–541.
music therapy on elderly with moderate/severe dementia. J. 18. Särkämö, T., M. Tervaniemi, S. Laitinen, et al. 2014. Cog-
Music Ther. 43: 317–333. nitive, emotional and social benefits of regular musical
8. El Haj, M., L. Fasotti & P. Allain. 2012. The involun- activities in early dementia: randomized controlled study.
tary nature of music-evoked autobiographical memories in Gerontologist 54: 634–650.
Alzheimer’s disease. Conscious Cogn. 21: 238–246. 19. Narme, P., S. Clément, N. Ehrlé, et al. 2014. Efficacy of musi-
9. Cuddy, L.L. & J. Duffin. 2005. Music, memory, and cal interventions in dementia: evidence from a randomized
Alzheimer’s disease: is music recognition spared in demen- controlled trial. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 38: 359–369.
tia, and how can it be assessed? Med. Hypotheses 64: 229–235. 20. Altman, D.G., K.F. Schulz, D. Moher, et al. 2001. The
10. Fraisse, P., G. Oléron & J. Paillard. 1953. Dynamogenic effects revised CONSORT statement for reporting randomized tri-
of music; experimental study. Annee Psychol. 53: 1–34. als: explanation and elaboration. Ann. Intern. Med. 134:
11. Blood, A.J. & R.J. Zatorre. 2001. Intensely pleasurable re- 663–694.
sponses to music correlate with activity in brain regions im- 21. American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and
plicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. (DSM-IV).
98: 11818–11823. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
12. Salimpoor, V.N., M. Benovoy, K. Larcher, et al. 2011. 22. Ekman, P. & W.V. Friesen. 1978. Facial Action Coding System.
Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.
and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci. 14: 23. Spielberger, C.D. 1983. Manual for the State–Trait Anxiety
257–262. Inventory (STAI). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist
13. Khalfa, S., S.D. Bella, M. Roy, et al. 2003. Effects of relaxing Press.
music on salivary cortisol level after psychological stress. 24. Narme, P., A. Tonini, F. Khatir, et al. 2013. Nonpharmaco-
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 999: 374–376. logical treatment for Alzheimer’s disease: comparison be-
14. Raglio, A., G. Bellelli, P. Mazzola, et al. 2012. Music, mu- tween musical and non-musical interventions. Geriatr. Psy-
sic therapy and dementia: a review of literature and the chol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 10: 215–224.
recommandations of the Italian Psychogeriatric Association. 25. Bellelli, G., A. Raglio & M. Trabucchi. 2012. Music inter-
Maturitas 72: 305–310. ventions against agitated behaviour in elderly persons with
15. Ueda, T., Y. Suzukamo, M. Sato, et al. 2013. Effects of mu- dementia: a cost-effective perspective. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychi-
sic therapy on behavioral and psychological symptoms of atry 27: 327.

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (2015) 249–255 


C 2014 New York Academy of Sciences. 255

You might also like