You are on page 1of 7

Research in Translation

Should Burden of Disease Estimates Include Cannabis


Use as a Risk Factor for Psychosis?
Louisa Degenhardt1*, Wayne D. Hall2, Michael Lynskey3, John McGrath4,5, Jennifer McLaren1, Bianca
Calabria1, Harvey Whiteford2,5, Theo Vos2
1 National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia, 2 School of Population Health, University of Queensland, Herston,
Australia, 3 Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri, United States, 4 Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland,
St. Lucia, Australia, 5 Queensland Centre for Mental Health Research, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Australia

Introduction
Summary
Evidence has accumulated suggesting
that regular cannabis use is associated with
psychotic symptoms and disorders in the
N Comparative risk assessments estimate the proportion of a disease that can be
attributed to a particular risk exposure and are important guides for health
general population [1,2] and elevated planning.
among incident cases of psychosis [3,4].
In this paper, we present the arguments for,
N In observational studies, there has been consistent evidence that cannabis use
is associated with an increased risk of schizophrenia and more generally,
and implications of, considering cannabis psychosis.
use as a risk factor for psychosis in the 2005
Global Burden of Disease (GBD) project.
N There is debate about whether such observational evidence is sufficient to infer
that cannabis use is a contributory cause of psychosis.

Examining Risk Factors for


N Given the controversy, should the comparative risk assessment in the current
revision of the Global Burden of Disease (GBD) include an attribution of
Disease Burden psychosis to cannabis use?
Governments, policymakers, and funders N We argue that the risk assessment should be included because the evidence is
as good as that for many other risk factors included in the GBD, psychotic
need information on the comparative pop-
disorders are associated with substantial unavertable disability, and cannabis
ulation health impact of different diseases use is a potentially preventable exposure.
and risk factors when making decisions
about where to focus policy, services, and
research. This field was revolutionised when any disease because of concerns about psychotic outcomes in individuals who
the World Bank provided estimates using the quality of the evidence [8]. In the used cannabis, with the greatest risk
the disability-adjusted life year (DALY) [5]. intervening years there has been a steady among those who used cannabis most
This measure combined measures of prema- increase in the number and quality of frequently.
ture mortality (years of life lost [YLL]) and research studies that have been conducted It is useful to distinguish two primary
morbidity (years lived with disability [YLD]) exploring the links between cannabis use ways in which cannabis use could be a
in order to estimate GBD. Estimates of and psychosis. Overall, these studies ‘‘cause’’ of psychosis [12]. The strongest
burden attributable to various risk factors— indicate that chance is an unlikely expla- form of causal link is that heavy cannabis
‘‘comparative risk assessment’’ (CRA) exer- nation of their association [9–11]. Recent use causes a psychosis that would not
cises [6]—are particularly important because reviews of prospective general population otherwise have occurred. A second hy-
they quantify and allow comparison of the studies of associations between cannabis pothesis is that cannabis use is a contrib-
extent to which reduction or removal of use and later psychosis (Table 1) [10,11] utory cause: it might precipitate psychosis
exposure to risk factors would reduce disease concluded that although control for con- in vulnerable individuals—that it is one
burden by using a measure of estimated founding reduced the size of the associa- factor among many (including genetic
Population Attributable Risks (PAR). The tion, there was an increased risk of predisposition and other unknown causes)
GBD uses fairly standard criteria to evaluate
‘‘risk factors’’, in line with Bradford Hill’s [7] Citation: Degenhardt L, Hall WD, Lynskey M, McGrath J, McLaren J, et al. (2009) Should Burden of Disease
oft-quoted criteria (Box 1). Estimates Include Cannabis Use as a Risk Factor for Psychosis? PLoS Med 6(9): e1000133. doi:10.1371/
journal.pmed.1000133
Evidence on the Association between Published September 29, 2009
Cannabis Use and Psychosis Copyright: ß 2009 Degenhardt et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
In the previous global CRA, cannabis medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
use was not included as a risk factor for Funding: This work was given funding support from the Australian Government Department of Health and
Ageing. LD is the recipient of an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Senior
Research Fellowship. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish,
Research in Translation discusses health interven- or preparation of the manuscript.
tions in the context of translation from basic to Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
clinical research, or from clinical evidence to
practice. * E-mail: l.degenhardt@unsw.edu.au
Provenance: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org 1 September 2009 | Volume 6 | Issue 9 | e1000133


Box 1. Risk Factor Definitions in the 2005 GBD Project [66] cannabis use might temporarily trigger
some symptoms of psychosis among some
The GBD defines risks according to the following considerations: users. Such symptoms are clinically (and
significantly) distinct from a psychotic
N Risk factors should be potentially modifiable; disorder such as schizophrenia.
N Risks should be assessed irrespective of place in a causal chain or scientific Other drugs such as amphetamine have
discipline that has traditionally analysed the risk factor, as long as evidence of also been shown to have the potential to
causal effect can be established; trigger psychotic symptoms among some
N Risks are defined to be not too broad (e.g., diet or environment as a whole) or users [25]. Double-blind provocation stud-
too narrow (e.g., every single fruit and vegetable or every toxicant in tobacco ies using intravenous THC and related
smoke) with a relatively specific definition of risk factor exposure; compounds in healthy controls are pro-
N Protective as well as hazardous factors are considered. However, the absence of viding insights into the neurobiological
a specific intervention should not be assessed as a risk factor, but rather in correlates of cannabis-related transient
measurement of intervention coverage and effectiveness; and psychotic symptoms and neuro-cognitive
N There exist sufficient data on risk factor exposure and risk-factor disease impairments [26–29].
relationships. Several cross-sectional studies have ex-
amined the relationship between cannabis
use and self-reported psychotic experienc-
that act together to cause psychotic Is the Association Biologically es or psychotic symptoms in the general
disorders. Plausible? population. All have found that cannabis
The evidence suggests that it is more use (or cannabis use disorders) were more
likely that cannabis use precipitates psy- The principal psychoactive ingredient common among people reporting such
chosis in vulnerable persons, which is of cannabis is delta-9-tetrahydrocannabi- experiences; and these associations persist-
consistent with other lines of evidence nol (THC), which acts upon a specific ed after controlling for other variables
suggesting that there is a complex constel- cannabinoid receptor (CB1) in the brain [1,2,12,30]. Although these findings pro-
lation of factors leading to the develop- [19]. Although historically the dopaminer- vide important clues to the mechanisms of
ment of psychosis (the stress-diathesis gic system has been considered to play an action linking cannabis use and persistent
model of schizophrenia) and with studies important role in psychotic disorders [20], psychotic symptoms and/or clinical diag-
suggesting that gene-environment interac- there is increasing evidence that the noses, these outcomes are less of a concern
tions may provide some explanation of the cannabinoid system may also be involved for the research community.
association [13]. It is also consistent with [21]. Some studies have used animal It is not always clear whether the
conflicting evidence to date on whether models to explore the impact of THC psychotic symptoms endorsed in studies
changes in cannabis use have been and related compounds on brain function assessing the relationship between canna-
associated with changes in the incidence [21–24]. These results are also stimulating bis use and ‘‘psychosis’’ occurred only in
of psychotic disorders in the general new preclinical research aimed at describ- the context of cannabis intoxication, or
population [14–16]. ing neurobiological mechanisms of action whether the symptoms were a more distal
There is also some evidence that linking cannabis and outcomes of interest outcome of previous cannabis use. For
cannabis use is associated with increased to schizophrenia [22]. Rodent models are example, the Fergusson et al. study [31]
likelihood of relapse to psychosis among being developed to examine the impact of assessed the relationship between psychot-
those who have developed a psychotic THC exposure on pathways implicated in ic symptoms in the past month with
disorder [17], although the quality of clinical schizophrenia [21]. cannabis use in the past year. It remains
control for confounding in these studies possible that the psychotic symptoms
is poor [17]. In some studies cannabis use What Do We Mean by endorsed may have been experienced only
has also been associated with a younger while intoxicated. The instruments used to
‘‘Psychosis’’?
age of onset of psychosis [18], although measure psychotic outcomes in the Ar-
control for confounding variables in these Transient cannabis-induced psy- senault et al. [32], Henquet et al. [33], and
has also been poor. chotic symptoms. It is possible that van Os et al. [34] studies contain instruc-

Table 1. Summary of two systematic reviews investigating cannabis use as a risk factor for psychosis.

Study Details Adjusted Pooled Estimate (95% CI)

Moore et al. [11] Searched Medline, Embase, CINAHL, PsycINFO, ISI Web of Knowledge, ISI Proceedings, ZETOC, BIOSIS, Ever use: 1.41 (1.20–1.65)
LILACS, and MEDCARIB from their inception to September, 2006; searched reference lists of studies
selected for inclusion; contacted experts. Studies were included if longitudinal and population based.
Seven studies were included (some multiple papers). Data extraction and quality assessment were
done independently and in duplicate.
‘‘Heavy’’ use: 2.09 (1.54–2.84)
Arsenault et al. [12] The research strategies used were: computerized Medline and PsycLIT searches; cross-referencing of 2.34 (1.69–2.95)
original studies; contact with other researchers in the field. Studies that included a well-defined sample
drawn from population-based registers or cohorts and used prospective measures of cannabis use and
adult psychosis.

doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000133.t001

PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org 2 September 2009 | Volume 6 | Issue 9 | e1000133


tions not to include psychotic symptoms related to the risk of developing schizo- up period in individuals who had not
that only occur in the context of intoxica- phrenia (personal history of psychiatric reported psychiatric symptoms at baseline.
tion. Further, the authors of the Swedish disorder and parental divorce). Second, there was a dose-response rela-
conscript study [35] maintain that it is A number of longitudinal studies have tionship between frequency of cannabis
unlikely that substance-induced intoxica- since been reported that have all support- use at baseline and risk of psychotic
tion would have been misdiagnosed as ed the findings of the Andreassen et al. symptoms during the follow-up period.
schizophrenia. We turn now to the study. Zammit et al. reported a follow up Third, the relationship between cannabis
evidence relating to more persistent symp- of the Swedish cohort study, reporting on use and psychotic symptoms persisted
toms or disorders. risk over a 27-y follow up that covers most when they statistically controlled for the
Schizophrenia and other psychotic of the risk period for the onset of psychotic effects of other drug use. Fourth, the
disorders. In case-control studies disorders in a cohort that was first studied relationship between cannabis use and
[36,37], patients with schizophrenia are when 18–20 y old [35]. This study im- psychotic symptoms was stronger for cases
more likely to use cannabis than other proved on the earlier study in a number of with more severe psychotic symptoms that
psychiatric patients or normal controls ways. The psychiatric register provided were adjudged to need psychiatric care.
[38]. The prevalence of use in patients more complete coverage of all cases Fifth, those who reported any psychotic
with schizophrenia has varied between diagnosed with schizophrenia; there was symptoms at baseline were more likely to
studies but it is generally higher than rates better statistical control of a larger number develop schizophrenia if they used canna-
in the general population [38,39]. of potential confounding variables, includ- bis than were individuals who were not so
Cross-sectional community surveys of ing other drug use, IQ, known risk factors vulnerable.
psychiatric disorders have also document- for schizophrenia, and social integration; A study by Henquet et al. [33] replicat-
ed higher rates of substance use disorders the study distinguished between cases that ed the Swedish and Dutch studies in a 4-y
among persons with schizophrenia [40]. occurred in the first 5 y of the study period follow up of a cohort of 2,437 adolescents
Nearly half of the patients identified with and those that occurred more than 5 y and young adults between 1995 and 1999
schizophrenia in the US ECA study had a afterwards in order to look at the possible in Munich. Their participants were assess-
diagnosis of substance abuse or depen- role of a syndrome; and the study ed at baseline on cannabis use and
dence (28% for an illicit drug disorder) undertook separate analyses in those who psychotic symptoms using a questionnaire.
[41,42]. In an Australian population- only reported using cannabis at the initial Psychotic symptoms were assessed in early
based survey, 11.5% of those who report- assessment. adulthood using the Composite Interna-
ed that they had been diagnosed with Zammit et al. [35] also found cannabis tional Diagnostic Interview. They found a
schizophrenia met ICD-10 criteria for a use at baseline predicted an increased risk dose-response relationship between self-
cannabis use disorder in the past 12 mo, of schizophrenia during the follow-up reported cannabis use at baseline and the
and 21.2% met criteria for an alcohol use period. There was a dose-response rela- likelihood of reporting psychotic symp-
disorder. After adjusting for confounding tionship with frequency of use, which toms. As in the Dutch cohort, young
variables, those who met criteria for persisted after statistical control for con- people who reported psychotic symptoms
cannabis dependence were 2.9 times more founders, including a history of psychiatric at baseline were much more likely to
likely to report that they had been symptoms at baseline. The same relation- experience psychotic symptoms at follow
diagnosed with schizophrenia than those ships were observed in the subset of the up if they used cannabis than were peers
who did not [1]. sample who only reported cannabis use at who did not have such a history.
The first evidence that cannabis use baseline and among cases diagnosed in the Arseneault et al. reported a prospective
may precipitate schizophrenia came from first 5 y after assessment and for the study of the relationship between adoles-
a 15-y prospective study of cannabis use subsequent 22 y. cent cannabis use and psychosis in young
and schizophrenia in 50,465 Swedish Zammit et al.’s findings were consistent adults in a New Zealand birth cohort
conscripts [43]. This study investigated with those of a study conducted by Van (n = 759). Participants were assessed inten-
the relationship between self-reported Os and colleagues [34]. This was a 3-y sively on risk factors for psychotic symp-
cannabis use at age 18 y and the risk of longitudinal study of the relationship toms and disorders since birth [32], and
being diagnosed with schizophrenia in the between self-reported cannabis use and psychotic disorders were conservatively
Swedish psychiatric case register during psychosis in a community sample of 4,848 assessed according to DSM-IV diagnostic
the next 15 y. Those who had tried people in the Netherlands. Participants criteria, with corroborative reports from
cannabis by age 18 y were 2.4 times more were assessed at baseline on cannabis and family members or friends on social
likely to receive a diagnosis of schizophre- other drug use. Psychotic symptoms were adjustment. They assessed psychotic
nia than those who had not. The risk of a assessed using a computerised diagnostic symptoms at age 11 y before onset of
diagnosis of schizophrenia was related to interview. A diagnosis of psychosis was cannabis use and distinguished between
cannabis use in a dose-response way to the validated in positive cases by a diagnostic early and late onset of cannabis use. They
number of times cannabis had been used telephone interview with a psychiatrist or also examined the specificity of the
by age 18. Compared to those who had psychologist. A consensus clinical judge- association between cannabis use and
not used cannabis, the risk of developing ment was made on the basis of the psychosis by conducting analyses of the
schizophrenia was 1.3 times higher for interview material as to whether individ- effects of: (1) other drug use on psychotic
those who had used cannabis one to ten uals had a psychotic disorder for which symptoms and disorders; and (2) cannabis
times, three times higher for those who they were in need of psychiatric care. use on depressive disorders.
had used cannabis between one and 50 Van Os et al. replicated and extended Arseneault et al. found a relationship
times, and six times higher for those who the findings of the Swedish cohort in a between cannabis use by age 15 y and an
had used cannabis more than 50 times. number of important ways. First, cannabis increased risk of schizophreniform disor-
These results remained after statistical use at baseline predicted an increased risk der by age 26 y. Controlling for other
adjustment for two variables that were of psychotic symptoms during the follow- drug use did not affect the relationship.

PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org 3 September 2009 | Volume 6 | Issue 9 | e1000133


The relationship was no longer statistically found that cannabis dependence at age the small number of cases, the use of open
significant after adjustment for reporting 18 y predicted an increased risk of ended interviews, and the use of multiple
psychotic symptoms at age 11 y, which psychotic symptoms at age 21 y (relative analyses.
probably reflected the small number of risk [RR] of 2.3). This association was
psychotic disorders observed in the sam- smaller but still significant after adjust- The Effects of Varying Outcome
ple. The small number of cases also limited ment for potential confounders (RR of Measures
the ability of the study to examine 1.8). More recently, Fergusson and col-
predictors of psychotic disorders at age leagues examined the association between There are several major criticisms of the
26 y. The measurement of cannabis and cannabis and psychotic symptoms until above evidence. The first concerns the
other drug use was crude (viz, none, 1–2 age 25 y with the same cohort of young varying outcome measures that different
times, and 3 or more times), although this adults, using a more sophisticated struc- studies have used. These include ‘‘psycho-
was more likely to work against finding tural equations modelling design that sis,’’ psychotic symptoms, and schizo-
relationships. accounted for both observed and non- phreniform disorders diagnosed using
There was also specificity in the effects observed confounding factors [31]. As psychiatric interviews and psychiatric case
of cannabis on schizophreniform disorder: with their earlier study, they concluded registers.
there was no relationship between other that the association between cannabis and How should this affect confidence in the
drug use and psychotic disorders, and no psychosis did not appear to be explained study findings? We suggest that they are
relationship between cannabis use and by confounding factors, and that the less of an issue than they first appear. First,
depression. There was also an interaction direction of the association appeared to as noted above, there is a growing
between psychosis risk and age of onset of be from cannabis use to symptoms of recognition that psychotic-like experiences
cannabis use, with earlier onset being psychosis rather than vice versa. can provide valuable clues with respect to
more strongly related to psychosis. There One study of high risk young people has underlying neurobiological mechanisms
was also the suggestion of an interaction failed to report an association between and shared risk factors for psychotic
between cannabis use and vulnerability, cannabis use and psychosis risk. This study disorders. Categorical diagnostic criteria
with a higher risk of psychosis among identified 100 young people at ‘‘ultra do not provide the final word on these
cannabis users who reported psychotic high’’ risk for psychosis [46] because of disorders. The exploration of psychotic
symptoms at age 11 y. family history or prodromal symptoms of symptoms (or psychotic-like experiences)
Caspi and colleagues subsequently used psychosis (on the basis of one or more of has become a very fertile area of research
the cohort to examine an interaction the following: schizophrenia in a first [48]. These studies have generally shown
between cannabis use and a functional degree relative; the presence of attenuated that persons with these symptoms have an
polymorphism of the COMT gene that psychotic symptoms; or a brief limited elevated risk of being formally diagnosed
codes for dopamine in their effects on the psychosis) in whom 18% reported symp- with a psychosis later in life [49]. In the
risk of psychosis [44]. They found that the toms of cannabis dependence in the past section below, we review recent data on
25% of the cohort who were homozygous year. They assessed whether cannabis this issue.
for the polymorphism and used cannabis users were more likely to develop psychosis Second, most studies of the association
were 10.9 times more likely to have in the following year, but did not find any (with sample sizes of 1,000–4,000) have
developed a schizophreniform disorder association, regardless of the frequency of very low statistical power for detecting any
than peers with the same polymorphism cannabis use. effect that cannabis use has on the risk of
who did not use cannabis. In the absence Increasingly, researchers in the field of diagnosed psychotic disorders. More prev-
of this polymorphism, young adults who psychosis are examining the concept of alent outcomes, such as subclinical psy-
used cannabis were not at any increased psychotic spectrum features as risk factors chotic symptoms, have provided greater
risk of psychosis. for psychosis [47]. Recent work has found power to examine associations.
Apart from clinical diagnoses, several that these symptoms are common in the Third, we would also argue that the
longitudinal studies have also examined general population distribution and can persistence of a correlation between can-
the relationship between cannabis use and persist over a 20-y period. Two major nabis use and these variously measured
subclinical (or isolated) psychotic symp- trajectories have been identified: persistent outcomes is more suggestive of a robust
toms. Fergusson, Horwood, and Swain- ‘‘schizophrenic nuclear symptoms’’ (which relationship than the contrary. This is
Campbell have reported a longitudinal resemble psychosis) and persistent ‘‘schizo- because the use of differing measures of
study of the relationship between canna- typal symptoms’’ (more closely resembling varying predictive validity may be expect-
bis dependence at age 18 y and the schizotypal personality disorder) [47]. ed to attenuate rather than positively bias
number of psychotic symptoms reported Cannabis use during adolescence has been measures of association between cannabis
at age 21 y in the Christchurch birth found to be associated with ‘‘high load’’ use and psychosis. Finally, the Swedish
cohort in New Zealand [45]. They schizophrenia nuclear symptoms during and Dutch studies that have investigated
assessed cannabis dependence using adulthood—but not so for the schizotypal diagnosed psychotic disorders have found
DSM-IV criteria and psychotic symptoms symptom cluster. More frequent cannabis the same associations as studies of psy-
were assessed by ten items from the SCL- use was more strongly associated with chotic symptoms.
90. Because this was a birth cohort that persistent high load symptoms for the
had been assessed throughout childhood entire follow-up period. These findings Temporal Relationship
and adolescence Fergusson et al. were suggest that there may be different aetio-
able to adjust for a large number of logical dimensions for these two symptom Many prospective studies share the
potential confounding variables, includ- dimensions, with an interaction between weakness that they cannot precisely specify
ing self-reported psychotic symptoms at biological vulnerability and unique psy- the timing of first cannabis use and the
the previous assessment, other drug use, chosocial risk factors for each symptom onset of psychotic symptoms. Participants
and other psychiatric disorders. They cluster; limitations of the study included have usually been assessed once a year or

PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org 4 September 2009 | Volume 6 | Issue 9 | e1000133


less often and asked to retrospectively self-medication). This has led some to this strategy cannot be used to explore the
report their cannabis use during the past object to calculation of estimates of popu- association between cannabis and psycho-
year(s). This assessment has often been in lation attributable risk (PAR) because the sis. The use of twin or sibling-pair analyses
terms of the total number of times adjusted estimates are modest (typically can reduce unmeasured residual con-
cannabis was used, or the number of times around 2–3), and so open to the alternative founding to a certain extent [59], but
on average that cannabis was used each explanation of uncontrolled confounding. these studies will still be vulnerable to
week or month. Nonetheless, there are For example, some have suggested that the criticisms of incomplete control for con-
multiple prospective studies of representa- propensity to take risks and engage in founding. We have no choice then but to
tive samples of the general population, all socially disapproved behaviour may be a make cautious inferences about the role of
of which show that cannabis use at one common cause of cannabis use and psy- cannabis in psychosis on the basis of
point in time is associated with psychotic chotic symptoms [53]. Fergusson et al. observational studies.
symptoms at a later one, even after using a attempted to address these criticisms by
range of controls for confounding and using fixed effects regression models to The Importance of Population
various statistical approaches to analysis. adjust for all unmeasured confounders [31]. Attributable Risk
Studies undertaking more temporally Some may argue that a causal inference
fine-grained measurements have provid- demands evidence that the cessation of Calculation of a PAR is important to
ed results consistent with these cruder cannabis use reduces these risks, as the place the magnitude of the cannabis and
measurements. A French study using an evidence of risk reversal on cessation in the psychosis association in a population
experience sampling method [50] found a British doctors’ study strengthened the case health context. Arsenault et al. [11]
positive association between self-reported for a causal relationship between cigarette concluded that elimination of all cannabis
cannabis use and unusual perceptions, smoking and lung cancer and other diseases use would reduce the incidence of schizo-
and a negative association with hostility, [54]. However, even this evidence did not phrenia in the United Kingdom by
over periods of hours. In those with pre- persuade some sceptics in the case of approximately 8%, assuming that the
existing psychotic symptoms, cannabis tobacco use, with some arguing (uncon- relationship was ‘‘causal’’ in the sense that
use was more strongly associated with vincingly) that those at lowest risk of these schizophrenia would not have occurred in
strange impressions and unusual percep- adverse health outcomes found it easier to the absence of cannabis use; Zammit et al.
tions, and its use did not decrease feelings quit [55]. What was probably more impor- [35] similarly estimated that 13% of
of hostility [50]. tant was the consilience of a complex array schizophrenia cases in Sweden were at-
Another study, involving monthly as- of different types of evidence that con- tributable to cannabis use.
sessment of psychotic symptoms and vinced most public health officials that Nonetheless, these PAR estimates must
cannabis use over 10 mo among persons cigarette smoking caused these diseases be heavily qualified. Risk models related to
with psychotic disorders, similarly found [56]. In recent years, the range of method- complex and heterogeneous syndromes
that more frequent cannabis use in one ologies used to investigate the association like schizophrenia will never be fully
month was related to increases in psychot- between cannabis use and psychosis has specified. Further, standard PAR estimates
ic symptoms a month later [51]. These increased and there appears to be a similar cannot account for the possibility that
lines of evidence suggest that the temporal convergence of evidence that the associa- cannabis has brought forward the age of
relationship criterion is satisfied. tion between the two is causal. onset in an individual who would have
It is difficult to see more conclusive otherwise developed the illness at a later
Has the Evidence Base Been evidence being produced for cannabis as a age without exposure to cannabis [60].
contributory cause of psychosis, or for the Notwithstanding these limitations, a PAR
Affected by Publication Bias?
results of such studies to be as convincing has utility from a public health perspective
Publication bias is a potentially more as the evidence from cigarette smoking. in that it combines information about
serious concern: If negative results have This is because: the relationship between exposure-risk effect size and the preva-
been withheld from publication then the cannabis and psychosis is not as strong as lence of the exposure and helps the
consistent positive results would be far less that between smoking and lung cancer; the research community to prioritise public
impressive than they seem from the prevalence of cannabis use is so much health interventions, a central aim of the
published systematic reviews [52]. This lower than that for smoking; and the GBD exercise. Because it is unlikely that
possibility was investigated by the authors outcomes of psychosis are not as easy to we will ever have fully specified models,
of one systematic review who surveyed study as mortality was in the Doll and Hill we should use PARs cautiously and
researchers in the area asking about any follow up of the British doctors. How then conduct sensitivity analyses to assess the
studies with negative results that had not can we resolve the uncertainty that effects of uncertainty in our estimates.
been published [10]. They concluded that remains?
this was not a serious issue in this instance. Epidemiology is an imprecise science, Is It Premature to Suggest that
and recent experience has taught us to be Cannabis Is a Risk Factor for
Is There Residual Uncontrolled cautious in making causal inferences from Psychosis?
Confounding? observational studies [57,58]. With respect
to cannabis, the findings from prospective Some commentators may well argue that
The most difficult task in drawing causal longitudinal studies may still be vulnerable it is premature to conclude that the
inferences from observational studies is to residual confounding. The best way to relationships between cannabis use and
excluding the possibility that the relation- deal with both known and unknown psychosis are causal, which raises the
ship between cannabis use and psychosis is confounding from interventions (e.g., question of what the standard of proof
due to other uncontrolled factors (e.g., HRT) is to conduct randomised controlled should be causal inference. Some may
other drug use, genetic predisposition to trials to explore the impact of an exposure argue for ‘‘proof beyond reasonable
develop schizophrenia and use cannabis, or on the health outcome of interest. Clearly, doubt,’’ the standard implicitly used in the

PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org 5 September 2009 | Volume 6 | Issue 9 | e1000133


last iteration of the GBD [8]. It is rare, burden of disease (BoD) attributable to research imperative, since there will be no
however, to meet this standard of proof for cannabis if the critics are correct that estimated burden. If we do attempt to
noncommunicable diseases other than uncontrolled confounding explains the estimate burden, future work will examine
smoking-related diseases. What has relationships between cannabis use and the accuracy of our estimates and refine
changed since the last iteration of the psychosis. In Australia, for example, them as evidence accumulates. Debates
GBD? The broad approach to all risk cannabis use was included as a risk factor may emerge and (hopefully) improvements
factors has been to set the standard of proof in the Australian BoD study, assuming made as new evidence supports or chal-
at ‘‘more likely than not,’’ rather than causal relationships for cannabis depen- lenges the assumptions made. Estimates
‘‘beyond reasonable doubt.’’ If the latter dence, psychosis, suicide, and car crashes made in GBD 2005 should be seen as a
was the standard of proof, then no adverse [64]. Even after assuming that these first step in a process that can and should
health consequences of cannabis would be relationships were causal, cannabis was be improved with new data and new
considered apart from dependence. not a major contributor to disease burden insights, including work for future esti-
If we had treatments that resulted in in Australia, accounting for 0.2% of all mates of country and global disease
complete, immediate, and sustained remis- disease burden, which amounted to 10% burden.
sion for all individuals who develop psycho- of the total burden attributable to all illicit
sis, then the role of cannabis as an drugs [65]. These estimates are important Acknowledgments
aetiological agent may attract less attention. for public policy purposes, because failure
But schizophrenia remains a poorly under- to make them allows untested estimates to We thank the following individuals for com-
stood group of disorders. Even our best ments and reviews of previous versions of this
be offered in public policy debate. paper: Ruben Baler, Susan Weiss, Wilson
treatments are suboptimal [61,62]. In the In the GBD project, we are considering
absence of better treatments, the most Compton, James C. Anthony, and Maria Elena
several possible ways in which cannabis Medina Mora. The GBD Expert Group on
effective way to reduce the disability associ- and psychosis may be linked. A range of Mental Disorders and Illicit Drug Use compris-
ated with schizophrenia is to prevent its es the following core members: Professor Louisa
estimates will be made as follows: (1) a
occurrence when we can [63]. Thus, when Degenhardt (co-chair), Professor Harvey Whiteford
model that will assume greater disorder
considering potential risk factors for schizo- (co-chair), Professor John McGrath, Professor
severity among those using cannabis
phrenia, we argue that candidates that offer Wayne Hall, Dr. Guilherme Polanczyk, Dr.
regularly who have already developed
the opportunity for public health interven- Shekhar Saxena, Professor Oye Gureje, Professor
the disorder; (2) a model that will assume Ronald Kessler, Dr. Cille Kennedy, Dr. Maria
tions should be accorded more attention
the association reflects earlier onset of the Elena Medina-Mora, and Professor Martin
(e.g., education about the potential risks of
disorder among those who would have Prince.
cannabis use). Even exposures that may
developed it anyway; (3) a model that will
account for a small attributable fraction of
those with the disorder warrant scrutiny. assume reduced remission from schizo- Author Contributions
phrenia once it has developed; and (4) a
ICMJE criteria for authorship read and met:
model that assumes increased incidence of
A Way Forward LD WDH ML JM JM BC HW TV. Wrote the
schizophrenia. first draft of the paper: LD. Contributed to the
Making estimates of the proportion of It is important to consider the conse- writing of the paper: WDH ML JM JM BC HW
psychoses attributable to cannabis will in quences of not estimating this risk. There TV.
effect provide worst case estimates of the will be a reduced public health, policy, or

References
1. Degenhardt L, Hall WD (2001) The association regional burden of disease attributable to selected schizophrenia: model projections of the impact
between psychosis and problematic drug use major risk factors. Geneva: World Health Organi- of the rise in cannabis use on historical and future
among Australian adults: findings from the sation. pp 1109–1176. trends in schizophrenia in England and Wales.
National Survey of Mental Health and Well- 9. Degenhardt L, Hall WD (2006) Is cannabis a Addiction 102: 597–606.
being. Psychol Med 31: 659–668. contributory cause of psychosis? Can J Psychiatry 17. Zammit S, Moore TM, Lingford-Hughes A,
2. Tien AY, Anthony JC (1990) Epidemiological 51: 556–565. Barnes T, Jones P, et al. (2008) Effects of cannabis
analysis of alcohol and drug use as a risk factor for 10. Moore T, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A, use on outcomes of psychotic disorders: system-
psychotic experiences. J Nerv Ment Dis 178: Barnes T, Jones PB, et al. (2007) Cannabis use atic review. Br J Psychiatry 193: 357–363.
473–480. and risk of psychotic or affective mental health 18. Hambrecht M, Haefner H (2000) Cannabis,
3. Wheatley M (1998) The prevalence and relevance outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet 370: vulnerability, and the onset of schizophrenia: an
of substance use in detained schizophrenic 319–328. epidemiological perspective. Aust N Z J Psychiatry
patients. J Forensic Psychiatr 9: 114–129. 11. Arseneault L, Cannon M, Witton J, Murray RM 34: 468–475.
4. Barbee JG, Clark PD, Crapanzo MS, Heintz GC, (2004) Causal association between cannabis and 19. Hall W, Degenhardt L, Lynskey M (2001) The
Kehoe CE (1989) Alcohol and substance abuse psychosis: examination of the evidence. health and psychological consequences of canna-
among schizophrenic patients presenting to an Br J Psychiatry 184: 110–117. bis use. Canberra: Australian Publishing Service.
emergency psychiatry service. J Nerv Ment Dis 12. Hall WD (1998) Cannabis use and psychosis. 20. Julien R (2001) A primer of drug action. New
177: 400–417. Drug Alcohol Rev 17: 433–444. York: Worth Publishers.
5. World Bank (1993) World development report 13. Henquet C, Di Forti M, Morrison PD, 21. Murray RM, Morrison PD, Henquet C, Di
1993: investing in health. New York: World Bank. Kuepper R, Murray R (2008) Gene-environment Forti M (2007) Cannabis, the mind and society:
6. Lopez AD, Mathers C, Ezzati M, Jamison D, interplay between cannabis and psychosis. Schi- the hash realities. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:
Murray C (2006) Global and regional burden of zophr Bull 34: 1111–1121. 885–895.
disease and risk factors, 2001: systematic analysis 14. Ajdacic-Gross V, Lauber C, Warnke I, Hacker H, 22. Boucher AA, Arnold JC, Duffy L, Schofield PR,
of population health data. Lancet 367: Murray RM, et al. (2009) Changing incidence of Micheau J, et al. (2007) Heterozygous neuregulin
1747–1757. psychotic disorders among the young in Zurich. 1 mice are more sensitive to the behavioural
7. Hill AB (1965) The environment and disease: Schizophrenia Res 95: 9–18. effects of Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Psycho-
association or causation? Proceed Roy Soc 15. Degenhardt L, Hall W, Lynskey M (2003) Testing pharmacology (Berl) 192: 325–336.
Medicine 58: 295–300. hypotheses about the relationship between can- 23. Gorriti MA, Rodriguez de Fonseca F, Navarro M,
8. Degenhardt L, Hall WD, Lynskey M, Warner- nabis and psychosis. Drug Alcohol Depend 71: Palomo T (1999) Chronic (-)-delta9-tetrahydro-
Smith M (2004) Illicit drug use. In: Ezzati M, 37–48. cannabinol treatment induces sensitization to the
Lopez AD, Rodgers A, Murray C, eds. Com- 16. Hickman M, Vickerman P, MacLeod J, psychomotor effects of amphetamine in rats.
parative quantification of health risks: global and Kirkbride J, Jones PB (2007) Cannabis and Eur J Pharmacol 365: 133–142.

PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org 6 September 2009 | Volume 6 | Issue 9 | e1000133


24. Vigano D, Guidali C, Petrosino S, Realini N, the mentally ill: prevalence, reasons for use, and psychosis and depression among young adults
Rubino T, et al. (2009) Involvement of the effects on illness. Am J Orthopsychiatry 64: with psychotic disorders: findings from a ten-
endocannabinoid system in phencyclidine-in- 30–39. month prospective study. Psychol Med 37:
duced cognitive deficits modelling schizophrenia. 39. Green B, Young R, Kavanagh D (2005) Cannabis 927–934.
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12: 599–614. use and misuse prevalence among people with 52. Young N, Loannidis J, Al-Ubaydli O (2008) Why
25. Bell D (1973) The experimental reproduction of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 187: 306–313. current publication practices may distort science.
amphetamine psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 29: 40. Anthony JC, Helzer JE (1991) Syndromes of drug PLoS Medicine 5: e201. doi:10.1371/journal.
35–40. abuse and dependence. In: Robins LN, pmed.0050201.
26. Bhattacharyya S, Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Regier DA, eds. Psychiatric disorders in America: 53. MacLeod J, Oaks R, Copello A, Crome I,
Martin-Santos R, Nosarti C, et al. (2009) Delta- the Epidemiologic Catchment Area. New York: Egger M, et al. (2004) Psychological and social
9-tetrahydrocannabinol modulates medial tempo- Free Press. sequelae of cannabis and other illicit drug use by
ral and striatal functions in humans: a neural basis 41. Reiger DA, Farmer ME, Rae DS, Locke BZ, young people: a systematic review of longitudinal,
for the effects of cannabis on learning and Keith SJ, et al. (1990) Comorbidity of mental general population studies. Lancet 363:
psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 442–451. disorders with alcohol and other drug abuse: 1579–1588.
27. Borgwardt S, Allen P, Bhattacharyya S, Fusar- results from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area 54. Doll R (1998) Uncovering the effects of smoking:
Poli P, Crippa JA, et al. (2008) Neural basis of (ECA) Study. JAMA 264: 2511–2518. historical perspective. Stat Methods Med Res 7:
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol: 42. Cuffel B, J, Heinthoff KA, Lawson W (1993) 87–117.
effects during response intervention. Biol Psychi- Correlates of patterns of substance abuse among 55. Eysenck SB (1980) The causes and effects of
atry 64: 966–973. patients with schizophrenia. Hosp Community smoking. London: Temple Smith.
28. D’Souza DC, Perry EB, MacDougall L, Psychiatry 44: 247–251. 56. Brandt AM (2007) The cigarette century. New
Ammerman Y, Cooper T, et al. (2004) The 43. Andreasson S, Engstrom A, Allebeck P, York: Basic Books.
psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9- Rydberg U (1987) Cannabis and schizophrenia: 57. Davey Smith G, Ebrahim S (2001) Epidemiolo-
tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: a longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. gy–is it time to call it a day? In J Epidemiol 30:
implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharma- Lancet 2: 1483–1486. 1–11.
cology 29: 1558–1572. 44. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, McClay J,
58. Smith GD (2001) Reflections on the limitations to
29. D’Souza DC, Cho HS, Perry E, Krystal JH Murray R, et al. (2005) Moderation of the effect
epidemiology. J Clin Epidemiol 54: 325–331.
(2004) Cannabinoid ‘model’ psychosis, dopamine- of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psy-
59. Lynskey MT, Heath AC, Bucholz KK,
cannabinoid interactions and implications for chosis by a functional polymorphism in the
Slutske WS (2003) Escalation of drug use in
schizophrenia. In: Castle D, Murray R, eds. catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: longitudinal
early-onset cannabis users vs co-twin controls.
Marijuana and madness: psychiatry and neuro- evidence of a gene X environment interaction.
JAMA 289: 427–433.
biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Biol Psychiatry 57: 1117–1127.
30. Thomas H (1996) A community survey of adverse 45. F er g u s s o n D M , H o r w o o d L J , S w a in - 60. Vineis P, Kriebel D (2006) Causal models in
effects of cannabis use. Drug Alcohol Depend 42: Campbell NR (2003) Cannabis dependence epidemiology: past inheritance and genetic future.
201–207. and psychotic symptoms in young people. Environmental Health 5: 21.
31. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM (2005) Psychol Med 33: 15–21. 61. Andrews G, Issakidis C, Sanderson K, Corry J,
Tests of causal linkages between cannabis use and 46. Phillips LJ, Curry C, Yung AR, Yuen HP, Lapsley H (2004) Utilising survey data to inform
psychotic symptoms. Addiction 100: 354–366. Adlard S, et al. (2003) Cannabis use is not public policy: comparison of the cost-effectiveness
32. Arseneault L, Cannon M, Poulton R, Murray R, associated with the development of psychosis in of treatment of ten mental disorders.
Caspi A, et al. (2002) Cannabis use in adolescence an ‘ultra’ high-risk group. Aust N Z J Psychiatry Br J Psychiatry 184: 526–533.
and risk for adult psychosis: longitudinal prospec- 36: 800–806. 62. Andrews G, Sanderson K, Corry J, Issakidis C,
tive study. Br Med J 325: 1212–1213. 47. Rossler W, Reicher-Rössler A, Angst J, Murray R, Lapsley H (2003) Cost-effectiveness of current
33. Henquet C, Krabbendam L, Spauwen J, Gamma A, et al. (2007) Psychotic experiences in and optimal treatment for schizophrenia.
Kaplan C, Lieb R, et al. (2005) Prospective the general population: a twenty-year prospective Br J Psychiatry 183: 427–435.
cohort study of cannabis use, predisposition for community study. Schizophr Res 92: 1–14. 63. Saha S, Barendregt JJ, Vos T, Whiteford H,
psychosis, and psychotic symptoms in young 48. Eaton WW, Romanoski A, Anthony JC, McGrath J (2008) Modelling disease frequency
people. Br Med J 330: 11–14. Nestadt G (1991) Screening for psychosis in the measures in schizophrenia epidemiology. Schi-
34. van Os J, Bak M, Hanssen M, Bijl RV, de general population with a self-report interview. zophr Res 140: 246–256.
Graaf R, et al. (2002) Cannabis use and psychosis: J Nerv Ment Dis 179: 689–693. 64. Begg S, Vos T, Barker DC, Stanley L, Lopez A
a longitudinal population-based study. 49. Engqvist U, Rydelius PA (2008) The occurrence (2008) Burden of disease and injury in Australia in
Am J Epidemiol 156: 319–327. and nature of early signs of schizophrenia and the new millennium: measuring health loss from
35. Zammit S, Lewis G (2004) Exploring the psychotic mood disorders among former child diseases, injuries and risk factors. Med J Aust 188:
relationship between cannabis use and psychosis. and adolescent psychiatric patients followed into 36–40.
Addiction 99: 1353–1355. adulthood. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment 65. Begg S, Vos T, Barker B, Stevenson C, Stanley L,
36. Schneier RR, Siris SG (1987) A review of Health 2: 30. et al. (2007) The burden of disease and injury in
psychoactive substance use and abuse in schizo- 50. Verdoux H, Gindre C, Sorbara F, Tournier M, Australia 2003. Canberra: Australian Institute of
phrenia: patterns of drug choice. J Nerv Ment Dis Swendsen J (2002) Cannabis use and the Health and Welfare.
175: 641–652. expression of psychosis vulnerability in daily life. 66. Ezzati M, Lopez AD, Rodgers A, Murray R, eds
37. Smith FR, Hucker S (1994) Schizophrenia and Eur Psychiatry 17: 180S–180S. (2004) Comparative quantification of health risks:
substance abuse. Br J Psychiatry 165: 13–21. 51. Degenhardt L, Tennant C, Gilmour S, global and regional burden of disease attributable
38. Warner R, Taylor D, Wright J, Sloat A, Schofield D, Nash L, et al. (2007) The temporal to selected major risk factors. 2nd ed. Geneva:
Springett G, et al. (1994) Substance use among dynamics of relationships between cannabis, World Health Organisation.

PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org 7 September 2009 | Volume 6 | Issue 9 | e1000133

You might also like