You are on page 1of 10

International Journal of Behavioral Development # 2002 The International Society for the

2002, 26 (2), 145–153 Study of Behavioural Development


http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/01650254.html DOI: 10.1080/01650250042000708

Yoruba toddlers’ engagement in errands and cognitive


performance on the Yoruba Mental Subscale

Oluyomi A. Ogunnaike Robert F. Houser Jr.


University of Wisconsin—Stevens Point, USA Tufts University, Medford, USA

Responsibility training is an essential component of child-rearing practices in several African


societies. To inculcate responsibility, caregivers allocate their children household duties and send
them on errands; these include (but are not limited to) fetching objects and purchasing items. Such
errands foster the acquisition of cognitive, social, and economic competencies. In this paper, the
relationship between the types of errands engaged in by Yoruba toddlers of southwest Nigeria and
cognitive performance using an adapted version of the Bayley Mental Scales of Development,
referred to as the Yoruba Mental Subscale, is examined. Findings revealed that children who engaged
in purchasing items and retrieving speciŽ c objects for the caregiver had a signiŽ cantly higher
performance on the Yoruba Mental Subscale compared to children who did not engage in such
errands. With regard to performance on the Bayley Mental Scales, Ž ndings revealed no signiŽ cant
association with the errands examined. The implications of these Ž ndings for cross-cultural research
and early childhood education are discussed.

Children’s cognitive and social development is inseparable from an apt description of the role of errands in the following words
the social milieu in that what children learn is a cultural ‘‘adults presumably keep some kind of mental tally of the
curriculum: from the earliest days, they build on the skills and proportion of errands that a given child performs adequately,
perspectives of their society with the aid of other people.
and this serves as an index of how ‘tumikila’ (responsible) the
(Rogoff, 1990, p. 190)
child is’’ (p. 64). It follows then that the more errands a child is
able to perform successfully, the more she/he is entrusted with
Societies differ in terms of child-rearing practices and goals for other responsibilities.
development (Aina, 1992; LeVine et al., 1994; Super & The above research emphasised the importance of errands
Harkness, 1986). For example, responsibility, obedience, in child rearing. However, there is a paucity of research on the
respect for elders, and helpfulness are central to the child- relationship between speciŽ c errands and developmental out-
rearing practices in several African cultures (Babatunde, 1992; come, especially cognitive performance of African children.
Nsamenang, 1992; Zeitlin et al., 1995). Such research is needed to clarify the dynamics and potentials
Sending the child on errands within and outside the home is of errands vis-à-vis child development with respect to
one of the ways through which responsibility is fostered in intelligence. Intelligence is deŽ ned differently across societies,
African societies (Harkness & Super, 1992; Nsamenang & thus, in the following section, we examine how intelligence is
Lamb, 1994; Serpell, 1993; Whiting & Whiting, 1975). deŽ ned, in particular, within the African context.
Examples of errands include putting things away, fetching
nearby or far objects, relaying messages or bringing items to
neighbours, and buying items from neighbours (Lancy, 1996;
Emic deŽ nitions of intelligence
Ogunnaike, 1997). These errands provide the African child
with opportunities to learn about the environment, dynamics The deŽ nition of intelligence within cross-cultural contexts has
of interacting with others, and practise being helpful and received much attention and documentation (Berry, 1984;
responsible (Nsamenang, 1992; Serpell, 1993). GreenŽ eld, 1994; Mundy-Castle, 1975; Serpell, 1993). Central
Nsamenang (1992) noted that caregivers assess the social to these authors’ deŽ nitions and perspectives of intelligence is
maturity of the growing African child through his/her ability to the signiŽ cance of the cultural environment and values.
perform errands and the child’s ability to complete errands Intelligence according to Mundy-Castle (1975) is re ective
successfully in uences caregiver perception of the child’s of ‘‘different strategies of adaptation involving distinctive
responsibility and level of competence. Serpell (1993) gave patterns of culturally determined skills’’ (p. 345). Among

Correspondence should be addressed to Oluyomi A. Ogunnaike, This paper is based on Ž ndings from my doctoral dissertation, which
School of Education, University of Wisconsin—Stevens Point, Stevens examined family sociodemographics and cognitive performance of
Point, WI 54481, USA; e-mail: oogunnai@uwsp.edu Yoruba toddlers on Bayley Mental Scales of Development. UNICEF
The present study was part of a larger study titled ‘‘Child (Nigeria) and Tufts University School of Nutrition provided the
Development for the Computer Age’’ conducted in Ogun and Lagos funding for the study.
States, Nigeria by Tufts University School of Nutrition with UNICEF I thank Professor Marian Zeitlin, UNICEF/Tufts University Project
Nigeria and University of Lagos, Nigeria. The project was sponsored Director for permission to utilise the data on mental performance of
by a grant from UNICEF, New York and the Joint Nutrition School Yoruba toddlers. I also thank Professor Robert Serpell, Dr Heidi
Programme, Italy. Keller, and Dr Patricia Caro for reviewing the manuscript.
146 OGUNNAIKE AND HOUSER / ERRANDS AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE

Western societies, intelligence is largely deŽ ned in terms of member of society (Babatunde, 1992; Ogunnaike, 1997;
technological skills, while among non-Western societies, Ohuche & Otaala, 1981; Timyan, 1988).
especially in Africa, intelligence is deŽ ned in terms of social
skills and relationships (Mundy-Castle, 1975). The works of
researchers, namely, Dasen (1984), Nsamenang and Lamb Housing, trading, and responsibility training among
(1994), Serpell (1993), Super and Harkness (1986, 1997) the Yoruba
lend credence to the centrality of social skills, such as It is common to Ž nd houses built close together among Yoruba
responsibility, obedience, helpfulness, and using initiative, in communities. The houses are rectangular single-storey build-
the African deŽ nition of intelligence. For example, among the ings with a central corridor and a yard at the back (Eades,
Kipsigis of Kenya, an intelligent child was perceived as one 1980). This style of house is commonly referred to as ‘‘face-to-
who performs household chores, goes on errands, and takes face’’ (Zeitlin et al., 1995). As many as three or more different
care of the younger ones without being asked (Super & families can live together under a single roof on a rental basis.
Harkness, 1986). Depending on affordability, a family may occupy one or two
Among the Baoule, the term ‘‘n’glouele’’ (Dasen, 1984, p. rooms in a house of 6 to 8 rooms. The proximity of houses and
126) translates to ‘‘intelligence’’ with a prominent attribute, communal living arrangements allow for close adult monitor-
‘‘o ti kpa’’. This term connotes responsibility, obedience, ing of children’s activities. For example, if a child was found
willingness and/or initiative to perform chores, errands, or walking away from home, someone was likely to call the
tasks, without prior instructions. Compliance, politeness, attention of the child’s mother, sibling, or relative to verify
and respect for elders, ‘‘agnyhie’’ (Dasen, 1984, p. 127) whether the child was sent on an errand. If so, the neighbour
were also highly valued attributes of intelligence among the was then likely to maintain a close watch or assign a suitable
Baoule. The A-Chewa people of Zambia deŽ ne intelligence, companion for the child (Aina, Agiobu-Kemmer, Etta, Zeitlin,
‘‘nzelu’’ as a combination of social responsibility and & Setiloane, 1993; Ogunnaike, 1997).
cognitive sharpness (Serpell, 1993). The social responsibility Trading is a major occupation among the Yoruba. Thus, the
dimension is referred to as ‘‘tumikila’’ (Serpell, 1993, p. 36), child is exposed to a rich language utilised by caregivers as they
which connotes reliability, cooperation, obedience, and engage in commercial and other transactions (Ohuche &
helpfulness. Otaala, 1981). The child observes and listens to, and imitates
The present study examines the relationship between older siblings, mothers, neighbours, and customers exchange
intelligence re ected in responsibility training (errands) and greetings, engage in conversations about recent or past events,
developmental outcome among Yoruba toddlers of southwest purchase items, retrieve or put away objects, bargain and
Nigeria. Particular attention is paid to how the child’s ability to negotiate prices of items such as foodstuffs, household items,
perform speciŽ c errands (e.g., putting things away and and goods (Aina, 1992; Timyan, 1988).
purchasing items) relate to cognitive performance on an
adapted version of the Bayley Mental Scale of Development
named the Yoruba Mental Subscale.
Customs of child care and child rearing

The conceptual framework of the study This aspect emphasises caregiver efforts in facilitating the
development of the child through speciŽ c ‘‘sequences of
First, an overview is provided of the Yoruba caregiving behavior or institutional arrangements’’ (Super & Harkness,
practices, settings, and parental ethnotheories as they pertain 1997, p. 27). Child rearing among the Yoruba can be
to errands, organised according to the Developmental Niche described as a deliberate attempt to ‘‘teach’’ the child from
framework (Super & Harkness, 1986, 1997). an early age how to relate to others appropriately, be helpful,
The developmental niche framework as described by Super responsible, and respectful (Babatunde, 1992; Zeitlin et al.,
and Harkness (1986, 1997) represents an attempt to bridge a 1995). The child is taught by family members, relatives, and
gap between psychology and anthropology to explain the other adults how to listen, observe, and imitate others who
development of the young child within a cultural context. The exhibit obedience, respect for elders, and sel essness (Baba-
developmental niche concept was used to explore the process tunde, 1992; Ohuche & Otaala, 1981). Respect for elders is
through which culture mediates child development, and it re ected particularly through greetings. Boys are taught how to
consists of three component subsystems: (1) physical and prostrate, while girls are taught how to curtsy (Babatunde,
social settings in which the child lives; (2) culturally regulated 1992). Yoruba caregivers teach their children to be responsible
customs of child care and child rearing; and (3) psychology of through participation in household chores and errands. Before
the caretakers. age Ž ve, Yoruba girls are taught how to perform household
chores, namely, washing, sweeping, and cooking and how to
take care of their younger siblings (Akinware, Wilson-
Physical and social settings Oyelaran, Ladipo, Pierce, & Zeitlin, 1992).

This component highlights the importance of the type of


The process of early responsibility training among the
environment and people to which the child is exposed early in
Yoruba
life (Super & Harkness, 1997). The Yoruba child grows up in a
highly stimulating environment full of human and material Because responsibility training is commonly practised in
resources. The child is exposed to multiple caregiving by family several African societies, this section will provide relevant
members, neighbours, and relatives, all of whom are respon- information about such practices among the Yoruba culture,
sible for ensuring that the child grows up to be a responsible and also cite Ž ndings from a few other African cultures.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2002, 26 (2), 145–153 147

As early as two years of age, Yoruba children are asked to Psychology of the caretakers
fetch nearby or far objects, put objects away, purchase items,
perform household chores (e.g., sweeping or washing), and According to Super and Harkness (1997), parental behaviours
also how to carry things for older members of the community and expectations for their children are guided by ‘‘ethnothe-
(Aina et al., 1993). Among the Kpelle of Liberia, young ories’’. Ethnotheories refer to the beliefs about the nature of
children engage in carrying water and Ž rewood and serving children, what constitutes their needs, parenting goals, as well
meals (‘‘fetch and carry’’, Lancy, 1996, p. 145). In their study as the beliefs about effective styles of child rearing. For
of six cultures, Whiting and Whiting (1975) noted that example, a ‘‘good child’’ is described as loyal, hardworking,
mothers in what they termed ‘‘simple societies’’ (e.g. Nyan- obedient, and respectful by Kpelle adults in Liberia (Lancy,
songo of Kenya) expected their children to convey message, 1996). The Yoruba people believe that children are ‘‘the
fetch objects, and work on the farm and around the home. highest good’’ (Zeitlin et al., 1995, p. 157). Children represent
Successful completion of errands is highly valued and the vessel through which societal goals, values, hope, and
rewarded through praise and increased assignment of domestic aspirations are projected and nurtured by the young and the
and other responsibilities (Lancy, 1996, Lloyd, 1970; Whiting old (Aina, 1992).
& Edwards, 1988).
In teaching the child how to perform errands, the caregiver
How caregivers’ beliefs affect responsibility training
might begin by asking the child to fetch nearby objects or put
familiar objects away where they belong. Gradually, the child is
among the Yoruba
asked to perform errands that take him/her away from the Central to Yoruba cultural beliefs is the concept of responsi-
immediate household. Among the Yoruba, successful perfor- bility vis-à-vis mobility. Yoruba caregivers believe that early
mance of an errand may require the ability to comprehend and attainment of mobility has implications for adequate and
process given information (Aina et al., 1993; Ogunnaike, effective training in responsibility (Aina et al., 1993). A Yoruba
1997). For example, a Yoruba child instructed by the mother child that walks early is regarded as healthy and sent on errands
as follows ‘‘Lo sinu yara, ko si ba mi gbe apo ti mo ma gbe lo si oja or trusted with other responsibilities. Thus, the child’s motor
wa’’ (literally, ‘‘go inside the room and bring me the purse I ability is fostered from an early age. For example, Agiobu-
take with me to the market’’) will need to work out what part of Kemmer (1984) noted that Yoruba caregivers foster early
the room the purse might be, the colour, shape, and size of the mobility by placing a favourite toy at a distance for the child to
particular purse. The ability to work out these aspects with or retrieve. Aina and colleagues (Aina et al., 1993), and Zeitlin
without assistance from older siblings or others facilitates a and associates (Zeitlin et al., 1995) learned that Yoruba
successful performance of this particular errand. caregivers enhance mobility by reducing the child’s intake of
heavy staple foods rich in carbohydrate or starch. Findings
from interviews and informal observations of low-income
How adults facilitate responsibility training among the
households in Lagos and Ogun states reveal that from the
Yoruba and other African cultures age of three months, Yoruba caregivers prop up girls in a sitting
Findings from informal observations and interviews held with position with cushions (Aina et al., 1993). Due to the fear of
certain Yoruba caregivers (Aina et al., 1993) also suggest how crushing the testicles, boys, from the later age of Ž ve months,
adults in the neighbourhood enhance the child’s successful were propped up in a sitting position with cushions. Thus, it
completion of errands. Due to the proximity of houses in could be assumed that mobility among girls occurs earlier than
several low-income neighbourhoods (refer to the section on boys. Such early mobility among Yoruba girls is likely to affect
physical setting), adults overtly or covertly watch out for how frequently they are sent on errands compared to their male
children as they walk around and play with their friends. counterparts.
Depending on the perceived need of a child sent on errands, In conclusion, responsibility training and multiple caregiv-
the adults might assign an older companion to facilitate the ing are central to parenting beliefs in many African societies,
completion of the assigned task. Such facilitation has been particularly, among Yoruba caregivers. These beliefs, espe-
documented in Vygotskian literature (see GreenŽ eld, 1984; cially responsibility training, guide the caregiver-child interac-
Rogoff, 1990; cf. Lancy, 1996; Segall, Dasen, Berry, & tions that are re ected in practices such as reducing the intake
Poortinga, 1999). of heavy staple foods, propping girls in a sitting position earlier
Yoruba adults facilitate the child’s engagement and than boys and sending the child on errands. The proximity of the
accomplishment of a task through tacit guidance (Aina et al., houses (physical and social setting) and a sense of communal
1993; Rogoff, 1990). These authors note that if a child is sent responsibility for the young enhance multiple caregiving
to purchase an item from a neighbour, the child’s mother will among the Yoruba people of southwest Nigeria.
inform the neighbour about the child’s mission. Both adults,
although they may be engaged in other activities or transac-
tions, are likely to monitor the child’s itinerary or ask an older Errands and child developmental outcomes
sibling to keep an eye on the child.
In summary, responsibility, obedience, helpfulness, and To explore the relationship between errands and cognitive
reliability are central to child rearing among the Yoruba and performance, an adapted version of the Bayley Mental Scales
other African cultures (Lancy, 1996; Serpell, 1993; Whiting & of Development (MSD; Bayley, 1969) referred to as the
Whiting, 1975). Children are expected to listen, watch, and Yoruba Mental Subscale (YMS) was designed. This test
imitate behaviours re ective of obedience, responsibility, and format was chosen in order to have an evaluation distinct from
helpfulness. In particular, adults teach, guide, and provide the naturally occurring contexts.
necessary support that enhances children’s successful comple- The study also examined the impact of location on
tion of errands and chores (Segall et al., 1999). children’s performance based on the literature that suggests
148 OGUNNAIKE AND HOUSER / ERRANDS AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE

different developmental outcomes for children living in urban Bayley Mental Scale of Development was also examined. The
versus rural settings (Aina et al., 1993; Hope & Bierman, 1998; following hypotheses guided the study:
Widmayer et al., 1990). In a study cited by Nerlove and
Snipper (1981), urban children were reported to experience 1. Because errands constitute an important cultural activity
more exposure to a variety of languages, several adult Ž gures, geared toward promoting competency among Yoruba
and lived in more crowded conditions compared to their children (Akinware et al., 1992; Zeitlin et al., 1995),
counterparts in rural settings. Also, urban children differed those who engaged in errands will have a signiŽ cantly
from rural children in their performance on cognitive tasks. higher performance on the Yoruba Mental Subscale and
Urban children, who were found to be more assertive and the Bayley Mental Scale of Development compared to
independent, had signiŽ cantly higher performance on explora- their counterparts who did not.
tory-manipulative tasks compared to rural children. The latter, 2. Errands will differ in their association with cognitive
however, performed better on experimental tasks which performance. Children who are sent on errands that take
required compliance and deference to the experimenter. Other them away from the immediate household, especially,
authors, such as Aina and associates (1993), emphasise the purchasing items, utilise a variety of skills (e.g.,
need to consider children’s familiarity with test materials, perceptual-motor, language, memory, and problem-
which may or may not be associated with urban-rural settings solving strategies) (Ohuche & Otaala, 1981). Thus,
in considering the role of location on performance. More children who purchase items for caregivers will have a
studies are needed on the urban-rural differences in children’s signiŽ cantly higher cognitive performance on the YMS
performance before Ž nal conclusions can be made. than children who did not.

The study was part of a larger, cross-sectional study


examining the relationship between parenting beliefs, caregiv-
The Yoruba mental subscale and study ing practices, and developmental outcomes outcome (e.g.,
hypotheses cognitive competence and nutritional development) among the
Yoruba people of southwest Nigeria.
A total of 25 items from the Bayley Mental Scales of
Development re ective of salient culturally relevant skills
fostered by Yoruba caregivers, namely, listening, observation, Method
imitation, and ability to follow instructions (Babatunde, 1992;
Ohuche & Otaala, 1981) were chosen and adapted as the Participants
Yoruba Mental Subscale. These items required the child to
name or point to objects, imitate the tester’s action, listen and A total of 207 low-income households in urban, semi-urban,
obey the tester (comprehension). Items that required naming and rural Yoruba communities participated in the study.
and comprehension were included based on Ž ndings from Eligibility for household selection included the following
UNICEF studies and focus group discussions conducted by speciŽ cations: mother in Yoruba; target child is 22–26 months
Tade Aina and associates (Aina et al., 1993). Aina’s Ž ndings old; birth certiŽ cate to verify age; child is not a twin; both
suggest that caregivers, especially, older siblings, encouraged mother and child are present in the household. A systematic
younger children to identify and name common objects (e.g., sampling system that involved the selection of every second
plates, silverware, clothes, food items, body parts, and pictures house was used to ensure the inclusion of all eligible house-
around the house). Further, Yoruba mothers who were holds in the study.
interviewed by Aina and his associates on how they prepared The urban sample (n ˆ 95) was drawn from a predomi-
their toddlers for formal schooling emphasised the importance nantly low-income settlement of Makoko in Lagos City. The
of the child’s ability to name objects in the environment (Aina rural sample (n ˆ 20) was drawn from six selected small rural
et al., 1993). villages in Ifo-Otta Local Government Area (LGA) in Ogun
The process of constructing the YMS involved statistical State. The semi-urban sample (n ˆ 92) was selected from two
analyses of the initial 25 items for consistency. Only 15 of these medium-sized, semi-urban towns in Ifo-Otta LGA. The rural
items were found to have high alpha coefŽ cients, and thus, and semi-urban settlements are located off the main Lagos-
were utilised as the YMS. This was done to establish a Abeokuta road about an hour from Lagos by vehicle.
measure/index that re ects cognitive abilities fostered in this
niche that are accessible to elicitation from this population in a
test format, as distinct from naturally recurring contexts. The
Materials
items on the YMS are described in detail in the method section The following materials were used: (1) A comprehensive
of this paper. sociodemographic questionnaire containing questions about
Similar adaptations of the Bayley Mental Scales was earlier child care, responsibility training (errands), beliefs and
carried out by Sigman et al. (1988) in their work with Embu attitudes, and socioeconomic status (age, occupation, educa-
toddlers in a rural community in Kenya, East Africa by using tion) of caregivers. (2) A Yoruba Mental Subscale, adapted
local materials or omitting items that were unfamiliar to Embu from the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley, 1969).
toddlers. The Subscale consisted of 15 items selected from the Bayley
In this study, the relationship between Yoruba toddler’s Mental Scales. They included items on building with red cubes
engagement in errands, namely, putting things away, and (Nos. 143, 161, 154); imitation of crayon strokes (No. 125);
purchasing items, and cognitive performance on the YMS in picture identiŽ cation (Nos. 132, 139); discrimination of
three different locations, namely, rural, semi-urban, and urban, objects (Nos. 144, 152); prepositions (Nos. 158, 162, 163);
was examined. The relationship between these errands and the and object naming (Nos. 130, 138, 141). These items were
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2002, 26 (2), 145–153 149

chosen based on their re ection of skills such as listening, Table 1


respect, obedience, and following instructions. Yoruba eth- Yoruba Mental Subscale: items and variables
notheory particularly fosters the ability to listen, watch keenly
what is going on in the environment, and say little (especially in Items % N
the presence of strangers or adults) except when asked to.
Builds towers of 6 cubes 73.40 207
Builds towers of 8 cubes 42 207
Builds train of cubes 52 202
Design and procedure Finds 2 objects 89.70 204
Fieldwork began in the second week of 1988, after two months Points to 3 pictures 59.50 200
of training, instrument development, and pre-testing by Points to 5 pictures 36 200
Ž eldworkers. Nine female Yoruba students, two of whom were Imitates crayon strokes 91.60 203
graduates (one is the Ž rst author) of psychology and sociology Discriminates 2 objects 88.60 201
at the University of Lagos, Nigeria, visited selected households Discriminates 3 objects 65.80 201
Understands 2 prepositions 35.80 204
where interviews and testing took place.
Understands 3 prepositions 9.80 204
Mothers were interviewed in Yoruba, the local language, on Understands concept of one 17.20 204
feeding, child care, parental aspirations and beliefs, responsi- Names 1 object 45.10 204
bility training, health, exposure to media, literacy, socio- Names 2 objects 20.70 203
economic status, and family history. Pertinent to this study was Names 3 objects 11.20 205
the question about responsibility training through errands.
Mothers were asked ‘‘Have you already started to teach
(child’s name) to do small errands for you? Has the child
started to: (a) put things away? (b) buy things?’’ Yoruba Mental Subscale (YMS). A total of 25 items that
Yoruba mothers were also asked other questions about required comprehension, imitation, obedience, and following
responsibility training such as fetching objects, washing own directions were chosen from the Bayley Mental Scales. Each
hands, washing cups and plates, and doing household chores item was recoded on a 2-point scale (0 ˆ fail, 1 ˆ pass), the
(e.g., sweeping and washing clothes). As the mother answered higher score indicative of a pass. Some omitted or refused
these and other questions, another Ž eldworker tested the child items, for example, pointing to parts of the doll (No. 128) and
on the Bayley Mental and Motor Scales of Development. The attains toy with stick (No. 122), were not included in the YMS.
Bayley Mental Scales assess cognitive skills such as perception, The consistency of performance across the items was explored
memory, language, and rudimentary abstract thinking. The using the Kuder–Richardson 20 reliability coefŽ cient. Only 15
data were recorded on precoded forms that were checked for of these items revealed high consistency and the alpha-
errors within 24 hours of collection. coefŽ cient calculated from these items indicated they should
be retained for use as the Yoruba Mental Development Index
(YMDI). The number of items passed on the YMS was
Scoring calculated for each Yoruba toddler and differences in cognitive
Maternal responses. The mother’s responses to the errands’ performance across location were analysed using ANOVA
questions were coded as follows: 1 ˆ no, child has not started cross-tabulation (chi-square). Spearman correlation was used
doing this; 2 ˆ yes, child does this. The data were analysed at to examine the relationship between maternal report of errands
Tufts University with the Statistical Package for the Social and performance on the YMS.
Sciences (SPSSX 1988). Differences in maternal report of
errand performance across location were examined using
Results
cross-tabulation (chi-square) and analysis of variance (AN-
Descriptive information about the errands reported by Yoruba
OVA).
mothers and items on the Yoruba Mental Subscale are
Locations. The locations (independent variables) were coded presented in Table 1. Information about the developmental
on a 3-point scale: 1 ˆ rural, 2 ˆ semi-urban, 3 ˆ urban. characteristics of the index child is shown in Table 2.

Table 2
Developmental characteristics of rural, semi-urban, and urban children

Rural Semi-urban Urban

% N % N % N % N

Errands
Child puts things away 92.7 206 90 20 90.1 92 95.7 94
Child buys things 75.2 206 70 20 75 92 76.6 94

Overall Mean N Mean N Mean N Mean N


Physical Development Index 103.8 200 99.3b 18 101.9a 90 106.6a,b 92
Bayley Mental Development Index 92.6 207 83.1a 20 91.7a,b 92 95.4a,b 95
Yoruba Mental Development Index 7.6 187 5.7a 17 7.3a, b 80 8.2a,b 90
Child’s age 23.8 207 23.7 20 23.9 92 23.8 95

Note: Means having the same subscript are signiŽ cantly different at p < .01.
150 OGUNNAIKE AND HOUSER / ERRANDS AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE

Errand data Table 4


Regression of urban location, purchase of items,
Performance of errands across locations. Differences regarding
female gender, putting away items, and semi-
errand performance across location was analysed using cross-
urban location with the age-adjusted Yoruba
tabulation (chi-square) and ANOVA. Mothers reported that a
Mental Development Index
higher percentage of children in the urban and semi-urban areas
engaged in purchasing items and putting things away, compared Independent variables Beta SigniŽ cance
to the percentage of children reported in the rural areas.
Urban location .42 p < .001
Association among errands, gender, and age. Findings reported in Buys things .15 p < .05
Table 3 indicate that children who put things away were also sent Female ¡.16 p < .05
to purchase items (r ˆ .19, p < .01). Differences in participation Puts things away .07 n.s.
Semi-urban location .25 n.s.
based on gender, age, and location were also noted. Older
Adjusted R2 .09
Yoruba toddlers, especially girls, were more likely to be sent to Model F 4.12
put things away and to purchase items for the caregiver. Probability < .001

Errands and cognitive performance. Children who bought


things for their caregivers (r ˆ .26, p < .001) had signiŽ cantly
(Agiobu-Kemmer, 1984; Ginsburg, Posner, & Russell, 1981;
higher scores on the YMS compared to children who put
Oloko, 1994; Serpell, 1993). As it was hypothesised, children
things away (r ˆ .14, p < .10). Also, older children (r ˆ .44,
who engaged in errands had a signiŽ cant higher performance
p < .001), and those residing in the urban area (r ˆ .21, p <
on the Yoruba Mental Subscale than children who did not
.01) scored signiŽ cantly higher on the YMS compared to
engage in errands. In particular, children’s engagement in item
children who were younger and those living in the rural areas.
purchase for caregivers was associated with signiŽ cant perfor-
None of the reported errands was signiŽ cantly associated with
mance on the YMS.
performance on the Bayley Mental Scales. However, younger
This Ž nding could be attributed to several factors, one of
children (r ˆ ¡.25, p < .001), and children who lived in the
which is re ected in the level of child’s competence. Children
urban area (r ˆ .28, p < .001) had signiŽ cantly higher scores
who were asked to purchase things may have had a history of
on the Bayley Mental Scales compared to older children.
successful completion of errands and perceived by their
Also, a simple linear regression was performed with the
caregivers to be more reliable and responsible. Among the
Yoruba Mental Development Index (YMDI) predicted by age
Yoruba, such ‘‘reliable’’ children are trusted with additional
of child. The unstandardised residuals of this regression were
responsibilities, especially those that are related to commercial
calculated in order to create an age-adjusted YMDI. Multiple
transactions (Ohuche & Otaala, 1981). Thus, these ‘‘reliable’’
linear regression analysis was performed to create a model that
children acquire and enhance skills (for details, see Serpell,
examined possible relationships between errands and the age-
1993) that may have facilitated performance on the YMS.
adjusted YMDI. Table 4 presents the results of the multiple
Similar Ž ndings have been reported in other cross-cultural
regression analysis. Purchasing items for the caregiver sig-
research. For example, Ž ndings from the study conducted
niŽ cantly predicted the age-adjusted YMDI scores. Similarly,
among young Dioula children of the Ivory Coast (Ginsburg et
female gender and urban location of residence were signiŽ cant
al., 1981), suggest that children’s frequent manipulation of
predictors. Although the adjusted R2 -value is fairly low, three
merchandise (e.g., sorting and regrouping items into groups of
of the variables studied were signiŽ cant in predicting cognitive
Ž ves and tens) was associated with enhanced performance on
performance.
addition and subtraction tasks. Also, Oloko (1994) noted that
Yoruba school-age children who engaged in street trading had
Discussion higher academic ratings in English, arithmetic, and general
knowledge, compared to their nonworking counterparts.
The present data are congruent with the literature suggesting Present Ž ndings also indicate no signiŽ cant associations
that cultural activities are associated with child development among errands and performance on the Bayley Mental Scales.

Table 3
Spearman correlation coefficients between errands and the Yoruba Mental Subscale Index (YMDI) and Bayley Mental
Development Index (MDI)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Puts things away .19** .15* ¡0.1 0.1 .14‡ 0.05


2. Buys things .25*** ¡0.1 0.04 .26*** ¡0.01
3. Age of child 0.04 0.11 .44*** 0.06
4. Sex of child (female ˆ 0, ¡0.06 .21** ¡.25***
male ˆ 1)
5. Location (urban ˆ 3, 0.11 .28***
semi-urban ˆ 2, rural ˆ 1)
6. YMDI .61***
7. MDI

p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2002, 26 (2), 145–153 151

The Western orientation of the Bayley has been cited as a developmental processes in general, it is important to under-
mediating factor (Aina et al., 1993). Also, the fact that Yoruba stand that the Yoruba or any other African child does not
children are reared in what Robert Serpell (1993, p. 23) automatically qualify to buy things as soon as she/he is two
described as a ‘‘qualitatively different cultural environment’’ years old. Children are provided learning opportunities to
could account for the lack of signiŽ cant performance on the practise running other errands such as putting things away,
Bayley. fetching objects, or relaying messages to others in the
Present Ž ndings also conŽ rm an association among certain household. And regardless of the errands, children are
errands and cognitive performance on the YMS. As mentioned expected to listen, and try their best in working out what is
earlier, children who engaged in purchasing items, an errand, expected of them with or without others’ assistance (Zeitlin et
which took children away from their immediate household had al., 1995).
a signiŽ cantly enhanced cognitive performance. The enhanced Present Ž ndings lend credence to Serpell’s claim about the
cognitive performance on the YMS by children who purchased difference in the child-rearing environment of African children
items for their caregivers could be attributed to caregiver compared to their counterparts in the Western or highly
perception and level of child’s competence (for details, see technological environment (see also Mundy-Castle, 1975). As
Nsamenang & Lamb, 1994; Serpell, 1993). Before being mentioned throughout the paper, the African child grows up in
entrusted with the responsibility of purchasing items, it is likely an environment that fosters responsibility, respect, obedience,
that the child demonstrated a certain level of competence in and helpfulness from an early age. The child grows up in the
understanding and communicating the rules of commercial environment to which he/she adapts to through listening,
transaction such as listening, understanding the concept of imitation, keen observation, being helpful and obedient. We
money, ability to navigate the neighbourhood and recognise believe that these social skills, although not widely acknowl-
common household items (Zeitlin et al., 1995). Caregivers and edged in child development research, except in cross-cultural
other adults are more inclined to send such a competent child studies, play a signiŽ cant role in cognitive performance of
to purchase household and/or other items more often, thus young African children. The Yoruba Mental Subscale has
enhancing his/her proŽ ciency in commercial transactions. And enabled us to assess the signiŽ cance of the association between
a child that is proŽ cient in commercial transactions within the the Yoruba child’s involvement in errands and performance on
immediate neighbourhood is entrusted with other commercial a cognitive assessment tool. Unlike the Bayley Mental Scales
activities in a larger context. that revealed a decline in performance with age, the YMS
Other factors associated with enhanced performance on the indicated enhanced performance among older children.
YMS include age and location. Older children scored Findings from Cameroon (Nsamenang & Lamb, 1994) and
signiŽ cantly higher compared to younger children, while Zambia (Serpell, 1993), clearly stressed that parental assign-
children living in urban areas performed signiŽ cantly better ment of responsibilities, particularly errands, underscores
than their rural counterparts. Recall that older Yoruba children caregiver perception of child’s capabilities and sense of
frequently engaged in errands, thus fostering the proŽ ciency of responsibility. Caregivers adopt a style of gradually introducing
skills, namely, language, cognition, and memory, which might their children to the challenges of errands that take the child
have translated to better performance on the YMS. In contrast away from home. For example, a 26-month-old Yoruba boy
to the positive link with age, younger children, overall, named Wole, is sent to purchase a bag of detergent from the
performed signiŽ cantly better on the Bayley Mental Scales neighbour’s store. Wole is likely to have participated in and
(for similar Ž ndings, see Coll, Buckner, Brooks, Weinreb, & successfully completed other errands such as fetching objects,
Bassuk, 1998). Thus, after the Ž rst year of life, there is a putting things away, etc., before being asked to purchase the
precipitous decline on the Bayley among Yoruba toddlers. The detergent.
performance of Yoruba toddlers could be an indication of the This procedure of a gradual or systematic increase in
cultural inappropriateness of the Bayley (Aina et al., 1993). responsibilities or tasks in accordance with the learner’s skill is
The 25 items, out of which 15 were Ž nally chosen to form the described as scaffolding (GreenŽ eld, 1984; Segall et al., 1999).
YMS, negated the age-related decline. Yoruba children had Like caregivers from other African societies, Yoruba caregivers
excellent performance on items that involved following begin the process of scaffolding vis-à-vis responsibility training
directions, imitation, and limited verbal interactions. In order by sending their young children on simple errands (e.g.,
to have a valid assessment tool, items had to be omitted in fetching nearby objects, or/and carrying water in a small bowl).
which the toddlers’ performance indicated no variability, for Although these errands may or may not require much caregiver
example, pointing to parts of a doll, imitating horizontal and assistance, as the child completes these simple errands
vertical strokes, and use of gestures (see Appendix for the items successfully, he/she is introduced to more complex errands
omitted from the YMS). A combination of the omitted and such as relaying messages or purchasing items from neigh-
retained items underlie the importance of providing children bours.
with tasks that require the use of familiar skills or skills The caregiver structures the child’s performance of these
promoted by their environment. errands through keen observation, assistance of others (older
Regarding location, performance on the Yoruba Mental siblings or capable peers), communication with the neighbour
Subscale may be associated with differences in the level of about the child’s itinerary, and simple instructions or steps to
familiarity with the test materials (cf. Nerlove & Snipper, facilitate success, for example, Wole’s mother may have tied
1981). By virtue of the impact of modernisation, children the cash for the detergent in a cloth and placed it in his pocket;
residing in urban locations may have been exposed to toys and or informed Wole that she was watching him to ensure his safe
other resources that demand a certain level of manipulation or arrival at the neighbour (see Segall et al., 1999: discussion on
interaction akin to the requirements of the YMS and other shaping, p. 191). Thus, older siblings, capable peers, and adult
items on the Bayley Mental Scales. caregivers serve as a scaffold. Collaboration with these adults
In terms of qualifying this research for understanding facilitates the child’s performance and skill acquisition.
152 OGUNNAIKE AND HOUSER / ERRANDS AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE

Conclusion Berry, J.W. (1984). Towards a universal psychology of cognitive competence. In


P.S. Fry (Ed.), Changing conceptions of intelligence and intellectual functioning:
Current theory and research (pp. 35–61). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Present Ž ndings require cautious interpretation due to the Coll, C.G., Buckner, J.C., Brooks, M.G., Weinreb, L.F., & Bassuk, E.L. (1998).
paucity of the items on the Yoruba Mental Subscale and also The developmental status and adaptive behavior of homeless and low-income
housed infants and toddlers. American Journal of Public Health, 88, 1371–
because other factors associated with performance, namely,
1374.
presence of siblings, frequency of errands, and sibling Dasen, P.R. (1984). The cross-cultural study of intelligence : Piaget and the
‘‘coaching’’ were not examined in the present study. An Baoule. In P.S. Fry (Ed.), Changing conceptions of intelligence and intellectual
examination of the frequency of errands, caregivers perception functioning: Current theory and research (pp. 107–134). Amsterdam: North-
Holland.
of child’s competence, and caregiver support will complement Eades, J.S. (1980). The Yoruba today. London: Cambridge University Press.
present Ž ndings and provide a broader picture of the role of Ginsburg, H.P., Posner, J.K., & Russell, R.L. (1981). The development of
errands in child rearing and children’s developmental out- mental addition as a function of schooling and culture. Journal of Cross-
comes in African cultures. Cultural Psychology, 12, 163–178.
GreenŽ eld, P.M. (1984). A theory of the teacher in the learning activities of
These Ž ndings have implications for cross-cultural research everyday life. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development
and early childhood educators. To broaden our understanding in social context (pp. 117–138). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
of the implications of cultural practices, educators and GreenŽ eld, P.M. (1994). Independence and interdependenc e as developmental
researchers will need to develop research methods that provide scripts: Implications for theory, research, and practice. In P.M. GreenŽ eld &
R.R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 1–
in-depth information about child-rearing goals, and how 37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
caregivers guide children’s behaviours to achieve these goals Harkness, S., & Super, C.M. (1992). Shared child care in east Africa:
speciŽ cally or broadly, and the mechanisms whereby errands Sociocultura l origins and developmental consequences . In M. Lamb, K.
Sternberg, C.-P. Hwang, & A. Broberg (Eds.), Child care in context: Cross-
or other methods of responsibility training relate to child cultural perspectives (pp. 441–459). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
development. For example, are there mediating variables in the Hope, T.L., & Bierman, K.L. (1998). Patterns of home and school behavior
child’s environment that contribute or affect certain child- problems in rural and urban settings. Journal of School Psychology, 36, 45–58.
rearing practices, which in turn affect child outcome? Are there Lancy, D.F. (1996). Playing on the mother-ground: Cultural routines for children’s
development. New York: Guilford Press.
differences in caregiver guidance in shared cultural activities? If
LeVine, R.A., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P.H., Keefer,
so, are these differences associated with child development and C.H., & Brazelton, T.B. (1994). Child care and culture: lessons from Africa. New
outcome? The present study has attempted to unravel how York: Cambridge University Press.
child rearing among the Yoruba relates to child development, Lloyd, B.B. (1970). Yoruba mothers’ reports of child rearing: Some theoretical
and methodologica l considerations. In P. Mayer (Ed.), Socialization: The
in particular, cognitive performance. approach from social anthropology (pp. 75–107). London: Tavistock.
Early childhood educators concerned about the issue of Mundy-Castle, A.C. (1975). Social and technologica l intelligenc e in western and
culture in their programmes will beneŽ t from these results. non-western cultures. In I. Pilowsky (Ed.), Culture in collision (pp. 344–348).
Adelaide: Australian National Association for Mental Health.
These educators could utilise the knowledge about the
Nerlove, S.B., & Snipper, A.S. (1981). Cognitive consequences of cultural
importance of cultural practices in fostering an atmosphere opportunity. In R.H. Munroe, R.L. Munroe, & B.B. Whiting (Eds.),
of respect and appreciation in their programmes. Present Handbook of cross-cultural human development (pp. 423–474). New York:
Ž ndings and other resources on cultural practices can serve Garland.
Nsamenang, A.B. (1992). Human development in cultural context: A third world
to inform curriculum planning and opportunities for parental perspective (Vol. 16). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
involvement in the classroom. Nsamenang, A.B., & Lamb, M.E. (1994). Socialization of Nso children in the
The social reality of the African child includes participation Bamenda grassŽ elds of northwest Cameroon. In. P.M. GreenŽ eld & R.R.
in work, chores, and errands, which in turn foster the Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 133–146).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
acquisition of culturally relevant skills and the learning of Ogunnaike , O.A. (1997). Yoruba toddlers: Relating cognitive performance to family
appropriate roles. The present Ž ndings exemplify the impor- sociodemographics and mediating factors in the child’s environment. Unpublished
tance of errands in the development of the Yoruba toddler. doctoral dissertation, Tufts University, Medford.
Ohuche, K.O., & Otaala, B. (Eds.). (1981). The African child and his environment
Manuscript received March 1999 (Vol. 3). New York: Pergamon.
Revised manuscript received August 1999
Oloko, B.A. (1994). Children’s street work in urban Nigeria: Dilemma of
modernizing tradition. In P.M. GreenŽ eld & R.R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-
cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 197–224). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
References Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social
context. New York: Oxford University Press.
Agiobu-Kemmer, A.I. (1984). Cognitive and affective aspects of infant Segall, M.H., Dasen, P.R., Berry, J.W., & Poortinga, Y.H. (1999). Human
development. In H.V. Curran (Ed.), Nigerian children: Development perspectives behavior in global perspective: An introduction to cross-cultural psychology. Revised
(pp. 74–117). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. second edition. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Aina, T.A. (1992). Child development and nutrition in Nigeria: Introduction Serpell, R. (1993). The signiŽ cance of schooling: Life journeys in an African society.
and overview. In T.A. Aina, F.E. Etta, & M.F. Zeitlin (Eds.), Child New York: Cambridge University Press.
development and nutrition in Nigeria: A textbook for education, health, and social Sigman, M., Neumann, C., Carter, E., Cattle, D.J., & D’Souza, S., & Bwibo, N.
service professionals (pp. 1–14). Lagos, Nigeria: Federal Government of Nigeria (1988). Home interactions and the development of Embu toddlers in Kenya.
and UNICEF. Child Development, 59, 1251–1261.
Aina, T.A., Agiobu-Kemmer, I., Etta, F.E., Zeitlin, M.F., & Setiloane, K. Super, C.M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptua-
(1993). Early childhood care, development and nutrition in Lagos State, Nigeria. lization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral
Phase 1 Survey Report from the Positive Deviance in Nutrition Research Development, 9, 545–569.
Project, produced by the Social Sciences Faculty, University of Lagos, and Super, C.M., & Harkness, S. (1997). The cultural structuring of child
Tufts University School of Nutrition. development. In J.W. Berry, P.R. Dasen, & T.S. Saraswathi (Eds.), Handbook
Akinware, M., Wilson-Oyelaran, E.B., Ladipo, P.A., Pierce, D., & Zeitlin, M.F. of cross-cultural psychology: Basic processes and human development (2nd ed., Vol.
(1992). Child care and development in Nigeria: A proŽ le of Ž ve UNICEF-Assisted 2, pp. 1–39). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
LGAs. Lagos, Nigeria: UNICEF. Timyan, J. (July, 1988). Cultural aspects of psycho-social development: An
Babatunde, E. (1992). A critical study of Bini and Yoruba value systems of Nigeria in examination of West African child-rearing practices. Paper presented at the
change: Culture, religion, and the self. Lewston, NY: Edwin Mellen. regional UNICEF workshop, Abidjan, Coà te d’Ivore.
Bayley, N. (1969). Manual for the Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Whiting, B.B., & Edwards, C.P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation
The Psychological Corporation. of social behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2002, 26 (2), 145–153 153
Whiting, B.B., & Whiting, J.W.M. (1975). Children of six cultures: A psychocultural
analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Widmayer, S.M., Petersson, L.M., Larner, M., Carnahan, S., Wingerd, J., &
Marshall, R. (1990). Predictors of Haitian-America n infant development at
twelve months. Child Development, 61, 410–415.
Zeitlin, M., Megawangi, R., Kramer, E.M., Colletta, N.D., Babatunde, E.D., &
Garman, D. (1995). Strengthening the family: Implications for international
development. Tokyo, Japan: United Nations University Press.

Appendix
Items omitted from the Yoruba Mental Subscale

Item description No.

Builds towers of 2 cubes 111


Builds towers of 3 cubes 119
Puts 9 cubes in cup 114
Follows directions, doll 126
Attains toy with stick 122
Points to parts of doll 128
Imitates horizontal and vertical strokes 147
Folds paper 157
Differentiates scribble from strokes 135
Uses gestures to make wants known 116

You might also like